Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 December 2009 Corticosterone as a Measure of Stress in Nest-Bound and Nest-Departed Long-Eared Owl Asio otus Chicks
Denver W. Holt, Anne Paulson, L. Michael Romero
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Long-eared Owl Asio otus nestlings usually depart from their nests at approximately 22 days of age, and cannot fly until approximately 35 days of age. Corticosterone has been Implicated as a mechanism Influencing nest departure in many avian species. We sampled corticosterone concentrations in wild nestling and nest-departed Long-eared Owl chicks to determine if this stress hormone Influenced nest departure. Baseline corticosterone titres were found to be similar in nest-bound and nest-departed young (10.69 ± 1.37 vs. 9.29 ± 1.58 ng/ml respectively), suggesting that stress was not the trigger for nest departure. Nest-bound chicks however did show lower stress-induced titres levels than nest-departed chicks (14.62 ± 1.98 vs. 21.58 ± 2.22 ng/ml, respectively). This suggests that nest-bound chicks may have a blunted response, perhaps due to age-related developmental constraints influencing corticosterone secretion.

INTRODUCTION

Glucocorticoids have been shown to be released by animals during times of stress, such as food shortage, habitat alterations, pollution, predation, research, and adverse weather (Wingfield et al. 1983, Smith et al. 1994, Hopkins et al. 1997, Norris et al. 1997, Wasser et al. 1997, Romero et al. 2000). Indeed, corticosterone, the primary avian glucocorticoid (Holmes & Phillips 1976) is rapidly secreted into the bloodstream following a stressful stimulus (Sapolsky et al. 2000). Nonstressed corticosterone levels appear to function primarily for physiological maintenance, whereas stress-induced corticosterone levels are believed to help regulate emergency functions (Dallman et al. 1993, Sapolsky et al. 2000). Perhaps the most important response to a stressful stimulus is behaviour, such as escaping a predator, fleeing inclement weather, or temporarily or permanently abandoning an area. The relationship between corticosterone release and these behaviours was supported by several studies (e.g. Astheimer et al. 1992, Wingfield & Ramenofsky 1997, Breuner et al. 1998, Romero & Wingfield 2001).

In addition to emergency behaviours in response to stressors, corticosterone may also play a role in behaviours linked to normal life history events. For instance, in owls, corticosterone has been implicated in natal and juvenile dispersal for Eastern and Western Screech Owls (Megascops asio and M. kennicottii) (Ritchison et al. 1992, Dufty & Belthoff 1997, Belthoff & Dufty 1998) and pre-fledging nest departure in Snowy Owls Bubo scandiacus (Romero et al. 2006).

Long-eared Owl Asio otus is a nocturnal, open-country foraging species (Holt 1997). Only female Longeared Owls incubate eggs for approximately 25–30 days, and the young hatch asynchronously (Marks et al. 1994, Holt et al. 1999). Females feed and brood nestlings for the first three weeks of life, while males provide food. After young Long-eared Owls branch or depart from their nests at about 22 days old (Seidensticker et al. 2006), they hide in surrounding vegetation for about two more weeks, and begin their first flights at about 35 days old (Seidensticker et al. 2006).

In many bird species, it has been suggested that nest-bound chicks may leave their nests early because of some stressful stimulus. For example, food competition, food shortage, reduced food delivery by parents, predation, sibling rivalry, size differences among nestlings, among others reasons (Holt et al. 1992, Heath 1997, Romero et al. 2006). Because Long-eared Owls exhibit early nest departure behaviour, we were able to test the influence of corticosterone titres in nestbound and nest-departed chicks before fledging. Our hypothesis was that an increase in corticosterone titres was the trigger that initiated nest departure.

METHODS

Our study was conducted in the Missoula and Mission Valleys of western Montana, USA. The valleys are characterized by rolling hills of sagebrush, grasslands, kettle ponds, man-made reservoirs, creeks, and rivers. Basically these are farm, ranch, and conservation lands. We located four owl nests in the Missoula Valley (46°54′ N, 114°07′W) in 1997, and nine nests in the Mission Valley (47°27′N, 114°07′W and 47°38′N, 114°11′W) in 1998. Within these nests, we acquired blood samples from 46 chicks; 21 in 1997 and 25 in 1998. Of these, 19 chicks were sampled within the nest and 27 chicks that had departed the nest. All nests were approached by foot and climbed with a ladder or freehand. Chicks were passed down to colleagues to be sampled. Blood samples were taken from the brachial vein with a sterile 18 gauge hypodermic needle. We collected 60 to 120 µl of blood in heparinized microhematocrit tubes. We then staunched the blood flow with cotton. Time is a critical factor when collecting blood for comparing corticosterone levels to a stress response (Romero & Romero 2002). All chicks were sampled within three min of the initial start of the climb or handling and then again 30 min later. The period between the initial bleeding and 30 min was the restraint period and is known to induce a stress response and consequently the release of corticosterone (Wingfield & Romero 2001). Blood was centrifuged within 12 h at 400 g to separate plasma from red blood cells. Plasma was stored frozen and transferred to Tufts University for analysis. Plasma was analyzed for corticosterone titres using a previously published radioimmunoassay (see Wingfield et al. 1992, Romero et al. 2006). Briefly, plasma samples were equilibriated with small amounts of tritiated corticosterone and then extracted with redistilled dichloromethane. Samples were dried with nitrogen, and then re-suspended in a sodium phosphate buffer. Bound and unbound fractions were separated with dextran-coated charcoal. To determine plasma corticosterone concentrations, bound fractions were counted, compared to a standard curve and adjusted by the percent recovery (see Romero et al. 2006). All samples were included in a single assay and the intra-assay variability was 4.6%.

Comparisons between nest-bound and nest-departed chicks were made using a mixed model repeated measure ANOVA with nest identity as a random variable to control for potential nest effects. We followed the main ANOVA with Tukey's post hoc tests. Alpha levels were set at P < 0.05.

RESULTS

Both nest-bound and nest-departed chicks increased their corticosterone titres over 30 min of handling and restraint (F1,44 = 24.2, P < 0.0001), although the increase was much more robust in the nest-departed chicks (Fig. 1). Although there was no overall difference in nest-bound and nest-departed chicks (F1,44 = 1.94, P = 0.17), there was a significant interaction between nest status and the corticosterone response to restraint (F1,44 = 5.26, P < 0.03). Post hoc analysis indicated that baseline titres were not different, but nest-bound chicks had significantly lower corticosterone titres at 30 min than did nest-departed chicks.

DISCUSSION

There is an abundance of evidence that a stressful stimulus elicits a corticosterone response in a wide variety of animals (Harvey et al. 1984, Sapolsky et al. 2000, Romero et al. 2006), including habitat alterations in Northern Spotted Owls Strix occidentalis caurina Wasser et al. (1997).

In the tree-nesting Long-eared Owl, chicks depart their nests at approximately 22 days (Seidensticker et al. 2006) and do not fledge until about 35 days old. It is unknown why Long-eared Owl chicks depart their nests approximately two weeks before they can fly. Given that there was no difference in corticosterone values between nest-bound and nest-departed Long-eared Owl chicks, stress does not appear to be the mechanism driving pre-fledging nest-departure. Seidensticker et al. (2006) felt that pre-fledging nest-departure was probably an artifact of predation pressure and not necessarily a stressful nest situation.

Previous studies on pre-fledging nest departure in ground-nesting Short-eared Owl Asio flammeus and Snowy Owl chicks suggest predation pressure was the selective force driving this behaviour. In ground-nesting Short-eared Owls, Holt et al. (1992) suggested that selective pressures for early nest departure (14–16 d) was probably mammalian predation — conceivably a stressful stimulus. Short-eared Owls do not fledge until approximately 30 days old (Holt et al. 1992). Romero et al. (2006) reached the same conclusion for Snowy Owls, whose chicks depart their ground nests at about 18–20 days of age, and are flightless for approximately 21–28 more days on the treeless tundra.

Figure 1.

Corticosterone response to capture and restraint in Long-eared Owl chicks both remaining at the nest (nest-bound) and roosting on branches away from the nest (nest-departed). Each point represents the mean ± SE for the samples indicated.

f01_593.eps

Contrary to the above however, wild American Kestrel Falco spaverius corticosterone levels did increase just prior to leaving the nest (Heath 1997). Similar to many small owls, Kestrels are cavity nesters. To our knowledge corticosterone titres have not been evaluated in relation to nest departure on any cavity nesting owl species. The cavity-nesting juvenile Eastern and Western Screech Owls studied by Ritchison et al. (1992), Dufty & Belthoff (1997), Belthoff & Dufty (1998) were not part of a nest departure study. In Belthoff & Dufty (1998), increased corticosterone titres in captive birds correlated with juvenile dispersal in free living birds, however juvenile or natal dispersal is different than pre-fledging nest departure.

Although nest-bound Long-eared Owls cannot be sexed by plumage, given our sample sizes, we must have sampled both male and female chicks. We suggest that the sex of the owl probably had no influence on nest-departure. Dufty & Belthoff (1997) reported no difference in corticosterone titres between sexes of young Western Screech Owls at the time of dispersal from their natal area. Similarly, neither brood size or food provisioning likely affected our results because, anywhere from 3–6 chicks departed from each nest and corticosterone titres remained similar for all chicks. Similar results were reported for Snowy Owl chicks (Romero et al. 2006). However, in a brood size manipulation study on nesting Barn Owls Tyto alba, Roulin et al. (1999) detected an effect of brood size on nestlings but not on parents. With two additional young placed in the nest, nestling mortality was higher, and body mass of the surviving male and female nestlings was lower in enlarged than reduced broods.

In another study, there were no differences between corticosterone titres in adult breeding and non-breeding male and female Long-eared Owls (Romero et al. 2009). In contrast, Wasser et al. (1997) found differences in corticosterone samples between adult Northern Spotted Owls and attributed their results to differences in male and female parental duties.

Our results fit a growing pattern of juvenile birds having a dampened corticosterone response to restraint that slowly changes to match the adult response as the chicks age. Studies of Northern Mockingbirds Mimus polyglottos (Sims & Holberton 2000), Common Redpoll Carduelis flammea (Romero et al. 1998), and White-crowned Sparrows Zonotrichia leucophrys (Wada et al. 2007) all show that younger chicks have lower corticosterone responses than older chicks. Consequently, the lower corticosterone titres after restraint in our nest-bound chicks may also represent a developmental process.

In summary, our data show that stress does not appear to influence pre-fledging nest-departure in Long-eared Owl chicks. In observations of 204 Long-eared Owl nests over 23 years, DWH has not witnessed brood reduction. Thus, we do not think that aspects of stress, brood size or sex of the young induce pre-fledging nest departure. Rather, other factors such as predation may be responsible.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We thank the many volunteers who have helped trap and band Long-eared Owls over the past 23 years. We also thank the private land owners, Flathead Indian Reservation Department of Wildlife, Montana Department of Fish Wildlife and Parks, and the United States Fish and Wildlife Service, for permission to work on their lands. Partial funding was provided by US National Science Foundation grants (IBN-9975502 and IOB-0542099) to LMR.

REFERENCES

1.

L.B. Astheimer , W.A. Buttemer & J.C. Wingfield 1992. Interaction of corticosterone with feeding, activity and metabolism in passerine birds. Ornis Scand. 23: 355–365. Google Scholar

2.

J.R. Belthoff & A.M. Dufty Jr. 1998. Corticosterone, body condition and locomotor activity: a model for dispersal in screech owls. Anim. Behav. 55: 405–415. Google Scholar

3.

C.W. Breuner , A.L. Greenberg & J.C. Wingfield 1998. Noninvasive corticosterone treatment rapidly increases activity in Gambel's White-crowned Sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophyrs gambellii). Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 111: 386–394. Google Scholar

4.

M.F. Dallman , A.M. Strack , S.F. Akana , M.J. Bradbury , E.S. Hanson , K.A. Scribner & M. Smith 1993. Feast and famine: critical role of glucocorticoids with insulin in daily energy flow. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 14: 303–347. Google Scholar

5.

A.M. Dufty & J.R. Belthoff 1997. Corticosterone and the stress response in young Western Screech Owls: effects of captivity, gender, and activity period. Physiol. Zool. 70: 143–149. Google Scholar

6.

S. Harvey , J.G. Phillips , A. Rees & T.R. Hall 1984. Stress and adrenal function. J. Exp. Zool. 232: 633–645. Google Scholar

7.

J. Heath 1997. Corticosterone levels during nest departure of juvenile American Kestrels. Condor 99: 806–811. Google Scholar

8.

W.N. Holmes & J.G. Phillips 1976. The adrenal cortex of birds. In: I. Chester-Jones & I.W. Henderson (eds) General, comparative, and clinical endocrinology of the adrenal cortex. Academic Press, London, pp. 293–240. Google Scholar

9.

D.W. Holt 1997. The Long-eared Owl (Asio otus) and forest management: A review of the literature. J. Raptor Res. 31: 175–186. Google Scholar

10.

D.W. Holt , S.M. Melvin & B. Steele 1992. Nestling growth rates of Short-eared Owls. Wilson Bull. 104: 326–333. Google Scholar

11.

D.W. Holt , R. Berkley , C. Deppe , P.L. Enriquez-Rocha , P.D. Olsen , J.L. Petersen , J.L. Rangel-Salazar , K.P. Segars & K.L. Wood 1999. Strigidae species accounts. In: J. del Hoyo, A. Elliot & J. Sargatal (eds) Handbook of birds of the world, Vol. 5. Lynx Edicions, Barcelona, pp. 153–242. Google Scholar

12.

W.A. Hopkins , M.T. Mendonca & J.D. Congdon 1997. Increased circulating levels of testosterone and corticosterone in southern toads, Bufo terrestris, exposed to coal combustion waste. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 108: 237–246. Google Scholar

13.

J.S. Marks , D.L. Evans & D.W. Holt 1994. Long-eared Owl (Asio otus). In: A. Poole & F. Gill (eds) The Birds of North America, no. 133. Academy of Natural Sciences Philadelphia, and AOU, Washington, D.C. Google Scholar

14.

D.O. Norris , S.B. Felt , J.D. Woodling & R.M. Dores 1997. Immunocytochemical and histological differences in the interrenal axis of feral brown trout, Salmo trutta, in metal-contaminated waters. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 108: 343–351. Google Scholar

15.

G. Ritchison , J.R. Belthoff & E.J. Sparks 1992. Dispersal restlessness: evidence for innate dispersal by juvenile Eastern Screech Owls? Anim. Behav. 43: 57–65. Google Scholar

16.

L.M. Romero & J.C. Wingfield 2001. Adrenalcortical responses to stress and tier modulation in free-living vertebrates. In: B.S. McEwen & H.M. Goodman (eds) Handbook of physiology: Section 7, The endocrine system: Coping with the environment; neural and endocrine mechanisms, 4. Oxford University Press, New York and London, pp. 211–234. Google Scholar

17.

L.M. Romero & R.C. Romero 2002. Corticosterone response in wild birds: the importance of rapid initial sampling. Condor 104: 129–135. Google Scholar

18.

L.M. Romero , J.M. Reed & J.C. Wingfield 2000. Effects of weather of corticosterone responses in wild free-living passerine birds. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 118: 113–122. Google Scholar

19.

L.M. Romero , D.W. Holt , M. Maples & J.C. Wingfield 2006. Corticosterone is not correlated with nest departure in Snowy Owl (Nyctea scandiaca) chicks. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 149: 119–123. Google Scholar

20.

L.M. Romero , D.W. Holt & J.L. Petersen 2009. Flushing effects and seasonal changes on corticosterone levels in adult Long-Eared Owls Asio otus. Ardea 97: 603–608. Google Scholar

21.

L.M. Romero , K.K. Soma & J.C. Wingfield 1998. The hypothalamus and adrenal regulate modulation of corticosterone release in Redpolls (Carduelis flammed): an Arctic-breeding songbird. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 109: 347–355. Google Scholar

22.

A. Roulin , A-L. Ducrest & C. Dijkstra 1999. Effects of brood size manipulation on parents and offspring in the Barn Owl Tyto alba. Ardea 87: 91–100. Google Scholar

23.

R.M. Sapolsky , L.M. Romero & A.U. Munck 2000. How do glucocorticoids influence stress-responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and adaptive actions. Endocr. Rev. 21: 55–89. Google Scholar

24.

M.T Seidensticker ,. D.T. Tyler Flockhart , D.W. Holt & K. Gray 2006. Growth and plumage development of nestling Long-eared Owls. Condor 108: 981–985. Google Scholar

25.

C.G. Sims & R.L. Holberton 2000. Development of the corticosterone stress response in young Northern Mockingbirds (Mimus polyglottos). Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 119: 193–201. Google Scholar

26.

G.T. Smith , J.C. Wingfield & R.R. Veit 1994. Adrenocortical response to stress in Common Diving Petrel, Pelecanoides urinatrix. Physiol. Zool. 67: 526–537. Google Scholar

27.

H. Wada , T.P. Hahn & C.W. Breuner 2007. Development of stress reactivity in White-crowned Sparrow nestlings: Total corticosterone response increases with age, while free corticosterone response remains low. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 150: 405–413. Google Scholar

28.

S.K. Wasser , K. Bevis , G. King & E. Hanson 1997. Noninvasive physiological measures of disturbance in Northern Spotted Owls. Conserv. Biol. 11: 1019–1022. Google Scholar

29.

J.C. Wingfield , M.C. Moore & D.S. Farner 1983. Endocrine responses to inclement weather in naturally breeding populations of White-crowned Sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophyrys pugetensis). Auk 100: 56–62. Google Scholar

30.

J.C. Wingfield , C.M. Vleck & C.M. Moore 1992. Seasonal changes of the adrenocortical response to stress in birds of the Sonoran Desert. J. Exp. Zool. 264: 419–428. Google Scholar

31.

J.C. Wingfield & M. Ramenofsky 1997. Corticosterone and facultative dispersal in response to unpredictable events. Ardea 85: 155–166 Google Scholar

Appendices

SAMENVATTING

Jonge Ransuilen Asio otus verlaten het nest wanneer ze ongeveer 22 dagen oud zijn. Het duurt dan nog bijna twee weken voordat ze kunnen vliegen. Men veronderstelt dat het stresshormoon corticosteron een rol speelt bij het vertrek van jonge vogels uit het nest. Om dit te onderzoeken werden hormoonspiegels gemeten bij jonge Ransuilen die nog op het nest zaten en bij jongen die het nest al hadden verlaten. Corticosterongehaltes verschilden niet tussen beide groepen (10,69 ± 1,37 en 9,29 ± 1,58 ng/ml) wat erop wijst dat het verlaten van het nest niet door stress gestuurd wordt. Dertig minuten nadat de jonge uilen waren gepakt, en daarmee bloot gesteld waren aan stress, waren de corticosterongehaltes minder toegenomen bij de vogels op het nest dan bij de jongen die het nest al hadden verlaten (14,62 ± 1,98 en 21,58 ± 2,22 ng/ml). Kennelijk reageren jongen op het nest zwakker op stressprikkels dan jongen die niet meer aan het nest gebonden zijn. Mogelijk komt dit omdat de vorming van corticosteron gebonden is aan de leeftijd van de vogels.

Denver W. Holt, Anne Paulson, and L. Michael Romero "Corticosterone as a Measure of Stress in Nest-Bound and Nest-Departed Long-Eared Owl Asio otus Chicks," Ardea 97(4), 593-596, (1 December 2009). https://doi.org/10.5253/078.097.0426
Published: 1 December 2009
KEYWORDS
Asio otus
Corticosterone
long-eared owl
nest-bound and nest-departed chicks
nestling
stress
Back to Top