Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
16 January 2020 Cheating Cheaters: Social Monogamy but Genetic Polygamy in a Brood Parasite
Diana Bolopo, Gianluca Roncalli, Daniela Canestrari, Vittorio Baglione
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Understanding the variability of mating systems is central in the study of animal behaviour. The advent of genetic techniques allowed combining social and genetic data, providing a more comprehensive view of reproductive strategies. In obligate avian brood parasites, the lack of constraints derived from offspring care allows for great plasticity of the mating system in response to changes in socio-ecological conditions. This potential for intraspecific variation makes brood parasites ideal model species to investigate the evolution of reproductive behaviour. We simultaneously studied for the first time social and genetic patterns of reproduction of the Great Spotted Cuckoo Clamator glandarius, combining radio-tracking and behavioural observations in the field with genetic parentage analyses. Our results reveal that loose pair bonds can be maintained in this species despite high levels of genetic polygamy and lack of territoriality, possibly because of the need of intra-pair cooperation for finding and parasitising the nests of its large hosts: the Carrion Crow Corvus corone and the Eurasian Magpie Pica pica. We also found that cuckoos mostly ranged around suitable foraging grounds, but females moved larger distances to find host nests to parasitise. Comparison of our results with previous reports on different populations also suggests a plasticity of Great Spotted Cuckoo territorial behaviour that may depend on population density, host choice and distribution of host nests.—Bolopo, D., Roncalli, G., Canestrari, D. & Baglione, V. (2020). Cheating cheaters: social monogamy but genetic polygamy in a brood parasite. Ardeola, 67: 39-56.

Entender la variabilidad de los sistemas de apareamiento es un aspecto crucial del estudio del comportamiento animal. El uso de técnicas moleculares ha permitido combinar observaciones del comportamiento social con datos genéticos para obtener una visión más completa de las estrategias reproductoras. En los parásitos obligados de cría, la falta de limitaciones derivadas de los cuidados parentales permite una mayor plasticidad del sistema de apareamiento en respuesta a las condiciones socioecológicas. Este potencial para la variación intraespecífica hace de los parásitos de cría excelentes modelos de estudio para investigar la evolución del comportamiento reproductor. Estudiamos por primera vez simultáneamente los patrones sociales y genéticos de la reproducción del críalo europeo Clamator glandarius, combinando radioseguimiento, observaciones de campo y análisis genéticos de paternidad. Nuestros resultados revelan que unos vínculos laxos de pareja pueden mantenerse en esta especie a pesar de altos niveles de poligamia genética y ausencia de territorialismo, posiblemente como consecuencia de la necesidad de cooperación de la pareja para parasitar los nidos de sus robustos hospedadores (la corneja negra Corvus corone y la urraca Pica pica). También encontramos que los críalos radiomarcados se movían preferentemente por los alrededores de las áreas de forrajeo, aunque las hembras podían realizar largos desplazamientos para encontrar nidos de hospedadores. La comparación de nuestros resultados con estudios previos en diferentes poblaciones también sugiere una plasticidad en el comportamiento territorial del críalo, que puede depender de la densidad de la población, la elección del hospedador y la distribución de sus nidos.—Bolopo, D., Roncalli, G., Canestrari, D. y Baglione, V. (2020). Engañando a los tramposos: monogamia social pero poligamia genética en un parásito de cría. Ardeola, 67: 39-56.

Introduction

Monogamy was accepted as the prevalent mating system in birds (Lack, 1968) until molecular techniques showed a different scenario. Extra-pair paternity in socially monogamous species has been detected in about 76% of the more than 250 species where genetic techniques have been used to determine paternity (reviewed by Brouwer & Griffith, 2019), showing that social patterns only weakly predict the number of sexual mates. Reproductive social relationships, however, remain a fundamental component of a species' mating system. Indeed, the selective processes that culminate in a given distribution of sexual mates can be fully understood only if the behavioural strategies at individual level; e.g. strength of within-pair social bonds, male mate guarding, intra and intersex competition, etc., are considered (Carranza, 2016). Therefore, ideally, social and genetic information should always be combined when investigating the breeding biology of birds.

Avian brood parasites, which lay their eggs in the nests of other species and have their offspring raised by foster parents (Davies, 2000), present a unique opportunity for testing the theory of evolution of mating systems (Kruger, 2007). These species are freed from most of the social, ecological and temporal constraints of parental care, and are therefore predicted to show a variety of key differences from nonparasitic species (reviewed by Hauber & Dearborn, 2003; Feeney & Riehl, 2019). Briefly, brood parasites may show 1) higher levels of both polyandry and polygyny as the lack of parental duties reduces the benefits of pair bonds, and 2) larger plasticity of mating strategies, which may adjust quickly to ecological and social current conditions (e.g. population density, host-species composition, etc.). Monogamy, however, could also be selected for, particularly when female-male cooperation increases egg laying success, either by facilitating the host nest location or by circumventing host defences against the parasite.

The degree of territoriality can also have important consequences on the mating system in both parental and parasitic species. Indeed, the strength of defence of home range boundaries may influence the probability of encounters among multiple potential sexual partners (Emlen & Oring, 1977; Hauber & Dearborn, 2003; Feeney & Riehl, 2019), leading to monogamy when territories are exclusive, or polygamy when home ranges largely overlap. Territoriality in brood parasites depends on the distribution and defensibility of both food resources and host nests, and therefore on the degree of spatial coincidence between feeding areas and host breeding areas. Decoupling of these areas may preclude effective exclusion of intruders, leading to non-territoriality (Martínez et al., 1998). The situation complicates further when the parasite can target different host species, if they differ in the distribution of their nests. Theory predicts that when resources (host nests, in this case) are patchily distributed, they are easier to monopolise and defend than when they are distributed evenly (Davies et al., 2012). Under this scenario, parasite territorial behaviour, and hence the encounter rate between potential sexual partners, will also depend on host choice. Studying brood parasite territoriality, and the factors that drive it, has therefore important implications for understanding their mating patterns.

In spite of the interest of studying brood parasites for understanding the transitions between different mating systems (Kruger, 2007), research on brood parasites has focused primarily on testing the predictions of co-evolutionary arms race theory, neglecting their natural history and, specifically, their reproductive and territorial behaviour (Feeney et al., 2014; Soler, 2014). This is due, at least in part, to the difficulties of their study. On the one hand, the lack of a reference point (the nest) for behavioural observations and the absence of conspicuous breeding behaviours (such as nest building, egg incubation and chick provisioning) hinder the characterisation of the social relationships between putative reproductive partners. On the other hand, genetic data are also difficult to collect because parents may have their offspring dispersed in many host nests over large areas that are difficult to sample. Furthermore, a comprehensive view of the mating system of a brood parasite species can only be achieved if social and genetic data are concurrent, because the expected behavioural plasticity of these species invalidates merging information from studies that are separated temporally or geographically.

Due to this complexity, the literature on the mating systems and territorial behaviour of avian brood parasites is particularly scant, hindering a comprehensive test of some key predictions of mating system theory. Colour-banding studies have reported on promiscuous or polygamous mating in the Orange-rumped (Yellow-rumped) Honey-guide Indicator xanthonotus (Cronin & Sherman, 1977), Village Indigobird Vidua chalybeata (Payne & Payne, 1977) and Pin-tailed Whydah Vidua macroura (Barnard & Markus, 1989). Radio-tracking techniques, especially on breeding females, have been used to study habitat use and territoriality on Common Cuckoo Cuculus canorus (Nakamura & Miyazawa, 1997; Vogl et al., 2002, 2004; Nakamura et al., 2005; Moskát et al., 2019), Brown-headed Cowbirds Molothrus ater (Rothstein et al., 1984), Shiny Cowbirds Molothrus bonariensis and Screaming Cowbirds Molothrus rufoaxillaris (Scardamaglia & Reboreda, 2014). The latter found that male and female Screaming Cowbirds that were trapped together showed a certain degree of social bonding during radio-tracking, suggesting social monogamy. A few molecular studies have been also carried out to describe genetic mating patterns, as well as egg laying ranges, of the Common Cuckoo (Jones et al., 1997), the Great Spotted Cuckoo Clamator glandarius (Martínez et al., 1998; Martínez et al., 1998), the Brown-headed Cowbird (Alderson et al., 1999; Strausberger & Ashley, 2003) and the Horsfield's Bronze-cuckoo Chalcites basalis (Langmore et al., 2007). Contrasting results from different genetic studies of the same species showed evidence of temporal and/or geographical variation in the levels of genetic polygamy (see Jones et al., 1997 and Marchetti et al., 1998 for the Common Cuckoo, Martínez et al., 1998; Bolopo et al., 2017 for the Great Spotted Cuckoo and Alderson et al., 1999 and Woolfenden et al., 2002 for the Brown-headed Cowbird), highlighting once more the need for concurrent social and genetic data to fully understand the mating systems of brood parasites.

In this study we address the relationship between genetic and social mating system in brood parasites, using the Great Spotted Cuckoo as the model species. This migrant non-evicting cuckoo species specialises in parasitising corvids, the Eurasian Magpie Pica pica being the primary host in most Palearctic populations, and the Carrion Crow Corvus corone the secondary host (Soler, 1990). Host choice, however, can reverse under particular ecological conditions, as occurs in our study area where the Great Spotted Cuckoo prefers crow nests (Baglione et al., 2017). Flexibility of the genetic mating system has been reported in this species, with the degree of polygamy varying temporally and geographically, most likely as a consequence of population density (Martínez et al., 1998; Martínez et al., 1998; Bolopo et al., 2017). The social mating system and territorial behaviour of this species, however, is largely unknown. Early reports by Arias de Reyna et al. (1987), based on recorded movements of unbanded individuals, categorised this species as territorial in the Sierra Morena (southern Spain), whereas a more recent genetic study (Martínez et al., 1998) showed a large overlap of female laying ranges at Guadix (southern Spain). Rühmann et al. (2019) also found no territoriality of GPS-tagged individuals during both the breeding and non-breeding seasons.

The Great Spotted Cuckoo may face counteracting pressures leading to either social monogamy or polygamy, making this species a valuable model for investigating the ecology of avian mating systems. The parasitic habit should reduce selective pressure for social monogamy (Hauber & Dearborn, 2003) but, conversely, the Great Spotted Cuckoo may need cooperation between sexual partners, and therefore pair bonds, for a) finding host nests, which is more demanding for a specialist brood parasite, than for generalists (Hauber & Dearborn, 2003), b) overcoming the defences of magpies, which mob the parasite and actively defend their nests (Soler et al., 1999), and c) increasing vigilance at crow territories to enhance detection of temporally unattended nests, which is essential for parasitising such a large host (Canestrari et al., 2009). In this study, we explored the outcome of this complex interaction and provide a comprehensive view of the Great Spotted Cuckoo mating system, by combining parentage analysis (reported in Bolopo et al., 2017) with concurrent radiotracking data that address its home range characteristics, territorial behaviour, habitat use and sexual social bonds.

Methods

Study site

The study was carried out in a 45km2 rural area in northern Spain (42°37′N, 5°26′W, Sobarriba, León). The area is characterised by a low-intensity agricultural landscape comprised by a mosaic of crops, meadows, riparian vegetation, poplar Populus sp. and pine Pinus sp. plantations, scattered shrubs, Pyrenean Oak Quercus pyrenaica forest patches, isolated trees and small patches of Holm Oaks Quercus ilex, and uncultivated land.

The Great Spotted Cuckoo is migratory in the study area and arrives to breed at the end of winter or early spring (March-April). Unlike other Iberian populations, the Carrion Crow Corvus corone is the main host at our study site, whereas the Eurasian Magpie Pica pica is the secondary host (Baglione et al., 2017). In this part of Spain, crows breed cooperatively in about 75% of territories, building open nests in the upper third of the trees. They may re-nest up to twice per season in the event of early nest failure (Canestrari et al., 2010). Magpies breed in unassisted pairs, build roofed nests hidden in brambles, scrubs, thorny bushes and occasionally in trees, and may re-nest once after a breeding failure (Birkhead, 1991). Both hosts place their nests at the edge of oak forests, in small tree patches or on isolated trees or bushes.

Field data collection

In March-April 2009 and 2010, we captured cuckoo adults by call playback and mist-netting in feeding areas (pine plantations). Each individual was marked with numbered metallic rings, measured and bled (∼100-200µl) from the brachial vein for sexing and parentage analyses (see Bolopo et al., 2017 for complete description). Cuckoos were fitted with radio transmitters Holohil BD-2 that weighed 1.6g (∼1% cuckoo average weight), attached to the two central tail feathers. After release, all captured birds were relocated in randomised order between 7am and 5pm UTC time, once or twice per day, from May 1st in 2009 and April 1st in 2010. The variation in the start of the relocation dates was due to logistic constraints for fieldwork in 2009. We relocated the birds with a directional antenna and a Followit RX98 receiver. We used the homing method (Fuller et al., 2005) for radio-tracking and, for each location, we established visual and acoustic contact with the tagged birds to precisely identify the habitat used and to record the social circumstance (alone, together with another tagged/untagged bird). Although this time-consuming procedure inevitably constrained the number of locations we could obtain for each bird, it provided the fine-scale information that was necessary to fulfil the aim of this study. When the individual was accompanied by a conspecific, we also noted how they interacted (sitting together/ moving co-ordinately/vocalising reciprocally versus chasing/ignoring each other). All locations were recorded in high resolution geo-referenced aerial photographs that enabled marking the exact positions of the cuckoos (up to 50cm accuracy).

We surveyed all crow and magpie nests in the study site from the end of March until the end of the breeding season (end of June) in 2009 and 2010. For each nest, we recorded laying date, hatching date, hatching success and fledging success for both hosts and parasite. When the clutch was found already complete, we visited the nest every 2-3 days to estimate hatching date and therefore laying date (incubation periods of crow, magpie and cuckoo eggs are 19-20, 17-18 and 13-14 days respectively; Canestrari et al., 2017; Álvarez & Arias de Reyna, 1974; Cramp & Perrins, 1994). When cuckoo chicks were four days old we obtained a small sample of blood (50-100µL) from their brachial vein for parentage genetic analyses (see details in Bolopo et al., 2017).

We captured and radio-tagged 11 adult cuckoos in 2009 (eight males, three females) and 13 adult cuckoos in 2010 (eight males, five females). One of the females captured in 2009 returned the following year; she was recaptured and radio-tagged, but disappeared from the study area before we could record any movement. On four occasions we captured two individuals in the net at the same time: one male-male (MM) and one male-female (MF) dyad in 2009, and two MF dyads in 2010. Nineteen birds (12 males and seven females) were radio-located until their death (three predated, one drowned in a well and one cause unknown), the disappearance of the radio signal (N = 7) or the end of the seasonal fieldwork (N = 7). The other five birds were never located again after capture, one due to predation and the rest most likely because they moved out of our study site. We surveyed 70 crow and 63 magpie nests in 2009, and 61 crow and 50 magpie nests in 2010, including second clutches; of these, 46, 10, 43 and 9 respectively were parasitised by Great Spotted Cuckoo.

Data analysis

Mapping

We elaborated a detailed habitat map at scale 1:3000 from geo-referenced aerial photographs using ArcGis 10 (ESRI, 2014). We categorised five habitat types: 1) “pines”: pine plantations; 2) “forest”: forest or plantations formed by trees other than pines, such as poplars, oaks or mixed formations; 3) “riparian”: riparian vegetation, either trees or bushy formations along rivers and streams; 4) “open”: open areas such as crops, meadows and uncultivated land with scattered shrubs, trees or small tree patches; and 5) “urban”: villages. Map accuracy was confirmed in the field.

Estimation of home range

Location data were analysed using the adehabitat package (Calenge, 2006) in R (R Core Team, 2019) to estimate the cuckoo home ranges. Only locations recorded more than one hour apart were used in the analyses to ensure time independence. We calculated individual home ranges using 100% minimum convex polygons (100% MCP) and 95% utilisation distribution (95% UD) with href smoothing factor. To examine the effect of the number of locations on home range area independently from the recording order, we used a bootstrap method (1,000 repetitions) to produce an area-locations curve for each individual (incremental area analysis). We used only MCPs for this analysis because utilisation distribution, which use the density of locations to calculate the area, can sometimes decrease in size after adding locations, making the interpretation of the graphical analysis more difficult.

Incremental area analysis is used often to define the minimum number of locations required to identify a home range, i.e. the average number of locations at which the home range area stops increasing despite adding new locations. Because we could not find such a limit (see Results), we used 20 locations per individual as a minimum threshold to calculate home range sizes. We chose that threshold to maximize our sample size while remaining within the range used in studies of other brood parasites (Greater Honeyguide Indicator indicator, Spottiswoode et al., 2016 and Common Cuckoo, Nakamura & Miyazawa, 1997). We obtained 20 or more locations from seven individuals in 2009 (four males and three females) and eight individuals in 2010 (five males and three females) for which we estimated 100% MCP and 95% UD. After a preliminary inspection of the data, we tested whether the home range indeed increased with the number of locations significantly faster for females than for males as the graphical results suggested. To do so, we modelled the progressive increments in size of the home ranges in function of the corresponding number of radio-tracked locations with a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) using the lme4 package (Bates et al., 2015) in R (R Core Team, 2019). Sex of the home range owner, year and the interaction between number of locations and sex were also fitted as explanatory variables. In the model, which comprised 507 bootstrapped home range values, a random factor was also fitted to account for multiple measures from the same individual.

To control for the different sampling dates and hence, the number of relocations per individual between the two study years, we ran the same analyses (bootstrap and GLMM) with another two different datasets: one including only those relocations of 2010 starting on the same date as in 2009 and another setting the maximum number of relocations per individual to the value obtained in 2009.

Finally, we combined home range data from this study with genetic data obtained during the same period of time (Bolopo et al., 2017) and mapped the nests where we found offspring of the radio-tracked individuals. We found offspring (N = 32) belonging to three radio-tracked females and six radio-tracked males ( Supplementary Material, Appendix 1, Table A2 (Rp_04_Supplementary Electronic Material_Bolopo_D. et al.pdf)). We obtained the laying range of the females, as minimum convex polygon, and checked for the spatial coincidence with the radio-tracked home range (95% UD). If partial or complete detachment was observed, we considered that the radio-tracked home ranges underestimated the size of the “effective” home range of the cuckoos, defined as the area that fulfils all the nutritional, defensive and reproductive needs of an individual (Burt, 1943).

Fig. 1.

GLMM results showing the significant interaction between sex and number of locations on the size of the home range area (100% MCP) of the radio-tracked Great Spotted Cuckoos. Grey area indicates the 95% confidence intervals. [Resultados del GLMM que muestran una interacción significativa entre el sexo del individuo y el número de localizaciones sobre el tamaño del área de campeo (100% MCP) de los críalos radiomarcados. Las áreas grises indican los intervalos de confianza al 95%.]

img-z7-7_39.jpg

Habitat use

We examined whether individuals established their home ranges (95% UD) randomly within the study area or favoured certain types of habitat (“second-order selection” sensu Johnson, 1980), using a compositional analysis of habitat use (Aebischer et al., 1993). The analysis was carried out with the compana function in the package adehabitatHS (Calenge, 2006) in R (R Core Team, 2019). We also used a selection ratio to measure the preference/avoidance for each habitat type: ratio = (rp)/(r + p), where r is the proportion of habitat type within the home range and p the proportion of that habitat type available within the study site. Ratio values of +1 indicate maximum selection, –1 maximum avoidance and 0 no preference. Then, we tested the significance of the selection ratio for each habitat type with Wilcoxon signed rank tests. We used the same compositional analysis and selection ratio to study the habitat use by cuckoos within their home ranges (“third-order selection” sensu Johnson, 1980). In this case, the selection ratio was calculated with r as the proportion of locations in each habitat type and p as the proportion of each habitat type within the home range.

Home range overlaps and social bonds

We calculated the proportion of home range (95% UD) overlap per dyad of radio-tracked individuals. We divided the overlapping dyads into four groups: females over females (FF), females over males (FM), males over males (MM) and males over females (MF). The overlap values indicate the proportion of the home range of the second individual of the dyad that is overlapped by that of the first individual. For example, in FM dyads the overlap value corresponds to the proportion of the male's home range that is overlapped by the female's home range. Therefore, for each dyad we obtained two overlap values, one for the overlap of individual A over individual B, and one for the overlap of individual B over individual A. To address differences in home range overlap between individuals of the same or different sex, we built a linear model using the lme4 package (Bates et al., 2015) in R (R Core Team, 2019). We set the percentage of overlap as the response variable, and the type of dyad (MM, FF, MF, FM) and year as explanatory variables.

We also recorded the social interactions of the tagged cuckoos. During radio-tracking, upon establishing visual contact with the birds, we noted whether the individual located was alone or accompanied by one or more other individuals. Using the radio-tracking data we analysed the social interactions between individuals that had a chance of interacting with each other, which was set at > 40% of overlap between their home ranges (N = 8 dyads,  Supplementary Material, Appendix 1, Table A3 (Rp_04_Supplementary Electronic Material_Bolopo_D. et al.pdf)). We used the wildlifeID package (Long, 2014) in R (R Core Team, 2019) to carry out a dynamic interaction analysis. We used the IAB interaction statistic (Benhamou et al., 2014), which uses both a distance threshold (set at 50m) and a time threshold (set at 3min), to detect interactions (avoidance vs attraction) between the animals.

Results

Estimation of home range

The home range of the radio-tracked individuals varied greatly: 91-1,225ha, 100% MCP, and 167-1,998ha, 95%UD ( Supplementary Material, appendix 1, Table A1 (Rp_04_Supplementary Electronic Material_Bolopo_D. et al.pdf)). We obtained an average of 26 relocations per individual in 2009 (range 21-34) and 44 in 2010 (range 21-60). The increase in home range size (100% MCP) with the number of relocations, proved significantly faster for females than for males (Table 1). Also, home ranges were bigger in 2010 than in 2009, although the tendency was marginally non-significant (Table 1).

To control for the effect of the different length of the sampling period between years, we ran two additional models. The first one, which comprised only the locations of the year 2010 obtained from May 3rd (first relocation day in the year 2009) showed qualitatively similar results ( Supplementary Material, Appendix 1, Table A4 (Rp_04_Supplementary Electronic Material_Bolopo_D. et al.pdf)), ruling out an effect of the starting date. In the second model, where we cut the number of locations of the year 2010 to the maximum number sampled in the year 2009 (i.e. 34 locations), the significant interaction between sex and number of locations was also confirmed, and the effect of year became significant, suggesting that home ranges were indeed larger in 2010 than in 2009 ( Supplementary Material, Appendix 1, Table A5 (Rp_04_Supplementary Electronic Material_Bolopo_D. et al.pdf)), as the initial model indicated.

Table 1

Factors affecting the size of the home range (100% MCP) of the Great Spotted Cuckoo. Results of a GLMM, with individual ID set as random factor. [Factores que afectan al tamaño del área de campeo (100% MCP) de los críalos. Resultados de un GLMM que incluye la identidad de los individuos como factor aleatorio.]

img-z9-6_39.gif

Offspring location and laying ranges

Genetic analysis reported in Bolopo et al. (2017) at the same study site, identified 55 adults in 2009 (28 males and 27 females) and 31 adults in 2010 (15 males and 16 females), inferred from the parentage analyses of cuckoo chicks samples (61 chicks sampled in 2009 and 28 in 2010). We found no genetic offspring of three of the captured adults in 2009 (two males and one female) and of another six in 2010 (two males and four females). Pooling all information together, the minimum number of adult cuckoos in the study site was 58 in 2009 and 37 in 2010. The fewer chicks sampled in 2010 were due to lower hatching success of the cuckoo eggs (56% in 2009 and 31% in 2010), which in turn affected the estimation of the number of adults in that year. In fact, cuckoo density most likely increased in 2010, as shown by a higher parasitism rate as compared to 2009 (Baglione et al., 2017). It is important to note here that our radio-tracking data are based on only a fraction of the adult population (maximum 20% in 2009 and 35% in 2010). This is particularly relevant for the interpretation of the home range overlaps described below.

We found between one and nine offspring belonging to nine radio-tracked individuals; six males and three females (see  Supplementary Material, Appendix 1, Table A1 (Rp_04_Supplementary Electronic Material_Bolopo_D. et al.pdf)). Overall, we found that 56.25% of the offspring were raised in host nests within the home range (95% UD) of their parents, with no significant differences between parental sex (two tailed Fisher exact test: P = 1, n = 32) or year (two tailed Fisher exact test: P = 0.490, n = 32). For the offspring outside the home ranges, the average distance ± 95% CI from the border of the parental home range to the parasitized nest was 1,907 ± 851m (range 200-5400m,  Supplementary Material; Appendix 1, Table A2 (Rp_04_Supplementary Electronic Material_Bolopo_D. et al.pdf)), with no significant difference between parental sex (two tailed Mann-Whitney test: U-value = 21.5, P = 0.749, N = 32) or year (two tailed Mann-Whitney test: U-value = 37, P = 0.795, N = 32). The large proportion of offspring raised outside the radio-tracked home range of females indicates that their “effective” home range, defined as the area that an individual uses to find resources for nutrition, protection and reproduction (Burt, 1943), was in fact larger than that drawn on the bases of radio-tracked movements.

We found that the laying sequences of the three radio-tracked females with known offspring did not follow a consistent sequential pattern, such as laying first within the home range and then outside, or vice versa. Female cgl11 had all three offspring raised outside her home range; the first egg was laid on March 29th and the last on May 28th. Female cgl17 had four offspring; the first three eggs were laid inside her home range whereas the last one was outside. Finally, female cgl9 laid the first egg inside her home range, the next two eggs outside, the fourth egg inside, the fifth outside and the last four eggs inside again. See  Supplementary Material, Appendix 1, Table A2 (Rp_04_Supplementary Electronic Material_Bolopo_D. et al.pdf) for detailed information on laying dates and distances to the home ranges.

Habitat use

Compositional analysis and Wilcoxon signed rank tests for second-order habitat selection detected significant avoidance of open areas and preference for pines plantations by the individuals when establishing their home ranges (Table 2), compared to the other habitats available. The compositional analysis for third-order selection detected that Great Spotted Cuckoos significantly preferred to visit areas of riparian vegetation whereas they avoided the urban, forest and open parts of their home ranges, and used pine plantations according to availability (Table 2).

Home range overlaps and social bonds

The radio-tracked home ranges of six dyads overlapped in 2009 as did those of 20 dyads in 2010, indicating that Great Spotted Cuckoos did not defend exclusive territories. Home range overlaps ranged from 0.46% to 100% among dyads, with an average of 27.8%. Average overlap of the different dyad types was: FF dyads 11.0% (N = 4, range 1.6-24.9%), FM dyads 33.9% (N = 16, range 0.8-100%), MF dyads 27.9% (N = 16, range 0.5-84.4) and MM dyads 25.5% (N = 16, range 1.1-86.2). There were no statistical differences in overlap between the different types of dyads (χ2 = 1.324, d.f. = 3, P = 0.723), although overlap values were higher in 2009 than 2010 (estimate ± SE = –25.47 ± 9.62, χ2 = 7.01, d.f. = 1, P = 0.008).

We found a total of eight dyads whose home range areas overlapped more than 40% (see  Supplementary Material, Appendix 1, Table A3 (Rp_04_Supplementary Electronic Material_Bolopo_D. et al.pdf) for details). The dynamic interaction analysis showed attraction between individuals for three of the dyads with the time and distance thresholds chosen ( Supplementary Material, Appendix 1, Table A3 (Rp_04_Supplementary Electronic Material_Bolopo_D. et al.pdf)). Dyads formed by male cgl10 and female cgl11 and, male cgl23 and female cgl24 showed significant attraction values (P < 0.05) whereas the dyad male cgl13 and female cgl17 only showed a tendency for attraction ( Supplementary Material, Appendix 1, Table A3 (Rp_04_Supplementary Electronic Material_Bolopo_D. et al.pdf)).

Table 2

Results of one-sample Wilcoxon signed rank tests for each habitat type to evaluate the significance of the selection ratio for second- and third-order habitat selection of the Great Spotted Cuckoos (N = 15). Negative significant estimates indicate avoidance of the habitat type and positive indicate preference. Significant results at P < 0.05 in bold. [Resultados del test de Wilcoxon en cada tipo de hábitat, para evaluar la significación de los análisis de segundo y tercer orden de selección del hábitat de los críalos (N = 15). Valores negativos indican evitación, positivos preferencia. Resultados significativos en negrita.]

img-z11-4_39.gif

Three male-female dyads seemed to constitute social pairs. Besides the attraction showed by the dynamic interaction analysis, their home ranges had similar shapes and males' home ranges were always overlapped almost completely by those of the females (99.9%, 99.8% and 100%). The common home range areas of the social pairs were also overlapped by other individuals of the population, both males and females (Figure 2), suggesting a lack of pair exclusive territoriality too. Genetic data showed successful breeding between male cgl13 and female cgl17, with two offspring raised in nests inside their shared home range (Figure 2). Surprisingly, we also found an extra-pair offspring of male cgl13 and an extra-pair offspring of female cgl17 together in a nest outside both individuals' home ranges (Figure 2 empty triangle,  Supplementary Material, Appendix 1, Table A1 (Rp_04_Supplementary Electronic Material_Bolopo_D. et al.pdf)). We found no offspring of the putative pair cgl10-cgl11, although we observed them copulating once. Instead, both individuals had descendants with different mates: male cgl10 within their shared home range, and female cgl11 outside both of their home ranges ( Supplementary Material, Appendix 1, Table A1 (Rp_04_Supplementary Electronic Material_Bolopo_D. et al.pdf)). No offspring of either individuals cgl23 and cgl24 were found.

Fig. 2.

Representation of radio-tracked home ranges (95% UD) of the Great Spotted Cuckoo and parasitised host nests. Grey shaded polygons represent female home ranges, dashed lines represent male home ranges. Black dots represent locations of parasitised nests with offspring belonging to adult cuckoos that were not radio-tagged. Open dots represent parasitised nests where genetic samples could not be collected due to hatching failure. Fully overlapped home ranges, on the left side, represent the social pair cgl13 (male) – cgl17 (female). Fully overlapped home ranges, on the right side, represent the social pair cgl10 (male) – cgl11 (female). Full triangles represent the location of common offspring of the cgl10cgl11 social pair. Open triangle represent the locations of two offspring that cgl10 and cgl11 obtained with extra-pair mates. [Representación de las áreas de campeo obtenidas con datos de radioseguimiento de los críalos y de las localizaciones de los nidos parasitados. Los polígonos coloreados indican las áreas de campeo de las hembras, las líneas discontinuas las áreas de los machos. Los puntos negros representan los nidos parasitados que contienen pollos de adultos que no fueron radiomarcados. Los círculos indican los nidos parasitados en los que no se pudieron conseguir muestras genéticas debido a fallos en la eclosión de los huevos. En el lado izquierdo, las dos áreas de campeo totalmente solapadas, pertenecen a la pareja social cgl13 (macho)cgl17 (hembra). Las dos áreas de campeo totalmente solapadas, a la derecha, representan la pareja social ‘pair’ cgl10 (macho)cgl11 (hembra). Los triángulos rellenos indican los pollos de la pareja cgl10 – cgl11. El triángulo vacío indica pollos que cgl10 y cgl11 obtuvieron mediante cópulas extrapareja.]

img-z12-1_39.jpg

Discussion

This is the first study, to the best of our knowledge, using concurrent genetic and behavioural data to obtain a comprehensive view of a brood parasite's mating system. Although the difficulties of data collection inevitably limited our sample sizes, the results shed light on the use of space in the Great Spotted Cuckoo, the socio-ecological factors that drive it and how these factors ultimately explain the combination of social monogamy and genetic polygamy uncovered in this study population.

From ranging behaviour to mating system

It has been long recognised that the environment affects the mating system of many animal populations by shaping their spatial behaviour (Davies et al., 2012). Such a link has not yet been explored in brood parasites, where the dependence on suitable hosts for reproduction adds further complexity. Theory predicts that a lack of territoriality should increase polygamous mating, by favouring encounters among potential sexual partners (Hauber & Dearborn, 2003). According to this hypothesis, we found that our highly polygamous Great Spotted Cuckoos (Bolopo et al., 2017) did not defend exclusive territories. The radio-tracked home ranges of the tagged individuals overlapped widely but surely underestimated the extent to which cuckoos share the space. Indeed, the genetic data revealed that i) we radio-tracked only a fraction of the whole population, ii) the “effective” home range of females, i.e. including laying areas, was bigger than their radio-tracked home range and iii) several different females laid their eggs inside the radio-tracked home ranges of other females (Figure 2). Therefore, based on complementary genetic and movement data, we define the Great Spotted Cuckoo as non-territorial in this population.

We suggest that a lack of territoriality, and ultimately polygamy, in the Great Spotted Cuckoo depends largely on the ecology of its hosts in the studied area. At Sobarriba, cuckoos mostly moved around riparian vegetation and pine plantations, which are the most productive foraging areas for the species, due to the abundance of caterpillars (pers. observ.), including the cuckoos' main prey, the Pine Processionary Moth Thaumetopoea pityocampa caterpillars (Valverde, 1953; Mestre Raventos, 1968). These are not, however, the preferred nesting habitats of their hosts (Soler, 1990; Canestrari et al., 2008, Baglione et al., 2016), forcing cuckoos to search for nests away from their feeding areas. The partial detachment between foraging and laying grounds may prevent territoriality, by precluding efficient patrolling and eviction of intruders, as Martínez et al. (1998) suggested for a population at Guadix (southern Spain). In addition, the peculiar preference of the Great Spotted Cuckoo for crow nests instead of magpie nests at our study site (Baglione et al., 2017), may constrain territoriality even further. Unlike magpies, which are also common at Sobarriba, crows defend large all-purpose territories and their nests are uniformly dispersed over relatively large areas (Baglione et al., 2005), making their defence unaffordable for the parasite.

Population density

Contrary to expectations, Martínez et al. (1998) found that the laying ranges of mainly monogamous females of Great Spotted Cuckoos also overlapped largely in Guadix (Granada, Andalucía, southern Spain). Therefore, although no information is available for males of the Guadix population, it seems that (non) territoriality alone cannot unequivocally predict the genetic mating pattern in this species. Plausibly, the rate of encounter of potential partners might depend also on the population density, as suggested by a previous comparative study that found that the levels of polygamy increased with cuckoo numbers (Bolopo et al., 2017). We therefore suggest that non-territoriality is a “permissive” step towards polygamy, which might arise only when the density of the population reaches a given threshold.

Flexible ranging behaviour?

In this study we have observed that radio-tracked home range size varied annually, being larger in 2010 than in 2009. This could be due to an increase in population density in 2010 (Baglione et al., 2017) that forced cuckoos to use larger areas to find enough host nests. However, given the large geographic variability of host territorial behaviour, which ranges from defence of exclusive territories to semi-coloniality (Birkhead, 1991; Baglione et al., 2005), we might ask whether the Great Spotted Cuckoo goes beyond simple adjustment of home range size and matches the flexibility of its hosts. Intriguingly, in a southern Spanish population in the Sierra Morena, where the cuckoo and its preferred host, the magpie, share foraging grounds and where host nests are especially aggregated compared to other areas (Arias de Reyna, 1998), this brood parasite was reported as territorial. It must be noted, however, that indications of territoriality (Arias de Reyna, 1998 and references therein) were based on data collected from unmarked individuals and should be taken with caution. The extent of plasticity of the territorial behaviour of the Great Spotted Cuckoo and its parallel effect on its mating patterns therefore need further investigation, ideally by applying appropriate standardised methodology (e.g. traceable marked individuals) in multiple populations.

Reconciling social monogamy and genetic polygamy

Specialist brood parasites are expected to rely on female/male cooperation to find a sufficient number of suitable host nests to parasitise (Hauber & Dearborn, 2003). Pair bonding, in addition, should favour parasites that target large hosts that possess defensive adaptations, because the coordinated action of two partners may increase the chances of laying (Hauber & Dearborn, 2003). Social monogamy would therefore theoretically provide advantages to the Great Spotted Cuckoo, which fulfils the two conditions explained above. Indeed, Arias de Reyna (1998) described a “distraction strategy” in this species, where the male elicits a defensive attack by the magpie hosts, while the female approaches the nest inconspicuously and lays the egg. However, recent video-recorded observations showed that cuckoo females can endure the attack of magpie females inside the nest and manage to lay eggs, without the assistance of a partner (Soler et al., 2014). This shows that intra-pair cooperation in the Great Spotted Cuckoo might be beneficial but is not strictly necessary to parasitise the magpie host. The same situation applies to crows, where females have been observed parasitising nests both alone or in coalition with a mate (DB and VB personal observations). Cuckoos need to find the crow nests unattended to lay their egg, because an incubating crow female, which is double size of the cuckoo, passively protects her clutch very effectively (Canestrari et al., 2009). Crows, unlike magpies, cannot be lured into leaving the nest temporarily because they do not mob the parasite (Soler, 1990), but the coordinated action of a cuckoo pair might increase monitoring of crow nests, improving the chances of laying. The question therefore is, to what extent facultative intra-pair cooperation might select for sexual social bonding in a species where there are counteracting factors that favour polygamy (e.g. lack of territoriality, see above).

Our results show that Great Spotted Cuckoos can form social pairs during breeding season, but relationships are rather loose, and each individual can be solitary a substantial part of the time. Our Great Spotted Cuckoo putative pairs (i.e. pairs of opposite sex that shared a large proportion of their home range) were radio-tracked together significantly more often than with any other (tagged or untagged) individual. On these occasions, the pairs behaved as such, moving cohesively and coordinately (pers. obs.), and vocalising often. This contrasts sharply with the behaviour of two males that, in spite of sharing a large proportion of their home ranges, were observed together only once. Therefore, after combining genetic and social patterns, the mating system of this species at our study site is better envisioned as loose social monogamy with a high frequency of extra-pair copulations, rather than polygamy.

Conclusions

The facts that ranging behaviour alone could not predict the genetic mating pattern in the Great Spotted Cuckoo and that, genetic data did not accurately reflect the social interactions between sexes, highlights the need for combining genetic and behavioural information to understand the life history of brood parasites. Our data showed a complex scenario in the Great Spotted Cuckoo, where loose pair bonds can be maintained despite high levels of genetic polygamy and absence of territoriality. However, this might be only one outcome along a gradient of variation of the mating and ranging system caused by factors such as food distribution, host availability and ecology, and the parasite's population density.

img-z17-12_39.jpg

Acknowledgements.

We are grateful to Miguel González-Vélez, Benedicto González and Gloria Robles for help in the field. This study was financially supported by Spanish Plan Nacional I+D/FEDER (research projects CGL2011-27260 and CGL2016-77636-P to V.B.).

Author contributions.

Diana Bolopo: Study conception, manuscript writing, data analysis. Gianluca Roncalli: Data collection, manuscript critical review. Daniela Canestrari: Study conception, manuscript critical review. Vittorio Baglione: Study conception, manuscript writing, funding acquisition.

References

1.

Aebischer, N.J., Robertson, P.A. & Kenward, R.E. (1993). Composition analysis of habitat use from animal radio-tracking data. Ecology , 74: 1313–1325. Google Scholar

2.

Alderson, G.W., Gibbs, H.L. & Sealy, S.G. (1999). Parentage and kinship studies in an obligate brood parasitic bird, the Brown-headed Cowbird (Molothrus ater), using microsatellite DNA markers. Journal of Heredity , 90: 182–190. Google Scholar

3.

Álvarez, F. & Arias de Reyna, L. (1974). Mecanismos de parasitación por Clamator glandarius y defensa por Pica pica. Doñana Acta Vertebrata , 1: 43–66. Google Scholar

4.

Arias de Reyna, L. (1998). Coevolution of Great Spotted Cuckoo and its hosts. In, S.I. Rothstein & S.K. Robinson (Eds.): Parasitic birds and their hosts, studies in coevolution , pp. 129–142. Oxford University Press. Google Scholar

5.

Arias de Reyna, L., Recuerda, P., Trujillo, J., Corvillo, M. & Cruz, A. (1987). Territory in the Great Spotted Cuckoo (Clamator glandarius). Journal Für Ornithologie , 128: 231–239. Google Scholar

6.

Baglione, V., Bolopo, D., Canestrari, D., Martínez, J.G., Roldan, M., Vila, M. & Soler, M. (2017). Spatiotemporal variation of host use in a brood parasite: the role of the environment. Behavioral Ecology , 28: 49–58. Google Scholar

7.

Baglione, V., Marcos, J.M., Canestrari, D., Griesser, M., Andreotti, G., Bardini, C. & Bogliani, G. (2005). Does year-round territoriality rather than habitat saturation explain delayed natal dispersal and cooperative breeding in the carrion crow? Journal of Animal Ecology , 74: 842–851. Google Scholar

8.

Barnard, P. & Markus, M.B. (1989). Male copulation frequency and female competition for fertilizations in a promiscuous brood parasite, the pin-tailed whydah Vidua macroura. Ibis , 131: 421–425. Google Scholar

9.

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B.M. & Walker, S.C. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software , 67: 1–48. Google Scholar

10.

Benhamou, S., Valeix, M., Chamaillé-Jammes, S., Macdonald, D.W. & Loveridge, A.J. (2014). Movement-based analysis of interactions in African lions. Animal Behaviour , 90: 171–180. Google Scholar

11.

Birkhead, T.R. (1991). The magpies: the ecology and behaviour of black-billed and yellow-billed magpies. T. & A.D. Poyser Ltd. Google Scholar

12.

Bolopo, D., Canestrari, D., Martínez, J.G., Roldan, M., Macías-Sanchez, E., Vila, M., Soler, M. & Baglione, V. (2017). Flexible mating patterns in an obligate brood parasite. Ibis , 159: 103–112. Google Scholar

13.

Brouwer, L. & Griffith, S.C. (2019). Extra-pair paternity in birds. Molecular Ecology . 31. https://doi.org/10.1111/mec.15259. Google Scholar

14.

Burt, W.H. (1943). Territoriality and Home Range Concepts as Applied to Mammals. Journal of Mammalogy , 24: 346–352. Google Scholar

15.

Calenge, C. (2006). The package “adehabitat” for the R software: A tool for the analysis of space and habitat use by animals. Ecological Modelling , 197: 516–519. Google Scholar

16.

Canestrari, D., Chiarati, E., Marcos, J.M., Ekman, J. & Baglione, V. (2008). Helpers but not breeders adjust provisioning effort to year-round territory resource availability in carrion crows. Animal Behaviour , 76: 943–949. Google Scholar

17.

Canestrari, D., Marcos, J.M. & Baglione, V. (2009). Cooperative breeding in carrion crows reduces the rate of brood parasitism by great spotted cuckoos. Animal Behaviour , 77: 1337–1344. Google Scholar

18.

Canestrari, D., Vera, R., Chiarati, E., Marcos, J.M., Vila, M. & Baglione, V. (2010). False feeding: the trade-off between chick hunger and caregivers needs in cooperative crows. Behavioral Ecology , 21: 233–241. Google Scholar

19.

Carranza, J. (2016). Sexo y sistemas de apareamiento. In, J. Carranza (Ed.): Etología adaptativa: El comportamiento como producto de la selección natural , pp. 373–406. Universidad de Córdoba. Google Scholar

20.

Cramp, S. & Perrins, C.M. (1994). Handbook of the bird of Europe the Middle East and North Africa . The birds of the Western Palearctic. Volume VIII. Crows to finches. Oxford University Press. Google Scholar

21.

Cronin, E.W. & Sherman, P.W. (1977). A resource-based mating system: the orangerumped honeyguide. Living Bird , 15: 5–32. Google Scholar

22.

Davies, N.B. (2000). Cuckoos, cowbirds and other cheats. T. & A.D. Poyser. Google Scholar

23.

Davies, N.B., Krebs, J.R. & West, S.A. (2012). An introduction to behavioural ecology (4th ed.). John Wiley & Sons Ltd. Google Scholar

24.

Emlen, S. & Oring, L. (1977). Ecology, sexual selection, and the evolution of mating systems. Science , 197: 215–223. Google Scholar

25.

ESRI (2014). ArcGIS Desktop 10.2. Redlands, CA: Environmental Systems Research Institute. Google Scholar

26.

Feeney, W.E., Welbergen, J.A. & Langmore, N.E. (2014). Advances in the study of coevolution between avian brood parasites and their hosts. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics , 45: 227–246. Google Scholar

27.

Feeney, W.E. & Riehl, C. (2019). Monogamy without parental care? Social and genetic mating systems of avian brood parasites. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences , 374: 1769. Google Scholar

28.

Fuller, M.R., Millspaugh, J.J., Church, K.E. & Kenward, R.E. (2005). Wildlife radiotelemetry. In, C.E. Braun (Ed.): Techniques for wildlife investigations and management (6th ed.), pp. 377–417. The Wildlife Society. Google Scholar

29.

Hauber, M.E. & Dearborn, D.C. (2003). Parentage without parental care: What to look for in genetic studies of obligate brood-parasitic mating systems. The Auk , 120: 1–13. Google Scholar

30.

Johnson, D. (1980). The comparison of usage and availability measurements for evaluating resource preference. Ecology , 61: 65–71. Google Scholar

31.

Jones, D.A., Gibbs, H.L., Matsuda, T., Brooke, M.L., Uchida, H. & Bayliss, M.J. (1997). The use of DNA fingerprinting to determine the possible mating system of an obligate brood parasitic bird, the Cuckoo Cuculus canorus. Ibis , 139: 560–562. Google Scholar

32.

Kruger, O. (2007). Cuckoos, cowbirds and hosts: adaptations, trade-offs and constraints. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences , 362: 1873–1886. Google Scholar

33.

Lack, D. (1968). Ecological Adaptations for Breeding in Birds. Methuen. Google Scholar

34.

Langmore, N.E., Adcock, G.J. & Kilner, R.M. (2007). The spatial organization and mating system of Horsfield's bronze-cuckoos, Chalcites basalis. Animal Behaviour , 74: 403–412. Google Scholar

35.

Long, J.A. (2014). wildlifeDI: Calculate indices of dynamic interaction for wildlife telemetry data. Retrieved from  https://cran.r-project.org/package=wildlifeDI  Google Scholar

36.

Marchetti, K., Nakamura, H. & Gibbs, H.L. (1998). Host-Race Formation in the Common Cuckoo. Science , 282: 471–472. Google Scholar

37.

Martínez, J.G., Burke, T., Dawson, D., Soler, J.J., Soler, M. & Møller, A.P. (1998). Microsatellite typing reveals mating patterns in the brood parasitic great spotted cuckoo (Clamator glandarius). Molecular Ecology , 7: 289–297. Google Scholar

38.

Martínez, J.G., Soler, J.J., Soler, M. & Burke, T. (1998). Spatial patterns of egg laying and multiple parasitism in a brood parasite: A non-territorial system in the great spotted cuckoo (Clamator glandarius). Oecologia , 117: 286–294. Google Scholar

39.

Mestre Raventos, P. (1968). Sobre Clamator glandarius en el Panadés (Cataluña). Ardeola , 14: 137–142. Google Scholar

40.

Moskát, C., Bán, M., Fülöp, A., Bereczki, J. & Hauber, M.E. (2019). Bimodal habitat use in brood parasitic common cuckoos (Cuculus canorus) revealed by GPS telemetry. The Auk , 136: 1–12. Google Scholar

41.

Nakamura, H. & Miyazawa, Y. (1997). Movements, space use and social organisation of radio tracked Common Cuckoos during the breeding season in Japan. Japanese Journal of Ornithology , 46: 23–54. Google Scholar

42.

Nakamura, H., Miyazawa, Y. & Kashiwagi, K. (2005). Behavior of radio-tracked Common Cuckoo females during the breeding season in Japan. Ornithological Science , 4: 31–41. Google Scholar

43.

Payne, R.B. & Payne, K. (1977). Social organization and mating success in local song populations of Village Indigobirds Vidua chalybeata. Zeitschrift Für Tierpsychologie , 45: 113–173. Google Scholar

44.

R Core Team (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Retrieved from  http://www.r-project.org/  Google Scholar

45.

Rothstein, S.I., Verner, J. & Steven, E. (1984). Radio-tracking confirms a unique diurnal pattern of spatial occurrence in the parasitic Brown-headed cowbird. Ecology , 65: 77–88. Google Scholar

46.

Rühmann, J., Soler, M., Pérez-Contreras, T. & Ibáñez-Álamo, J.D. (2019). Territoriality and variation in home range size through the entire annual range of migratory great spotted cuckoos (Clamator glandarius). Scientific Reports , 9: 1–11. Google Scholar

47.

Scardamaglia, R.C. & Reboreda, J.C. (2014). Ranging behavior of female and male shiny cowbirds and screaming cowbirds while searching for host nests. The Auk , 131: 610–618. Google Scholar

48.

Soler, J.J., Soler, M., Perez-Contreras, T., Aragon, S. & Moller, A.P. (1999). Antagonistic anti-parasite defenses: nest defense and egg rejection in the magpie host of the great spotted cuckoo. Behavioral Ecology , 10: 707–713. Google Scholar

49.

Soler, M. (1990). Relationships between the great spotted cuckoo Clamator glandarius and its corvid hosts in a recently colonized area. Ornis Scandinavica , 21: 212–223. Google Scholar

50.

Soler, M., Pérez-Contreras, T. & de Neve, L. (2014). Great spotted cuckoos frequently lay their eggs while their magpie host is incubating. Ethology , 120: 965–972. Google Scholar

51.

Soler, M. (2014). Long-term coevolution between avian brood parasites and their hosts. Biological Reviews , 89: 688–704. Google Scholar

52.

Spottiswoode, C.N., Begg, K.S. & Begg, C.M. (2016). Reciprocal signaling in honeyguide-human mutualism. Science , 353: 387–389. Google Scholar

53.

Strausberger, B.M. & Ashley, M.V. (2003). Breeding biology of brood parasitic brown-headed cowbirds (Molothrus ater) characterized by parent-offspring and sibling-group reconstruction. The Auk , 120: 433–445. Google Scholar

54.

Valverde, J.A. (1953). Contribution a la biologie du coucou-geai, Clamator glandarius L. I. Notes sur le coucou-geai en Castilla. Oiseaux , 23: 288–296. Google Scholar

55.

Vogl, W., Taborsky, B., Taborsky, M., Teuschl, Y. & Honza, M. (2004). Habitat and space use of European cuckoo females during the egg laying period. Behaviour , 141: 881–898. Google Scholar

56.

Vogl, W., Taborsky, M., Taborsky, B., Teuschl, Y. & Honza, M. (2002). Cuckoo females preferentially use specific habitats when searching for host nests. Animal Behaviour , 64: 843–850. Google Scholar

57.

Woolfenden, B.E., Gibbs, H.L. & Sealy, S.G. (2002). High opportunity for sexual selection in both sexes of an obligate brood parasitic bird, the brown-headed cowbird (Molothrus ater). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology , 52: 417–425. Google Scholar

Appendices

Supplementary Electronic Material

Additional supporting information may be found in the on-line version of this paper. See the volume 67(1) on  www.ardeola.org

 Table A1. (Rp_04_Supplementary Electronic Material_Bolopo_D. et al.pdf) Data from all the captured Great Spotted Cuckoos.

 Table A2. (Rp_04_Supplementary Electronic Material_Bolopo_D. et al.pdf) Known offspring of the radio-tracked individuals.

 Table A3. (Rp_04_Supplementary Electronic Material_Bolopo_D. et al.pdf) Description of the data obtained from dyads of individuals whose home ranges overlapped by 40% or more.

 Table A4. (Rp_04_Supplementary Electronic Material_Bolopo_D. et al.pdf) Factors affecting the home range size of the Great Spotted Cuckoo. Results of a GLMM, individual ID set as random factor. Data including only those relocations of 2010 starting on the same date as in 2009 (ie from May 3rd).

 Table A5. (Rp_04_Supplementary Electronic Material_Bolopo_D. et al.pdf) Factors affecting the home range size of the Great Spotted Cuckoo. Results of a GLMM, individual ID set as random factor. Data including only the maximum number of relocations per individual obtained in 2009 (ie 34 relocations).

Diana Bolopo, Gianluca Roncalli, Daniela Canestrari, and Vittorio Baglione "Cheating Cheaters: Social Monogamy but Genetic Polygamy in a Brood Parasite," Ardeola 67(1), 39-56, (16 January 2020). https://doi.org/10.13157/arla.67.1.2020.ra4
Received: 25 July 2019; Accepted: 18 November 2019; Published: 16 January 2020
KEYWORDS
área de campeo
Clamator glandarius
críalo europeo
Great Spotted Cuckoo
home range
interacción social
lazos de pareja
Back to Top