Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 April 2009 Foraging Site Selection of Two Subspecies of Bar-tailed Godwit Limosa lapponica: Time Minimizers Accept Greater Predation Danger Than Energy Minimizers
Sjoerd Duijns, Jacintha G.B. van Dijk, Bernard Spaans, Joop Jukema, Willem F. De Boer, Theunis Piersma
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Different spatial distributions of food abundance and predators may urge birds to make a trade-off between food intake and danger. Such a trade-off might be solved in different ways in migrant birds that either follow a time-minimizing or energy-minimizing strategy; these strategies have been assigned to two sub-species of Bar-tailed Godwits Limosa lapponica that use the European Wadden Sea during northward migration. At the study area on Terschelling we recorded feeding site selection, time budgets and intake rates (prey/min) in the period that both lapponica (energy minimizer) and taymyrensis (time minimizer) sub-species were present (late April till the end of May 2007). Prey availability (number of prey/m2) was negatively correlated to the distance from cover. Based on resightings of colour-ringed Bar-tailed Godwits, taymyrensis was foraging closer to cover, and for a higher proportion of time than lapponica (67% vs. 33%). During the high tide period taymyrensis was also foraging on inland coastal meadows, Moreover, taymyrensis was more vigilant than lapponica, whereas lapponica showed more resting and preening behaviour. Lapponica had a higher instantaneous intake rate, but taymyrensis had a higher overall intake rate and the birds were more successful in taking larger prey items than lapponica. Supposedly, due to the increased foraging time and additional foraging on the inland meadows, the time-minimizing taymyrensis achieved a higher fuel deposition rate than lapponica. Taymyrensis shifted towards food-rich areas, apparently accepting higher predation risks, whereas energy-minimizing lapponica avoided predation danger by foraging further from cover.

INTRODUCTION

A successful migration strategy results from optimizing at least three selective factors: time, energy and predation (Alerstam & Lindström 1990). Birds may minimize the time for migration, i.e. flight as well as fuelling time (maximize speed of migration), minimize the total energy cost of migration (energy cost of transport), or minimize predation risk during migration. Depending on the migration and energetic status, individuals may opt for different solutions to balance these selective factors. Theoretical studies of migration usually incorporated the dichotomy between time and energy minimization (Lindström & Alerstam 1992, Hedenström 1993, Vrught et al. 2007), and only few studies addressed the issue of predation minimization (e.g. Weber et al. 1998, Burns & Ydenberg 2002). Most empirical studies that have focused on predation minimization during migration are concerned with passerines (e.g. Lindström 1990, Fransson & Weber 1997, Schmaljohann & Dierschke 2005) and to a lesser extent with shorebirds, such as Western Sandpiper Calidris mauri (Ydenberg et al. 2002, Lank et al. 2003, Pomeroy 2006, Pomeroy et al. 2006, 2008), Dunlin Calidris alpina (Dierschke 1998) and Ruddy Turnstone Arenaria interpres (Metcalfe & Furness 1984). To explore in which way predation danger modulates decisions of birds on migration, we investigated foraging behaviour of two subspecies of Bar-tailed Godwit Limosa lapponica that are known to exhibit contrasting migration schedules.

The Bar-tailed Godwit is a long-distance migrant of which two subspecies occur along the East-Atlantic flyway (Engelmoer & Roselaar 1998, Engelmoer 2008). These two subspecies follow a leap-frog migration, with the European Wadden Sea as staging and stopover site (Drent & Piersma 1990). The subspecies taymyrensis (600 000 birds; Wetlands International 2006) visits the Wadden Sea twice a year during one month to replenish stores needed for migration between breeding areas in northern Siberia and wintering sites in West Africa (Smit & Piersma 1989, Engelmoer 2008). Taymyrensis follows a time-minimizing migration strategy (Scheiffarth et al. 2002): it faces a tight time schedule before leaving to the breeding grounds, and devotes as much time as possible for fuel deposition, while minimizing other energy consuming activities. By increasing foraging time, the time at the stopover site is minimized, and consequently time spent on migration is minimized. Lapponica (120 000 birds; Blew & Südbeck 2005, Wetlands International 2006), on the other hand, stays in the Wadden Sea for the entire winter before leaving to less distant breeding grounds in northern Scandinavia and the White Sea area (Cramp & Simmons 1983, Prokosch 1988, Engelmoer 2008). Lapponica follows an energy-minimizing migration strategy (Scheiffarth et al. 2002), as the subspecies builds up the required spring reserves over a long period before leaving to the breeding grounds. These birds do not have to forage the entire available time, which enables them to spend more energy and time on activities as moulting and predation avoidance.

Aerial predators of wader species, such as falcons, often make use of vegetation, hills, dikes and other structures to cover their approach when hunting close to the salt marsh (Bijlsma 1990, Cresswell & Whitfield 1994). Several studies have confirmed that the hunting success of aerial predators is higher on salt marshes than on open mudflats without any cover, suggesting that predation risk declines with distance from cover (Cresswell 1994, Ydenberg et al. 2002, Whitfield 2003, Dekker & Ydenberg 2004, van den Hout et al. 2008). To study the role of predation minimization on the stopover ecology of the two Bar-tailed Godwit sub-species, we focused on the importance of distance from cover. We recorded feeding site selection, time budgets and intake rates in the period that both subspecies are present in the Wadden Sea during northward migration from late April to the end of May.

METHODS

Study area

Data were collected on the tidal flats and on inland coastal meadows of the Dutch Wadden Sea island Terschelling (53°24′N, 05°21′E; Fig. 1). Bar-tailed Godwits are known to primarily forage on tidal flats (Cramp & Simmons 1983), although in spring they also use inland coastal meadows for feeding (Piersma et al. 1993, van de Kam et al. 2004).

Population identification

In 2001 the Royal Netherlands Institute for Sea Research (NIOZ) on Texel, in association with various ringing groups, started a colour-ringing program on the Bar-tailed Godwit. Based on the catching and/or previous resighting dates of colour-ringed individuals, they can be assigned to either the subspecies lapponica or taymyrensis. Birds caught and/or resighted between October and March in the Wadden Sea were assigned to the subspecies lapponica, and individuals caught and/or resighted in West Africa to the subspecies taymyrensis. In the Dutch Wadden Sea area, many birds were caught with ‘wilsternets’ (Jukema et al. 2001) on inland coastal meadows in May, or were resighted only in May. It is estimated that only a small fraction (6.7%) of the birds caught in May are lapponica (B.S., unpubl. data). We therefore assume that all birds caught and/or resighted only in May are taymyrensis. Up to March 2007, a total of 2173 Bar-tailed Godwits were colour-ringed, of which 15% were lapponica and 85% taymyrensis.

Figure 1.

The island Terschelling located in the Dutch Wadden Sea. Sites of benthic sampling and main foraging areas of Bar-tailed Godwits are indicated in black. The small grey circles indicate the most important high water tide roosts. The small inland triangles represent the main feeding sites on the meadows.

f01_51.eps

Since only a small fraction of the birds observed were colour-ringed, we tested the degree to which the two subspecies mixed while foraging. If they would not mix, the presence of colour-ringed individuals in a flock could indicate to which subspecies the flock belonged. In all flocks observed on inland coastal meadows and on tidal flats during low tide (3 hours before and after low water) that contained multiple colour-ringed individuals (n = 32), it never occurred that an assigned colour-ringed lapponica was foraging together with an assigned taymyrensis (Fisher's exact test, P < 0.001). Therefore, we assumed that flocks could be assigned to a subspecies on the basis of the identity of single individuals in the flock. In total we resighted 371 different colour-ringed Bar-tailed Godwits, of which 69 were lapponica and 302 taymyrensis.

Observations and benthos sampling

Behaviour of Bar-tailed Godwits was recorded during daylight on tidal flats, high water roosts and inland coastal meadows. Within each flock, 2-min focal animal observations were conducted (Martin & Bateson 1993) of randomly chosen birds with 20–60× zoom spotting scopes. Five different categories were distinguished: foraging, resting, preening, vigilance and flight. Vigilant behaviour was scored when the bird had raised the head from the head-down foraging position to at least a horizontal position (‘head-up vigilance’, Metcalfe 1984). Flight behaviour was scored when a focal bird flew in the air. Bar-tailed Godwits are highly dimorphic in bill and body size, which can affect their foraging behaviour (Smith & Evans 1973, Cramp & Simmons 1983, Glutz von Blotzheim et al. 1985), therefore the sex of each focal bird was recorded. Sex was determined by body size, coloration and bill length (Both et al. 2003). Abdominal profiles (ranging from 1 — lean —to 5 — abdomen bulging) were scored to estimate the body condition of the birds (Wiersma & Piersma 1995). Every 30 min the number of individuals was estimated, and the contours of the flock were plotted on Google Earth maps.

Prey items were scored to determine the intake rate. Bar-tailed Godwits in the Wadden Sea mudflats mainly feed on lugworm Arenicola marina, ragworm Nereis diversicolor, catworm Nephtys hombergii and bristleworm Scoloplos armiger (Scheiffarth 2001). When foraging on inland coastal meadows, Bar-tailed Godwits mainly feed on cranefly larvae Tipula paludosa (Piersma et al. 1993, van de Kam et al. 2004). The prey items were scored in four classes: (1) small polychaetes (all items up to half the bill length; i.e. smaller than 5 cm), (2) large polychaetes (all items exceeding half the bill length; i.e. larger than 5 cm), (3) lugworms, and (4) cranefly larvae.

To assess food availability of the tidal areas, soil samples were taken on spots where foraging flocks had been observed. Per foraging area, 10 random samples with a depth of 15 cm (equal to ±1.5 times the bill length) were taken with a standard PVC tube (1/56 m2) in a randomly placed square of 100 × 100 m. The samples were sieved over a 1-mm mesh sieve and the four main polychaetes species were identified and measured by a ruler to the nearest 5 mm. Per sample, we calculated the abundance of prey/m2. As our sampling sessions on inland coastal meadows failed, we took cranefly densities from a similar grassland polder on another Dutch Wadden Sea island, Schiermonnikoog (Tinbergen 1981).

Cover

To assess the distance from cover per subspecies, the location of each colour-ringed individual on the tidal flat and the inland coastal meadows was mapped. The distance from each foraging location to cover (e.g. dike, tree, scrub) was measured (m) from the centre of the flock. In this way the distance of each foraging colour-ringed individual from cover could be determined.

Data analysis

For the analysis we merged observations into so-called ‘sessions’, with a mean for both sexes. Each session ended when the observed flock flew up, the flock size changed or the session exceeded one hour. Over all tidal hours, we performed 1569 observations (692 low tide hours; i.e. 3 hours before and after low water) which we pooled per sex into 445 sessions (195 low tide hours), resulting in a mean number of 3.5 observations (3.5 low tide hours) per session. In the meadows we conducted 192 observations which were also pooled per sex into 49 sessions, resulting in 3.9 observations per session. Analyses are based on the sessions, with mean values for males and females to correct for sex in the model, hence sample sizes refer to sessions and not to individual birds. In all models sex is included as fixed factor to allow for morphological and behavioural differences between the sexes. Tidal and weather (i.e. temperature, wind speed, wind direction) data were obtained from Rijkswaterstaat Rijksinstituut voor Kust en Zee and the Royal Netherlands Meteorological Institute (KNMI), and were used as covariates whenever they were significant in the model.

Taymyrensis shows fast increases in body mass when staging in the Wadden Sea in May, whereas lapponica increases with a rather slow rate (Prokosch 1988, Drent & Piersma 1990). To determine the fattening rate of the two subspecies, mean abdominal profiles (±SE) per session of all tidal hours during the fattening period of both subspecies were used. A GLM was performed, with date as covariate, to assess differences between the two subspecies.

To assess differences in intake rate between the two subspecies, we calculated the instantaneous intake rate (prey/min) as the number of prey items taken when foraging. Only sessions of low tide hours were used in the calculation. The overall intake rate (prey/min) is defined as the number of prey items taken during entire focal observation periods (2 min) and further includes all tidal hours, including the inland coastal meadows. Besides the overall intake rate, we calculated the intake rate for each of the four prey items (i.e. small polychaetes, large polychaetes, lugworm, cranefly larvae), which were all natural-log transformed. For the instantaneous and overall intake rate calculations, a GLM was performed with sex as an additional fixed factor. Data were natural-log-transformed to satisfy the assumption of normality (Zar 1996). Time budgets of both subspecies were determined by the percentage of time spent per activity within the 2-min observations.

Effects of cover (as proxy for predation danger) on foraging behaviour were tested by correlation analysis. The mean time (s) per session spent on each behavioural category was converted into percentages and arcsin-transformed. Only data of the low tidal hours have been used, as during high tide birds are much limited in where to forage. Moreover, we examined whether intake rate and prey availability were correlated with distance to cover. Analyses were performed with Pearson correlation (r) or Spearman's Rho (rs). Graphical data of these correlations were pooled into 250-m points. Meadows are shown in graphs, but are excluded from correlation analyses. We performed GLM's to test for differences in behavioural activity between the two subspecies, with sex as additional fixed factor, by pooling data from all tidal hours. The behavioural category flight did not satisfy the assumption of normality, and therefore the non parametric Mann—Whitney U test was used. Differences in foraging mean distance (±SE) from cover between the two subspecies were analysed with a Student's t-test, in which data of low tide hours and meadows were used.

Basic assumptions of parametric tests were examined by testing for normality, residual analysis, and the application of the Levene's test for equality variances. The reported mean values and SE were back-transformed without corrections. All analyses were performed using SPSS version 15.0.

RESULTS

Seasonal occurrence

From March until mid-April 2007, only colour-ringed lapponica individuals were seen. An influx of colourringed taymyrensis began on 22 April (Fig. 2). From the beginning of May, lapponica started to leave the study area and gradually decreased in numbers in May. Taymyrensis did not leave the study area until the end of May.

Fuelling patterns

Taymyrensis were on average slimmer than lapponica individuals (GLM, F70,650 = 5.30, P < 0.001). However, in taymyrensis the daily increase of abdominal profile was almost 10 times faster than in lapponica (Fig. 3).

Figure 2.

Daily number of resighted colour-ringed lapponica and taymyrensis Bar-tailed Godwits on Terschelling, spring 2007.

f02_51.eps

Figure 3.

Abdominal profile score (mean ± SE) of two Bar-tailed Godwit subspecies throughout spring 2007. Regression coefficients are 0.004 AP-units/d for lapponica (F1,434 = 7.14, R2 = 0.016, P = 0.008) and 0.043 AP-units/d for taymyrensis (F1,283 = 136.13, R2 = 0.325, P < 0.001).

f03_51.eps

Figure 4.

Godwit time budgets (%; A–E), intake rate (prey/min; F), and prey availability (prey/m2; G) in relation to distance from cover (pooled into 250-m classes). The dotted line represents cover. The light grey area represents 95% CI of all resighted colour-ringed taymyrensis individuals, the dark grey area represents 95% CI of all resighted colour-ringed lapponica individuals, and the darkest grey area is the overlap. Correlations are shown in each graph (meadows excluded from analysis). Sample sizes refer to number of sessions.* indicates P < 0.05.

f04_51.eps

Figure 5.

Activity pattern of two Bar-tailed Godwit subspecies in relation to tidal stage. The category ‘other’ includes preening, vigilance and flight. Sample sizes (total number of birds recorded) are given for each hour.

f05_51.eps

Table 1.

Time budgets (% of time) of two subspecies of Bar-tailed Godwit, based on 2-min observation periods. Means and SE are based on arcsin-transformed data, given are back-transformed values.

t01_51.gif

Table 2.

Overall intake rate (prey/min) by prey class taken by two subspecies of Bar-tailed Godwit. Means and SE are based on log-transformed data, given are back-transformed values.

t02_51.gif

Correlations with distance to cover

Resting and preening increased with distance from cover, whereas time spent foraging and prey availability decreased with distance (Fig. 4). Vigilance, intake rate and time spent flying were not correlated with distance from cover.

Lapponica foraged at a mean distance of 397 ± 67 m from cover, and taymyrensis at a mean distance of 85 ± 21 m from the nearest cover (t-test, t21 = 4.26, P < 0.001). In particular taymyrensis on inland coastal meadows were foraging close to cover.

Foraging behaviour

Taymyrensis spent more time foraging than lapponica (Table 1) as the subspecies was still foraging during high tide when lapponica was roosting (Fig. 5). Lapponica spent more time resting and preening, whereas taymyrensis was more vigilant. There was no difference in time spent flying between the subspecies.

During low tide, lapponica had a higher instantaneous intake rate (3.5 prey/min) than taymyrensis (2.8 prey/min; F1,188 = 4.27, P = 0.040). However, the overall intake rate showed an opposite trend (1.1 and 1.3 prey/min, respectively; F1,431 = 6.56, P = 0.011). Taymyrensis ingested large polychaetes at a higher rate than lapponica, whereas intake rates of small polychaetes and lugworms did not differ between the subspecies (Table 2). No comparison was made for cranefly larvae, as only taymyrensis was foraging on inland coastal meadows. For taymyrensis, intake rates on tidal flats and meadows were similar (1.2 prey/min; F1,277 = 0.97, P = 0.326).

DISCUSSION

By using resightings of colour-ringed individuals, we confirmed the established patterns of lapponica and taymyrensis presence in the Wadden Sea (Prokosch 1988, Piersma & Jukema 1990, Scheiffarth et al. 2002, Engelmoer 2008). Lapponica was present during winter till mid May and taymyrensis from late April to late May. The idea that 20% of the wintering Bar-tailed Godwit population in the Wadden Sea consists of taymyrensis, as suggested by Engelmoer (2008) on the basis of biometry, was not supported by our resightings.

Body stores of taymyrensis arriving in the Wadden Sea at the end of April were close to depletion. Throughout their stay of only one month they were able to prepare for the next leg of the migration route of more than 3000 km. The apparently high daily food intake was achieved by (1) foraging a large proportion of time per day, which they achieved by feeding during high and low tide close to cover on mudflats, (2) feeding on inland coastal meadows, and (3) selecting sites where they found large prey species. Thus, our results confirm that taymyrensis follows a strategy of a time minimizer (Scheiffarth et al. 2002).

Taymyrensis foraged close to cover in areas with a higher prey density. By foraging close to cover, birds are more vulnerable to surprise attacks by raptors (e.g. Cresswell & Whitfield 1994, van den Hout et al. 2008), and apparently taymyrensis accepted a higher predation danger in favour of better feeding opportunities. The trade-off between food and danger has inspired many investigators (e.g. Milinski & Heller 1978, Sih 1980, Brown & Kotler 2004). Dark-eyed Juncos Junco hyemalis which had been deprived of food opted for a riskier habitat to enhance their feeding rates (Lima 1988). In a study of Western Sandpipers, heavy birds predominated at safe stopover sites, while leaner birds used sites where predation danger was greater (Pomeroy 2006). Our observations of Bar-tailed Godwits add well to these findings: initially heavier Bartailed Godwits foraged further from cover, and leaner birds closer to cover. Thus, the benefit of better feeding opportunities outweighed the risk of selecting more dangerous areas. This was clearest during high tide, when taymyrensis using inland coastal meadows, necessarily foraged close to cover (i.e. ditches, trees and buildings). A study on Ruffs Philomachus pugnax showed that birds foraging on meadows avoided fields that were located near trees and buildings (Verkuil & de Goeij 2003). We suspect that especially individuals that face difficulties in reaching their departure fuel load in time, would supplementary forage in the meadows. Caldow et al. (1999) showed that Oystercatchers Haematopus ostralegus tend to feed in meadows to supplement their low-tide intake on the tidal flats. Piersma & Jukema (1993) found that Bar-tailed Godwits at a coastal site were 40 g heavier than those feeding on inland meadows. In line of this finding, we found the mean abdominal profile score (corrected for date) of 1.8 in meadows compared to 2.2 on tidal flats (GLM, F1,279 = 10.424, P = 0.001).

We suggest that Bar-tailed Godwits traded distance to cover against food abundance. By increasing foraging time and by facing a higher predation risk, the timeminimizing taymyrensis ensured the accumulation of a sufficient fuel load, within a short stopover period, whereas the energy-minimizing lapponica minimized predation by foraging in the safer areas. It is expected that these strategies influence survival rates, and we predict higher mortality rates due to a higher predation risk for the taymyrensis subspecies during the migration seasons.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We are grateful to the volunteer ring readers, and especially to Harry Horn who resighted many colour-ringed Bar-tailed Godwits during the study period and continues to do so. He also helped and arranged accommodation during the fieldwork. We are grateful to the ringing groups of VRS Castricum en VRS Calidris Schiermonnikoog for catching and colour-ringing so many Bar-tailed Godwits. Many thanks to the State Forestry Service (Staatsbosbeheer Terschelling) for providing the accommodation on Terschelling, and to Wetterskip Fryslân for allowing us access to their private road. SD and JvD thank Wim den Brok for their pleasant stay on the campsite Nieuw Formerum, and David Cerasale for assistance with the benthic sampling and for the unforgettable memories. Casper Kraan assisted with creating the figure of Terschelling. We also thank Kees Camphuysen, Piet van den Hout, Jan van Gils and two anonymous referees for their helpful comments on the MS.

REFERENCES

1.

T. Alerstam & Å. Lindström 1990. Optimal bird migration: The relative importance of time, energy and safety. In: E. Gwinner (ed) Bird migration, physiology and ecophysiology. Springer-Verlag, Berlin, pp. 331–351. Google Scholar

2.

R.G. Bijlsma 1990. Predation by large falcons on wintering waders on the Banc d'Arguin, Mauritania. Ardea 78: 75–82. Google Scholar

3.

J. Blew & P. Südbeck 2005. Migratory Waterbirds in the Wadden Sea 1980–2000. Wadden Sea Ecosystem No. 20. Common Wadden Sea Secretariat, Trilateral Monitoring and Assessment Group, Joint Monitoring Group of Migratory Birds in the Wadden Sea, Wilhelmshaven. Google Scholar

4.

C. Both , P. Edelaar & W. Renema 2003. Interference between the sexes in foraging Bar-tailed Godwits Limosa lapponica. Ardea 91: 268–272. Google Scholar

5.

J.S. Brown & B.P. Kotler 2004. Hazardous duty pay and the foraging cost of predation. Ecol. Lett. 7: 999–1014. Google Scholar

6.

J. G. Burns & R. C. Ydenberg 2002. The effects of wing loading and gender on the escape flights of Least Sandpipers Calidris minutilla and Western Sandpipers Calidris mauri. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 52: 128–136. Google Scholar

7.

R.W.G. Caldow , J.D. Goss-Custard , R.A. Stillman , S.E.A. Le V. Dit Durell , R. Swinfen & T. Bregnballe 1999. Individual variation in competitive ability of interference prone foragers: the relative importance of foraging efficiency and susceptibility to interference. J. Anim. Ecol. 68: 869–878. Google Scholar

8.

S. Cramp & K.E.L. Simmons 1983. Handbook of the birds of Europe, the Middle East, and North Africa. Vol. III. Waders to Gulls. Oxford University Press, Oxford. Google Scholar

9.

W. Cresswell 1994. Age-dependent choice of Redshank (Tringa totanus) feeding location: profitability or risk? J. Anim. Ecol. 63: 589–600. Google Scholar

10.

W. Cresswell & D.P. Whitfield 1994. The effects of raptor predation on wintering wader populations at the Tyninghame estuary, southeast Scotland. Ibis 136: 223–232. Google Scholar

11.

D. Dekker & R.C. Ydenberg 2004. Raptor predation on wintering Dunlins in relation to the tidal cycle. Condor 106: 415–419. Google Scholar

12.

V. Dierschke 1998. High profit at high risk for juvenile Dunlins Calidris alpina stopping over at Helgoland (German Bight). Ardea 86: 59–69. Google Scholar

13.

R. Drent & T. Piersma 1990. An exploration of the energetics of leap-frog migration in arctic breeding waders. In: E. Gwinner (ed) Bird migration, physiology and ecophysiology. Springer-Verlag, Berlin, pp. 399–412. Google Scholar

14.

M. Engelmoer & C.S. Roselaar 1998. Geographical variation in waders. Kluwer, Dordrecht. Google Scholar

15.

M. Engelmoer 2008. Breeding origins of wader populations utilizing the Dutch Wadden Sea, as deduced from body dimensions, body mass, and primary moult. PhD Thesis, University of Groningen. Google Scholar

16.

T. Fransson & T.P. Weber 1997. Migratory fuelling in Blackcaps (Sylvia atricapilla) under perceived risk of predation. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 41: 75–80. Google Scholar

17.

U.N. Glutz von Blotzheim , K.M. Bauer & E. Bezzel (eds) 1985. Handbuch der Vögel Mitteleuropas. Band 7, Charadriiformes (2. Teil). Aula-Verlag, Wiesbaden. Google Scholar

18.

A. Hedenström 1993. Migration by soaring or flapping flight in birds the relative importance of energy cost and speed. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 342: 353–361. Google Scholar

19.

J. Jukema , T. Piersma , J.B. Hulscher , E.J. Bunskoeke , A. Koolhaas , & A. Veenstra 2001. Goudplevieren en wilsterflappers: eeuwenoude fascinatie voor trekvogels. Fryske Akademy & KNNV Uitgeverij, Leeuwarden / Utrecht. Google Scholar

20.

D.B. Lank , R.W. Butler , J. Ireland & R.C. Ydenberg 2003. Effects of predation danger on migration strategies of sandpipers. Oikos 103: 303–319. Google Scholar

21.

S.L. Lima 1988. Initiation and termination of feeding in Darkeyed Juncos: influences of predation risk and energy reserves. Oikos 53: 3–11. Google Scholar

22.

Å. Lindström 1990. The role of predation risk in stopover habitat selection in migrating Bramblings, Fringilla montifringilla. Behav. Ecol. 1: 102–106. Google Scholar

23.

Å. Lindström & T. Alerstam 1992. Optimal fat loads in migrating birds: a test of the time-minimization hypothesis. Am. Nat. 140: 477–491. Google Scholar

24.

P. Martin & P. Bateson 1993. Measuring behaviour: an introductory guide. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Google Scholar

25.

N.B. Metcalfe 1984. The effects of mixed-species flocking on the vigilance of shorebirds: who do they trust? Anim. Behav. 32: 986–993. Google Scholar

26.

N.B. Metcalfe & R.W. Furness 1984. Changing priorities: the effect of pre-migratory fattening on the trade-off between foraging and vigilance. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 15: 203–206. Google Scholar

27.

M. Milinski & R. Heller 1978. Influence of a predator on the optimal foraging behaviour of Sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus L.). Nature 275: 642–644. Google Scholar

28.

T. Piersma & J. Jukema 1990. Budgeting the flight of a long-distance migrant: changes in nutrient reserve levels of Bartailed Godwits at successive spring staging sites. Ardea 78: 315–337. Google Scholar

29.

T. Piersma & J. Jukema 1993. Red breasts as honest signals of migratory quality in a long-distance migrant, the Bar-tailed Godwit. Condor 95: 163–177. Google Scholar

30.

T. Piersma , A. Koolhaas & A. Dekinga 1993. Interactions between stomach structure and diet choice in shorebirds. Auk 110: 552–564. Google Scholar

31.

A.C. Pomeroy 2006. Tradeoffs between food abundance and predation danger in spatial usage of a stopover site by Western Sandpipers, Calidris mauri. Oikos 112: 629–637. Google Scholar

32.

A.C. Pomeroy , R.W. Butler & R.C. Ydenberg 2006. Experimental evidence that migrants adjust usage at a stopover site to trade off food and danger. Behav. Ecol. 17: 1041–1045. Google Scholar

33.

A.C. Pomeroy , D.A. Acevedo Seaman , R.W. Butler , R.W. Einer , T.D. Williams & R.C. Ydenberg 2008. Feeding-danger tradeoffs underlie stopover site selection by migrants. Avian Cons. Ecol. 3: 7. Available at :  http://www.ace-eco.org/vol3/iss1/art7/Google Scholar

34.

P. Prokosch 1988. The Schleswig-Holstein Wadden Sea as spring staging area for arctic wader populations demonstrated by Grey Plover (Pluvialis squatarola, L. 1758), Knot (Calidris canutus, L. 1758) and Bar-tailed Godwit (Limosa lapponica L. 1758). Corax 12: 273–442. Google Scholar

35.

G. Scheiffarth 2001. The diet of Bar-tailed Godwits Limosa lapponica in the Wadden Sea: combining visual observations and faeces analyses. Ardea 89: 481–494. Google Scholar

36.

G. Scheiffarth , S. Wahls , C. Ketzenberg & K.M. Exo 2002. Spring migration strategies of two populations of Bar-tailed Godwits, Limosa lapponica, in the Wadden Sea: time minimizers or energy minimizers? Oikos 96: 346–354. Google Scholar

37.

H. Schmaljohann & V. Dierschke 2005. Optimal bird migration and predation risk: a field experiment with Northern Wheatears Oenanthe oenanthe. J. Anim. Ecol. 74: 131–138. Google Scholar

38.

A. Sih 1980. Optimal behavior: Can foragers balance two conflicting demands? Science 210: 1041–1043. Google Scholar

39.

C.J. Smit & T. Piersma 1989. Numbers, mid-winter distribution and migration of wader populations using the East Atlantic Flyway. In: H. Boyd & J.Y. Pirot (eds) Flyways and reserve networks for water birds. IWRB Spec. Publ. 9, Slimbridge, pp. 24–63. Google Scholar

40.

P.C. Smith & P.R. Evans 1973. Studies of shorebirds at Lindisfarne, Northumberland. 1. Feeding ecology and behaviour of the Bar-tailed Godwit. Wildfowl 24: 135–139. Google Scholar

41.

J.M. Tinbergen 1981. Foraging decisions in Starlings (Sturnus vulgaris L.). Ardea 69: 1–67. Google Scholar

42.

J. van de Kam , B. Ens , T. Piersma & L. Zwarts 2004. Shorebirds, an illustrated behavioural ecology. KNNV Publishers, Utrecht. Google Scholar

43.

P.J. van den Hout , B. Spaans & T. Piersma 2008. Differential mortality of wintering shorebirds on the Banc d'Arguin, Mauritania, due to predation by large falcons. Ibis 150 (Suppl. 1): 219–230. Google Scholar

44.

Y. Verkuil & P. de Goeij 2003. Do Reeves make different choices? Meadow selection by spring staging Ruffs Philomachus pugnax in Southwest Friesland. Limosa 76: 1–12. Google Scholar

45.

J.A. Vrught , J. van Belle & W. Bouten 2007. Pareto front analysis of flight time and energy use in long-distance bird migration. J. Avian Biol. 38: 432–442. Google Scholar

46.

T.P. Weber , B.J. Ens & A.I. Houston 1998. Optimal avian migration: A dynamic model of fuel stores and site use. Evol. Ecol. 12: 377–401. Google Scholar

47.

Wetlands International. 2006. Waterbird Population Estimates — Fourth Edition. Wetlands International, Wageningen. Google Scholar

48.

P. Wiersma & T. Piersma 1995. Scoring abdominal profiles to characterize migratory cohorts of shorebirds: an example with Red Knots. J. Field Ornithol. 66: 88–98. Google Scholar

49.

D.P. Whitfield 2003. Redshank Tringa totanus flocking behaviour, distance from cover and vulnerability to Sparrowhawk Accipiter nisus predation. J. Avian Biol. 34: 163–169. Google Scholar

50.

R.C. Ydenberg , R.W. Butler , D.B. Lank , G. Guglielmo , M. Lemon & N. Wolf 2002. Trade-offs, condition dependence and stopover site selection by migrating sandpipers. J. Avian Biol. 33: 47–55. Google Scholar

51.

J.H. Zar 1996. Biostatistical Analysis. Third edition. Prentice-Hall, London. Google Scholar

Appendices

SAMENVATTING

In deze Studie is onderzocht in hoeverre bij de Rosse Grutto Limosa lapponica predatierisico een rol speelt bij de plaatskeuze tijdens het foerageren. In de Waddenzee komen twee ondersoorten voor: lapponica en taymyrensis. Zij houden er in het voorjaar een verschillende trekstrategie op na (lapponica, die in het waddengebied overwintert, een energie minimaliserende strategie; taymyrensis, die het waddengebied tijdens de noordwaartse trek alleen in mei gebruikt om op te vetten, een tijd minimaliserende strategie). We hebben van beide ondersoorten in 2007 op Terschelling de voedselplaatskeuze, tijdsbesteding en opnamesnelheid bepaald tijdens de periode dat beide ondersoorten hier aanwezig zijn (midden april tot eind mei). Bij de keuze van de foerageerplek is als maat voor gevaar de afstand tot de dekking (bijv. dijk, bomen of struiken) genomen. Het bleek dat er meer wordt gepoetst en gerust en minder wordt gefoerageerd naarmate de afstand tot de dekking groter is (voedselbeschikbaarheid is hoger dichter bij de dekking dan verder weg). Individueel gekleurringde vogels lieten zien dat de twee ondersoorten tijdens de laagwaterperiode niet mengen, zodat de groepen van beide ondersoorten goed zijn te onderscheiden. Het bleek dat lapponica gemiddeld verder van de dekking foerageerde dan taymyrensis. De laatste foerageerde langer en was waakzamer dan lapponica, maar rustte en poetste minder. De dagelijkse voedselopname lag bij taymyrensis hoger dan bij lapponica, voomamelijk doordat taymyrensis langer foerageerde (67% tegen 33% van de tijd) en tijdens hoogwater ook in de weilanden foerageerde. Het lijkt erop dat bij de afweging tussen voedselopname en predatierisico taymyrensis meer voor een maximale voedselopname kiest. Ze foerageren in rijkere voedselgebieden, maar moeten hiervoor wel een hoger predatierisico accepteren. Vanuit de noodzaak om in korte tijd het noodzakelijke vertrekgewicht te bereiken, ligt deze keus in de lijn met hun trekstrategie. Lapponica daarentegen heeft veel langer de tijd om lichaamsreserves op te bouwen. Omdat hun broedgebied minder ver weg is, hebben ze waarschijnlijk ook minder reserves nodig. Deze vogels kunnen het zieh blijkbaar permitteren te foerageren in de minder rijke gebieden waar het predatierisico veel kleiner is.

Sjoerd Duijns, Jacintha G.B. van Dijk, Bernard Spaans, Joop Jukema, Willem F. De Boer, and Theunis Piersma "Foraging Site Selection of Two Subspecies of Bar-tailed Godwit Limosa lapponica: Time Minimizers Accept Greater Predation Danger Than Energy Minimizers," Ardea 97(1), 51-59, (1 April 2009). https://doi.org/10.5253/078.097.0107
Received: 19 July 2008; Accepted: 1 September 2009; Published: 1 April 2009
KEYWORDS
feeding site selection
intake rate
Limosa lapponica lapponica
Limosa lapponica taymyrensis
migration strategy
predation
trade-off
Back to Top