Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 September 2013 Movement Efficiency and Behavior of Termites (Isoptera) in Tunnels with Varying Pore Sizes
Sook Junk Ku, Nan-Yao Su, Sang-Hee Lee
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Subterranean termites (Isoptera) build tunnel networks to obtain food resources and nesting space. When tunneling, termites encounter spatial heterogeneity, such as differing soil pore space and moisture levels. This heterogeneity creates 2 types of irregularities in the tunnel surface: (1) a bumpy tunnel-wall structure caused by variable soil texture; and (2) hollow space within the tunnel, where tunnels pass through soil pores. We previously explored the response of termites to bumpy tunnel structure. In the present study, we investigated termite behavior in response to differing volumes of soil pore space. This response behavior is closely related to movement efficiency. We designed 8-cm-long artificial tunnels with hollow spaces in a 2-dimensional arena. The hollow spaces were represented as circular holes with varying diameters D (2, 3, 4, 6, 8, or 10 mm), positioned at the center of the tunnel. Tunnel widths, W, were 2 and 3 mm. We systematically observed the movement of termites (Reticulitermes speratus kyushuensis Morimoto) at the hole, and measured the time required (τ) for termites to pass through the tunnels. Time τ was shorter for tunnels with holes between 2 and 6 mm diameter than for those with holes of 8 or 10 mm diameter, for tunnels of both widths. Time τ was significantly different between (W, D) = (2, 10) and (3, 10). These results were explained by 3 types of behaviors. The implications of these findings are briefly discussed in relation to termite foraging efficiency.

Subterranean termites are social insects that form colonies often containing hundreds of thousands to millions of individuals. Colony members disperse throughout the soil, constructing underground tunnel networks up to hundreds of meters long for foraging (Su et al. 1984). Construction of the tunnel network brings a termite colony into contact with food resources sometimes with the aids of external cues such as thermal shadows (Ettershank et al. 1980). The network systems are, therefore, a record of the foraging activities of these insects. In other words, the patterns of the networks include information on the trade-offs between foraging efficiency and other biological and environmental constraints, such as the number of active foragers, soil density, and food availability (Venna & Garneshaiah 1991; Hedlung & Henderson 1999; Su and Puche 2003; Le Comber et al. 2006a, b). The tradeoff is a type of foraging strategy in which termites efficiently allocate time and energy to obtaining and transporting food according to environmental conditions (Robson et al. 1995; Lee et al. 2006, 2007a). To better understand termite foraging strategy, it is important to analyze tunnel patterns in relation to the costs and benefits of constructing the tunnels (Okubo 1980; Shlesinger et al. 1995; Diekmann et al. 2000). However, few studies have addressed termite foraging behavior or strategy in relation to the entire tunnel system of a field colony because it is technically difficult to directly observe tunnel networks that may extend laterally over extensive (>1000 m2) areas (Su, 2001).

To address this difficulty, Lee et al. (2006) proposed a simple model to simulate the tunnel networks of Coptotermes formosanus Shiraki and Reticulitermes flavipes (Kollar) using tunneling rules based on experimental data. The model was used to reveal the relationship between food encounter rate and food-size distribution pattern. Food encounter rate was defined as the ratio of the number of food particles encountered by tunnels to the number of simulation events. Results of Lee at al. (2006) showed that the food encounter rate was the highest when food resources were heterogeneously distributed. Lee et al. (2007b) created a heterogeneous landscape structure in the lattice space using a neutral landscape model (Gardner et al. 1987; With 1997) and incorporated the landscape into the lattice model for the tunnel patterns. Their simulation revealed that landscape heterogeneity strongly influences food encounter rate in the presence of high food density, and that this influence was greater for primary tunnels. These 2 studies addressed food encounter rate rather than foraging efficiency, because the encounter rate is a simpler calculation and it indirectly reflects foraging efficiency. Lee & Su (2009) explored the effect of branching tunnels on foraging efficiency, defined as the ratio of the number of food particles encountered to total tunnel length. The amount of food encountered indicates the quantity of energy obtained, and the total tunnel length represents the energy expended by termites to excavate the tunnels. Lee & Su (2009) showed that the length and frequency of branching tunnels plays an important role in foraging efficiency. Jeon et al. (2010) developed a continuous model based on a 2-dimensional tunnel network pattern. Using the model simulation, they found that the best strategy, maximizing the foraging efficiency, for C. formosanus and R. flavipes would occur if the frequency of branching and the length of branching tunnels were simultaneously enhanced.

These previous studies were helpful in understanding foraging behavior and strategy in relation to overall tunnel pattern. However, the models used in these studies did not consider the behavior of termites at the individual level, and all individuals were assumed to be identical to engage in the same tunnel-digging activity, and to have the ability to move from place to place instantaneously. Termites foraging efficiency consists of food-finding and foodtransportation efficiencies (Lee et al. 2007a). Transportation efficiency relates to the speed with which termites move their food from the foraging site to their nest. Movement efficiency is closely related to food-transportation efficiency (Lee et al. 2007a; 2009), and is defined as the time taken by a termite to cover a certain tunnel distance. Obviously, termites with higher mean velocities are likely to contribute positively to transportation efficiency. For this reason, foraging efficiency used in the previous studies should be reconsidered, accounting for the role of individual termite behavior in movement efficiency.

Under field conditions, termites respond differently to local tunnel geometry (e.g., curvature and wall irregularity), which in turn strongly affects movement efficiency (Lee et al. 2008a, b). Sim & Lee (2012a, b) showed that tunnel curvature affects movement efficiency. Tunnels with a greater proportion of bends are associated with lower movement efficiency. Curvature also influences the time required to pass, when 2 termites encounter each other in the tunnel. 2 types of irregularities are common in termite tunnels: bumpy wall surfaces and hollow tunnel space. Bumpy wall surfaces may result from heterogeneity in soil properties such as particle size and moisture content (Bosnia et al. 1993; Jacques et al. 1999). Tunnel walls that have lower moisture contents are likely to be more fragile, and as termite bodies or external forces contact or act on the walls, bumpy surfaces may form. In addition, large soil particles encountered by termites while tunneling are not removed, which can result in a bumpy wall surface. Hollow tunnel space can be formed via biological or physical activity (Burke et al. 1999; Zue et al. 2008). When a termite tunnel passes through soil pore spaces with diameters larger than the tunnel width, the altered geometry at the point of intersection can result in hollow tunnel space. The size and shape of this hollow space vary according to soil heterogeneity (Yuki et al. 2006).

Fig. 1.

Experimental arena consisting of 2 layers (16 × 8 cm, 5 mm thick) of clear plexiglas and a middle, dark gray-colored layer (16 × 8 cm, 2 mm high). The middle layer (dark gray color) includes five 8-cm-long artificial tunnels with width W. A circular hole with diam D was located at the center of each tunnel.

f01_810.jpg

To our knowledge, few studies have been conducted on the movement efficiency of termites in relation to tunnel irregularity. Ku et al. (2012), who measured time taken by termites to pass the irregularity (simplified as rectangular shapes), found 4 types of response behaviors, and termites' passing times differed according to the height and width of the irregularity

In this study, we focused on termite movement efficiency in tunnels with different-sized hollow space. For simplification of the experiment and the interpretation of termite behavior, we considered the hollow space as a circular hole. We measured the time required for termites to pass through tunnels with circular holes of varying diameters, and carefully analyzed termite behavior in the holes.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

We collected the termite Reticulitermes speratus kyushuensis Morimoto from dead wood at Mt. Kyeryong, South Korea. This species is widely distributed in Korea and Japan (Park & Bae 1997). Termites were immediately separated from the dead wood following the methods of Tamashiro et al. (1973), and were placed inside a chamber containing wooden sticks as a food source. The chamber was maintained at a temperature of 28 ± 2 °C.

We used a 2-dimensional foraging arena for the experiment. The experimental arena consisted of 3 layers (16 × 8 cm, 5 mm thick) of clear Plexiglas with a dark gray middle layer (16 × 8 cm, 2 mm high) between 2 outer layers. We cut linear tunnel shapes of 2 widths W (W = 2,3 mm) in the middle layer, for the artificial tunnels. Each artificial tunnel was formed with a circular hole, representing soil pore space, at the center. The diameter of the hole, D, was 2, 3, 4, 6, 8, or 10 mm (Fig. 1). The middle layer included 5 artificial tunnels. Two introduction (entry) holes (1 cm diameter) were made at the end of each tunnel.

TABLE 1.

MEAN (± SD) TIME T VALUES (S) FOR TERMITES PASSING THROUGH TUNNELS WITH CIRCULAR HOLES OF W = 2 OR 3 MM, AND D = 2, 3, 4, 6, 8, AND 10 MM.

t01_810.gif

Fig. 2.

Snapshots of termite walking behavior through circular holes with W = 2 mm, and D = 2, 4, 6, 8, and 10 mm. Yellow triangles indicate antennae touching the sides of the hole. Consecutive yellow arrows indicate the movement path of the walking termite. The word “stopping” indicates that a termite is showing stopping behavior at the center of the hole.

f02_810.jpg

The arenas were maintained at 29 ± 1°C, in a room with relative humidity of 40 ± 2%. Five arenas were prepared for the study. Each arena was placed in a horizontal position, and 5 worker termites were placed into each tunnel through the introduction hole. We allowed termites to acclimatize to their new environment for 12 min (Hu et al. 2003; Wilson-Rich et al. 2007) before recording their movements for one hour with a digital camcorder (SONY HDR-CX700) mounted on the top of each arena. For each combination of W and D, 25 repetitions were performed. One hundred clips showing a single termite passing through the 8-cm tunnel were extracted from each video. Data from worker termites that did not make physical contact with neighboring termites while walking in the tunnels were used in the analysis. Using the recorded video footage, we measured the time (τ) taken by each termite to pass through the tunnel. Time τ was defined as the time taken for a termite to enter and exit the tunnel.. For statistical analysis, we used a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Tukey's post hoc and Scheffe's test for multiple comparisons and applied to a t-test for statistical comparison of the tunnel passing time.

TABLE 2.

STATISTICAL COMPARISON OF PASSING TIME τ, FOR TUNNELS WHERE W = 2 AND 0 = 2,4,6, 8, OR 10 MM; AND W = 3 AND D = 3, 4, 6, 8, AND 10 MM.

t02_810.gif

Fig. 3.

Snapshots of termite walking behavior through circular holes with W = 3 mm, and D = 4, 6, 8, and 10 mm.

f03_810.jpg

RESULTS

Termites remained in the introduction holes for approximately 3 min, rotating along the walls. This was probably a type of exploratory behavior to obtain information of the new environment. After this initial period, termites began to move into the tunnels, touching their antennae on the tunnel walls (Lee et al. 2008b). We allowed 12 min for termites to acclimate, after which we observed their walking behavior in the tunnels. We made observations of the way in which each termite passed the circular hole positioned at the center of its tunnel.

TABLE 3.

MEAN (± SD) TIME VALUES (S) REQUIRED FOR A TERMITE TO PASS THROUGH CIRCULAR HOLES WITH (W, D) = (2, 8), (2, 10), (3, 8) AND (3, 10). TIME WAS MEASURED FOR 2 BEHAVIORAL TYPES: THOSE THAT EXHIBITED STOPPING BEHAVIOR AT THE CENTER OF THE HOLE, AND THOSE THAT DID NOT.

t03_810.gif

Termites generally showed antennation behavior when walking through linear tunnels, and zigzagging behavior (Sim et al. 2012) for tunnels with W = 3 mm. For W = 2 mm, zigzagging behavior did not appear because termites did not have enough space to bend their bodies-a requirement for changing walking direction. Time duration of passage, τ. was significantly shorter for (W, D) = (2, 2), (2, 4), and (2, 6) than for (W, D) = (2, 8) and (2, 10) (Table 1). For (W, D) = (2, 2) (tunnel with no circular hole), termites walked without pausing, which resulted in a shorter passage time, τ. In the case of (W, D) = (2, 4), termites advanced into the circular hole and slightly touched the wall on the exit side of the hole (indicated by triangles in Fig. 2) without changing their moving direction. For (W, D) = (2, 6), termites bent their bodies to change their moving direction toward the left or right side of the wall. In this case, although termites changed their direction of movement toward the wall, they immediately reoriented and moved towards the exit, such that no significant increase in τ was observed (Table 1). Termites advancing to circular holes with (W, D) = (2, 8) also bent their bodies, and then touched one side of the wall and turned to the other side. After touching both side of the wall, termites exited the tunnels (see the movement path in Fig. 2). Interestingly, when moving from one side of the wall to the other, these termites frequently stopped at the center of the circular hole, resulting in increased τ (see Fig. 2). For (W, D) = (2, 10), termites showed similar behavior to that of (W, D) = (2, 8), but stopped for longer periods of time, further increasing τ (Fig. 2).

TABLE 4.

TERMITE BENDING ANGLES, θ (DEGREES), FORMED BETWEEN THE REAR END OF THE BODY AND THE CENTER OF THE MOUTH, FOR 2 TYPES OF WALKING BEHAVIOR: WALL-TOUCHING AND WALL-FOLLOWING.

t04_810.gif

In wider tunnels without circular holes [(W, D) = (3, 3)], termites showed similar behavior to those in tunnels with dimensions (W, D) = (2, 2), and τ did not differ statistically between these treatments (Table 2). Time τ for (W, D) = (3, 3) was significantly shorter than that for (W = 3, D = 4, 6, 8, and 10). Although termites tended to walk in a zigzag motion in tunnels with (W, D) = (3, 3), which essentially increased the length of their path through the tunnels, they walked at higher speeds in these wider tunnels compared to those in tunnels with (W, D) = (2, 2), which compensated for time expended in zigzag motions. When termites encountered a hole with (W, D) = (3, 4), some moved through without hesitation, while others touched the walls of the hole, hesitating for a short time. This behavior caused a statistically significant increase in τ (Fig. 3). When termites advanced into holes with (W, D) = (3, 6), (3, 8), and (3, 10), many touched one side of the wall, turned their bodies, touched the other side of the wall, and then moved out of the hole. As for holes with (W, D) = (2, 10), termites spent more time, motionless, in the center of holes with (W, D) = (3, 10), which increased τ.

Table 2 presents statistical comparisons of τ between (W = 2, D) and (W = 3, D). No differences in τ were found among D = 4, 6, and 8. In addition, τ was statistically the same for (W, D) = (2, 2) and (3, 3). However, τ was significantly longer for (W, D) = (2, 10) than for (W, D) = (3, 10).

DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS

Our results showed that fi01_810.gif was statistically shorter for (W, D) = (2, 2), (2, 4), and (2, 6) than for (W, D) = (2, 8) and (2, 10), and fi01_810.gif was statistically the same for (W, D) = (2,2), (2,4), and (2, 6). For tunnels with W = 3 mm, fi01_810.gif tended to increase with increasing D. These results can be explained by 3 types of behavior: (1) when termites encountered a smallsized hole—(W, D) = (2, 4), and (3, 4)—they touched the inside wall of the hole slightly, but advanced without stopping; (2) when termites encountered a medium-sized hole—(W, D) = (2, 6) and (3, 6)—they deviated their moving direction at the entrance of the hole to touch the left- or right-side wall of the hole, while continuing to advance without stopping; (3) for large-sized holes—(W, D) = (2, 8), (2, 10), (3, 8), and (3, 10)—termites touched the left- or rightside wall of the hole and then turned towards the wall on the other side. Immediately after touching both sides, termites exited the hole. In this case, when termites moved from one side of the wall to the other, they frequently stopped in the center of the hole. We speculate that in larger holes, termites would be completely separated from the wall when they moved from one side to the opposite side, and in the absence of a wall to guide their walking they may have lost the tactile cues and stopped walking. For holes with D < 8 mm, termites were almost in contact with the wall as they passed through the hole. We performed a statistical comparison of hole passing time for 2 cases, D = 8 and 10 mm: one in which a termite passed through the hole without stopping at the center; and one in which a termite stopped in the hole before passing through (Table 3). The results showed that stopping behavior strongly contributed to an increase in τ, suggesting that if tunnels intersect many larger-sized soil pores, termites> movement efficiency is likely to decrease. Because movement efficiency was defined as the time required for a termite to move between 2 defined points (Lee et al. 2008), an increase in τ would have a negative influence on foraging efficiency. In order to avoid this stopping behavior, termites may use a strategy in which they reduce hole size to a width that is compatible with their body size. Lee et al. (2008a) reported that when termites excavate tunnels, they move the excavated soil into a sharpangled corner to avoid decreasing their movement efficiency. This soil-filling behavior could also occur when termites encounter large soil pores. It would be interesting to experimentally investigate filling behavior in relation to soil pore size.

Fig. 4.

Snapshot representing the bending angle, θ, of a termite at the entrance of a circular hole.

f04_810.jpg

Fig. 5.

Wall-following and turning behavior of termites in circular holes with (W, D) = (3, 10).

f05_810.jpg

As shown in Table 3, intriguingly, termites paused longer in tunnels where (W, D) = (2,10) than in tunnels where (W, D) = (3, 10). Considering that the hole size was the same in both cases, the cause of this time difference is perplexing. We conjecture that for termites advancing from narrower tunnels, the 3 mm hole size may be relatively larger, which in turn may cause them to pause longer at center of the hole so as to acclimate to the perceived larger hole.

As shown in Figs. 2 and 3, most termites placed in tunnels where (W, D) = (2, 8), (2, 10), (3, 8), and (3, 10) touched one side of the wall, turned, and moved towards the other side of the wall. On the other hand, some termites in the same-sized tunnels showed wall-following behavior without turning towards either side. We measured the termites' bending angle, θ, for termites exhibiting wall-touching behavior in the hole, and those demonstrating wall-following behavior (Table 4). The bending angle was measured between the rear end and the center of the mouth (Fig. 4). The results indirectly indicate that bending angles of 30–40° may break the wall-following behavior. When θ is less than ≈ 20°, termites showed wall-following behavior along a semi-circular path and then exited the holes. This critical angle may be related to the degree of curvature of termite tunnels. Curvatures sharper than the critical angle are likely to cause a decrease in movement by obstructing wall-following behavior.

In addition to these behaviors, we observed that when termites met a relatively large-sized hole, as in (W, D) = (2, 8), (2, 10), (3, 8), and (3, 10), they frequently reversed their movement direction (Fig. 5). In most cases, this behavior occurred when termites showed wall-following behavior along the semi-circular path (Fig. 5).

Although our study was conducted using a small-sized arena and a simple-shaped irregularity to represent soil pore space generated by biological or physical factors, the data provide valuable insights into behavioral mechanisms of increasing movement efficiency. Our results are particularly useful for designing future experiments that require construction of an individual-based model (IBM) for studying termite behavior in tunnels (Lee & Su 2010). The individual approach requires application of object-oriented (or agent-based) modeling, which is closely related to the modeling that utilizes the behavioral repertoire of interactions between individuals, and between individuals and environmental conditions. The behavioral repertoire determines individual actions. IBMs may include life cycles of individual termites, competitive interactions among species, and complex environmental system dynamics. An important behavioral rule for an IBM is likely to be the termites' behavioral response to an irregularity, such as a soil pore, in relation to movement efficiency. This study provides new directions for empirical investigations of termite foraging efficiency as it relates to the termites' behavioral responses to soil pore space.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This research was supported by the National Institute for Mathematical Sciences (NIMS), Republic of Korea.

REFERENCES CITED

1.

W. J. P. Bosma , S. E. A. T. M. Bellin, A, Van Der Zee , and A. Rinaldo 1993. Linear equilibrium adsorbing solute transport in physically and chemically heterogeneous porous formations. 2. Numerical results. Water Resour. Res. 29: 4031–3043. Google Scholar

2.

I. C. Burke , W. K. Lauenroth , R. Riggle , P. Brannen , B. Madigan , and S. Beard 1999. Spatial variability in soil properties in the short grass steppe: the relative importance of topography, grazing, microsite, and plant species in controlling spatial patterns. Ecosystems 2: 422–438. Google Scholar

3.

U. Diekmann , R. Law , and J. A. J. Metz 2000. The Geometry of Ecological Interactions. Cambridge University Press. Google Scholar

4.

G. Ettershank , J. A. Ettershank , and W. G. Whitford 1980. Location of food resources by subterranean termites. Environ. Entomol. 9: 645–648. Google Scholar

5.

R. H. Gardner , B. T. Milne , M. G. Turner , and R. V. O'neill 1987. Neutral models for the analysis of broad-scale landscape pattern. Landscape Ecol. 1: 19–28. Google Scholar

6.

J. C. Hedlung , and G. Henderson 1999. Effect of available food size on search tunnel formation by the Formosan subterranean termite (Isoptera: Rhinotermitidae). J. Econ. Entomol. 92: 610–16. Google Scholar

7.

X. P. Hu , A. G. Appel , and J. F. A. Traniello 2003. Behavioral response of two subterranean termites (Isoptera: Rhinotermitidae) to vibrational stimuli. J. Insect Behav. 16: 703–715. Google Scholar

8.

D. Jacques , H. Vereecken , C. Mouvet , C. Moreau , and J. Feyen 1999. Spatial correlation of atrazine sorption parameters and related physical-chemical soil properties. J. Containment Hydrol. 36: 31–52. Google Scholar

9.

W. Jeon , S. -Y. Kang , N. -Y. Su , and S.-H. Lee 2010. A constraint condition for foraging strategy in subterranean termites. J. Insect Sci. 10, Article 123. Google Scholar

10.

S. J. Ku , Su. N. -Y ., and S.-H. Lee 2010. Directional selection by subterranean termites, Coptotermes formosanus Shiraki (Isoptera: Rhinotermitidae), at tunnel intersection. J. Entomol. Sci. 13: 363–366. Google Scholar

11.

S. J. Ku , Su. N. -Y ., and S.-H. Lee 2012. Measurement of time taken by the Formosan termite, Coptotermes formosanus, to pass tunnel intersections. J. Insect Sci. 12, Article 23. Google Scholar

12.

S. C. Le Comber , B. Nicholls , D. K. Rossmo , and P. A. Racey 2006a. Geographic profiling and animal foraging. J. Theor. Biol. 240: 233–240. Google Scholar

13.

S. C. Le Comber , E. W. Seabloom , and S. S. Romanach 2006b. Burrow fractal dimension and foraging success in subterranean rodents: a simulation. Behav. Ecol. 17: 188–195. Google Scholar

14.

S.-H. Lee , P. Bardunias , and N.-Y. Su 2006. Food encounter rates of simulated termite tunnels with variable food size/distribution pattern and tunnel branch length. J. Theor. Biol. 243: 493–500. Google Scholar

15.

S.-H. Lee , N. -Y. Su , and P. Bardunias 2007a. Optimal length distribution of termite tunnel branches for efficient food search and resource transportation. Biosystems 90: 802–807. Google Scholar

16.

S.-H. Lee , N. -Y. Su , P. Bardunias , and H.-F. Lee 2007b. Food encounter rate of simulated termite tunnels in heterogeneous landscape. Biosystems, 90: 314–322. Google Scholar

17.

S.-H. Lee , P. Bardunias , and N.-Y. Su 2008a. Rounding a corner of a bent termite tunnel and tunnel traffic efficiency. Behav. Process. 77: 135–138. Google Scholar

18.

S.-H. Lee , P. Bardunias , N.-Y. Su , and R.-L. Yang 2008b. Behavioral response of termites to tunnel surface irregularity. Behav. Process. 78: 397–400. Google Scholar

19.

S.-H. Lee , and N.-Y. Su 2009. The influence of branching tunnels on subterranean termites' foraging efficiency: Consideration for simulations. Ecol. Inform. 4: 152–155. Google Scholar

20.

S.-H. Lee , and N.-Y. Su 2010. Simulation study on the tunnel networks of subterranean termites and the foraging behavior. J. Asia-Pacific Entomol. 13: 83–90. Google Scholar

21.

A. Okubo 1980. Diffusion and Ecological. Mathematical Models. Springer, New York. Google Scholar

22.

H. C. Park , and T. W. Bae 1997. Morphological Description of Reticulitermes speratus kyushuensis Morimoto (Isoptera:Rhinotermitidae) in Southern part of Korea. Kor. J. Soil Zool. 2: 59–64. Google Scholar

23.

S. K Robson , Lesniak , M. G ., R. V. Kothandapani , J. F. A. Traniello , B. L. Thorne , and V. Fourcassie 1995. Nonrandom search geometry in subterranean termites. Naturwissenschaften 82: 526–528. Google Scholar

24.

S. Sim , S. J. Ku , and S.-H. Lee 2012. Directional selection by termites at a branching node created by a ballpoint pen. J. Asia-Pacific Entomol. 15: 447–450. Google Scholar

25.

Sim, S., and Lee, S.-H. 2012a. Measurement of the time required for termites to pass through tunnels with different curvatures. J. Insect Sci. 12, Article 64. Google Scholar

26.

S. Sim , and S.-H. Lee 2012b. Measurement of the time required for termites to pass each other in tunnels of different curvatures. Insect Sci., DOI 10.1111/j.1744– 7917.2012.01517.x. Google Scholar

27.

M. F. Shlesinger , G. M. Zaslavsky , and U. Frisch 1995. Levy Flights and Related Topics in Physics. Springer, Berlin. Google Scholar

28.

N.-Y. Su , and H. Puche 2003. Tunneling activity of subterranean termites (Isoptera: Rhinotermitidae) in sand with moisture gradients. J. Econ. Entomol. 96: 88–93. Google Scholar

29.

N.-Y. Su , M. Tamashiro , J. R. Yates , and M. I. Haverty 1984. Foraging behavior of the Formosan subterranean termite (Isoptera: Rhinotermitidae). Environ. Entomol. 13: 1466–1470. Google Scholar

30.

M. Tamashiro , J. K Fujii , and P.-Y. Lai 1973. A simple method to observe, trap, prepare large numbers of subterranean termites for laboratory and field experiments. Environ. Entomol. 2: 721–722. Google Scholar

31.

T. Venna , and K. N. Garneshaiah 1991. Non random search pattern of ants foraging on honeydew of aphids in cashew inflorescences. Animal Behav. 41: 7–15. Google Scholar

32.

N. Wilson-Rich , R. J. Stuart , and R. B. Rosengaus 2007. Susceptibility and behavioral responses of the dampwood termite Zootermopsis angusticollis to the entomopathogenic nematode Steinernema carpocapsae. J. Invert. Pathol. 95: 17–25. Google Scholar

33.

K. A. With , 1997. The application of neutral landscape models in conservation biology. Conserv. Biol. 11: 1069–1080. Google Scholar

34.

H. Yuki , K. L Kosugi , and M. Takahisa 2006. Changes in pore size distribution and hydraulic properties of forest soil resulting from structural development. J. Hydro. 331: 85–102. Google Scholar

35.

X. Zuo , H. Zhao , X. Zhao , T. Zhang , Y. Guo , S. Wang , and S. Drake 2008. Spatial pattern and heterogeneity of soil properties in sand dunes under grazing and restoration in Horqin Sandy Land, Northern China. Soil and Tillage Res. 99: 202–212. Google Scholar
Sook Junk Ku, Nan-Yao Su, and Sang-Hee Lee "Movement Efficiency and Behavior of Termites (Isoptera) in Tunnels with Varying Pore Sizes," Florida Entomologist 96(3), 810-817, (1 September 2013). https://doi.org/10.1653/024.096.0314
Published: 1 September 2013
KEYWORDS
eficiencia de forrajeo
eficiencia del movimiento de termitas
foraging efficiency
irregularidad de la superficie del tónel
red de tóneles de termitas
Termite movement efficiency
termite tunnel network
Back to Top