Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 December 2013 Mating Behavior of the Six-Banded Armadillo Euphractus Sexcinctus in the Pantanal Wetland, Brazil
Walfrido Moraes Tomas, Zilca CamposA, Arnaud Léonard Jean Desbiez, Danilo Kluyber, Paulo André Lima Borges, Guilherme Mourão
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Little is known about the reproductive behavior of Euphractus sexcinctus, but the chasing behavior of several individuals behind one another has been reported. We describe two observations of chasing behavior in the Pantanal, Brazil, which included a clear mating event. On these two occasions, a presumed adult male mounted another individual, presumed to be an adult female, during and after the chase, clearly indicating reproductive behavior. Our occasional records of chasing behavior, eventually including mounting, suggest a fairly defined mating period from the mid-dry season to the onset of the raining season in the Pantanal wetland.

The ecology and natural history of the six-banded or yellow armadillo (Euphractus sexcinctus Linnaeus, 1758) is still largely unknown (Medri, 2008). This includes a lack of understanding of mating and pre-mating behavior. Published reports on the chasing behavior among armadillos have been considered as an indication of reproductive competition among male six-banded armadillos (Desbiez et al., 2006), or as an aggressive display of adult male nine-banded armadillos (Dasypus novemcinctus) towards juveniles (Breece & Dusi, 1985) and one- to two-year-old males (McDonough, 1994). These chasing events have also been reported for the Brazilian three-banded armadillo (Tolypeutes tricinctus) (Marinho-Filho apud Guimarães, 1997).

On two occasions we recorded in digital video a group of adult six-banded armadillos, presumably males (‘males’), chasing another adult presumed as female (‘female’) in the southern Pantanal wetland, Brazil. The first observation occurred in July 2006 (19°02′06″S, 56°46′04″W) at approximately 17:00 hr, when we witnessed a successful attempt of one of the ‘males’ in mounting the ‘female’. Five ‘males’ were in a line running after one ‘female’, and one of the ‘males’ mounted the ‘female’ in a quick movement when it briefly stopped during the chase. After being mounted successfully the ‘female’ quickly resumed running, with the ‘male’ still attached (Fig. 1). The remaining ‘males’ started chasing the couple. The mounted running was as fast as when individuals were running alone and occurred for nearly 20 m, without release by the ‘male’.

Figure1.

A presumably female six-banded armadillo (Euphractus sexcinctus) mounted by a presumed male during a collective race, chased by another presumed male, in the Pantanal wetland, Brazil, July 2006. Photo: Walfrido Tomas.

f01_87.jpg

The mounted run finished when the ‘female’ entered a burrow, with the ‘male’ being left outside. While mounting, both ‘male’ and ‘female’ were running coordinately in a wandering line until the ‘female’ found the burrow hidden in a bunch grass bush (the video is available at  http://www.cpap.embrapa.br/imagens_documentais/euphractus_mating_walfrido_tomas_jul_2006.avi).

The second observation occurred on 16 October 2012 around 17:00 hr (19°08′36″S, 56°50′50″W). A pair of armadillos (presumably a female mounted by a male) crossed in front of our vehicle, chased by three other adults, presumably males. Again, the ‘female’ entered a burrow, with the ‘male’ being detached from the ‘female's’ back, and started digging and filling the entrance with sand. The ‘male’ that had mounted the ‘female’, as well as the other ‘males’ involved in the chasing event, started to dig vigorously at the burrow entrance and the surrounding substrate in an attempt to reach the ‘female’. The shoot was stopped around 18:00 hr due to the lack of light conditions to continue.

These two observations indicate that the chasing behavior reported by Desbiez et al. (2006) is a pre-mating behavior, or at least one of the mating repertories of six-banded armadillos. The unusually long penis of six-banded armadillo males, which can reach about 33% of its body length (McDonough & Loughry, 2001), may represent an adaptation to ensure fertilization during these mounted runs, if the reported events represent a pre-mating or even mating behavior. However, it is unclear if complete copulation is achieved during the mounted run or if E. sexcinctus females would accept another male afterwards. While a published description of a male mounting a female T. tricinctus while moving slowly (Marini-Filho & Guimarães, 2010) may be similar, it contrasts with the fast locomotion we witnessed.

The behavior we witnessed during the mounted runs differs substantially from the agonistic interaction between individuals reported for D. novemcinctus (Breece & Dusi, 1985; McDonough, 1994). Additionally, we found the observed behavior of E. sexcinctus to be different from the described attempt to prevent the access of a male competitor to a female in T. tricinctus (Marinho-Filho apud Guimarães, 1997; Marini-Filho & Guimarães, 2010). In both occasions the E. sexcinctus ‘males’ involved in the chase did not interact directly with one another. Several additional, anecdotal observations in the Pantanal involving two to six males also did not include any type of direct interactions among the males. Therefore, the competition for mating in E. sexcinctus seems to be based on the ability to be the first to reach and mount the female rather than any aggressive behaviors between males.

The frantic excavation by several males when a female is inside a burrow, such as described here, may explain the concentration of burrows in certain areas, often observed in the Pantanal. It has also been observed that the males may continue the chase inside the burrows, emerging at other openings in search of the female.

Our records help to clarify at least one of the questions raised by Desbiez et al. (2006). These authors were not certain whether the chasing behavior they reported had a reproductive function or rather represented a strategy to defend territories or food resources. Although no specimen was captured or sex-discriminated, some of the photos we have taken showed the erect penis of the mounting armadillo (Fig. 2), indicating that the chasing and mounting behaviors represent a reproductive event.

Figure2.

A male six-banded armadillo (Euphractus sexcinctus) mounting a female during a chase event in the Pantanal wetland, Brazil. Photo: The Pantanal Giant Armadillo Project.

f02_87.jpg

Finally, we recorded 11 events of chasing behavior by E. sexcinctus from 2006 to 2013 in the Pantanal (three in July, two in August, one in September, three in October, and two in November), and no records in other periods of the year. Mountings were observed three times, always associated with runs or chasings. These occasional records of chasing behavior from July to November in the Pantanal suggest a slightly defined mating period lasting from the mid-dry season to the onset of the raining season, which is in agreement with the records of pregnant females found in Central Brazil (September and October) and in Uruguay (January) (Bralow, 1965), as gestation lasts 60 to 64 days (Redford, 1985).

Acknowledgements

We thank Embrapa Pantanal for the logistic support during field work in which observations were made. We are also thankful to the Royal Zoological Society of Scotland for the support given to the Giant Armadillo Project.

References

1.

J. C. Barlow 1965. Land mammals from Uruguay: ecology and zoogeography. Doctoral Thesis, University of Kansas, Lawrence. 346 pp. Google Scholar

2.

G. A. Breece & J. L. Dusi . 1985. Food habits and home range of the common long-nosed armadillo Dasypus novemcinctus in Alabama. Pp. 419–427 in: The evolution and ecology of armadillos, sloths, and vermilinguas (G. G. Montgomery, ed.). Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, DC. Google Scholar

3.

A. L. J. Desbiez , P. A. Lima Borges & I. M. Medri . 2006. Chasing behavior in yellow armadillos, Euphractus sexcinctus, in the Brazilian Pantanal. Edentata 7: 51–53. Google Scholar

4.

M. M. Guimarães 1997. Área de vida, territorialidade e dieta do tatu-bola Tolypeutes tricinctus (Xenarthra, Dasypodidae) num Cerrado do Brasil Central. Master's thesis, Instituto de Ciências Biológicas, Universidade de Brasília, Brasília. Google Scholar

5.

O. J. Marini-Filho & M. M. Guimarães . 2010. Comportamento sexual de tatu-bola (Tolypeutes tricinctus, Dasypodidae). Edentata 11: 76–77. Google Scholar

6.

C. M. McDonough 1994. Determinants of aggression in nine-banded armadillos. Journal of Mammalogy 75:189–198. Google Scholar

7.

C. M. McDonough & W. J. Loughry . 2001. Armadillos. Pp. 796–799 in: The new encyclopedia of mammals (D. Macdonald, ed.) Oxford University Press, Oxford. Google Scholar

8.

I. M. Medri 2008. Ecologia e história natural de tatu--peba, Euphractus sexcinctus (Linnaeus 1758) no Pantanal da Nhecolândia, Mato Grosso do Sul. Doctoral Thesis, Universidade de Brasília, Brasília. 167 pp. Google Scholar

9.

K. H. Redford & R. M. Wetzel . 1985. Euphractus sexcinctus. Mammalian Species 252: 1–4. Google Scholar
Walfrido Moraes Tomas, Zilca CamposA, Arnaud Léonard Jean Desbiez, Danilo Kluyber, Paulo André Lima Borges, and Guilherme Mourão "Mating Behavior of the Six-Banded Armadillo Euphractus Sexcinctus in the Pantanal Wetland, Brazil," Edentata 14(1), 87-89, (1 December 2013). https://doi.org/10.5537/020.014.0113
Received: 19 November 2013; Accepted: 24 December 2013; Published: 1 December 2013
KEYWORDS
acasalamento
behavior
comportamento
Euphractus sexcinctus
mating
Pantanal
tatu-peba
Back to Top