Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 December 2015 The Ectoparasitoid Scleroderma guani (Hymenoptera: Bethylidae) Uses Innate and Learned Chemical Cues to Locate Its Host, Larvae of the Pine Sawyer Monochamus alternatus (Coleoptera: Cerambycidae)
Zhen Li, Baishu Li, Zhenjie Hu, J. P. Michaud, Jie Dong, Qingwen Zhang, Xiaoxia Liu
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

In Asia, the pine sawyer beetle Monochamus alternatus Hope (Coleoptera: Cerambycidae) is the most important vector of the pinewood nematode Bursaphelenchus xylophilus Steiner and Buhrer (Aphelenchida: Parasitaphelenchidae), the causal agent of pine wilt disease, and the ectoparasitoid Scleroderma guani Xiao et Wu (Hymenoptera: Bethylidae) is the most important natural enemy of this pest. Efficient host location is critical to parasitoid fitness, and chemical cues are key factors guiding the host searching process. This study was conducted to elucidate the odor cues guiding host location in S. guani and the impact of previous host experience on this behavior. Tests were conducted in which S. guani oriented to different odor resources associated with M. alternatus and its habitat, and components of these volatiles were analyzed with gas chromatography/mass spectrometry. Orientation to various odors also was investigated using adult S. guani subjected to different adult experiences. Female S. guani could easily distinguish M. alternatus larvae from other odor resources. Hydrocarbons were the main components of volatiles derived from M. alternatus larvae and were absent in volatiles from wood and frass associated with M. alternatus larvae. Female S. guani oriented most strongly to 1st and 3rd instars of M. alternatus. Prior experience of host odor without oviposition decreased orientation of S. guani towards the host, and the host used for rearing had no impact on subsequent odor orientation by adult S. guani. Volatile hydrocarbons emanating from host larvae might be the key to host location by adult S. guani, and experience with hosts appears to reinforce behavioral responses. The present results may be useful references for improving augmentative biological control using S. guani against M. alternatus and other forest pests.

Pine wilt disease (PWD) has caused devastating damage to pine forests worldwide, especially in Japan and China (Aikawa et al. 2006), and the combined timber losses and management costs have exceeded US$ 4 billion annually in China alone (Li et al. 2011). The pine wood nematode Bursaphelenchus xylophilus Steiner and Buhrer (Aphelenchida: Aphelenchoididae) is the pathogenic agent of PWD. Bursaphelenchus xylophilus has high pathogenicity and can cause the death of infected pines within several months (Han et al. 2007; Qiu et al. 2013). The pine sawyer beetle Monochamus alternatus Hope (Cerambycidae: Lamiinae) has been identified as the most important vector of the pine wood nematode in Asia (Ding et al. 2001), and management of M. alternatus is regarded as the key to control of PWD in China (Xu et al. 2008). Because M. alternatus larvae are solitary wood-boring insects living under pine bark, they are protected from contact pesticides and are very difficult to control (Wang & Yang 2008). Although the quick removal of all infected trees provides effective control of wood-boring insects, it is not a feasible tactic for M. alternatus-mediated, B. xylophilus- infected pines. Bursaphelenchus xylophilus can be transmitted to a dying tree, newly cut log, or living pine during feeding or oviposition by M. alternatus (Jones et al. 2008), and symptoms can be very difficult to detect in early stages. Once symptoms are evident, pine trees die soon thereafter (Jones et al. 2008). Thus, coevolved natural enemies that can effectively locate and parasitize the concealed larvae are among the best alternatives for sustainable long-term prevention of PWD.

The ectoparasitoid Scleroderma guani Xiao et Wu (Hymenoptera: Bethylidae) is indigenous to China and has been used widely for biological control of forest pests as it can parasitize the larvae and pupae of more than 50 species of wood-boring insects belonging to 22 families in 3 orders (Chen & Cheng 2000). This wasp has been considered the most effective biological control agent of M. alternatus and, consequently, B. xylophilus (Wang 2004; Xu et al. 2008). Parasitism of up to 81.3% of M. alternatus larvae has been reported in years of S. guani releases, and mortality of B. xylophilus-infected pines has been reduced by as much as 98.0% (Xu et al. 2002). Females of S. guani search for hosts by crawling quickly around tree trunks. Once they find the frass of M. alternatus on pine bark, they follow it to the host tunnel where they are able to locate M. alternatus larvae under bark in complete darkness (Yao & Yang 2008). Laboratory studies report that S. guani is an idiobiont (Lu et al. 2013) with a series of stereotypical host selection behaviors performed in a characteristic sequence (Zhang et al. 2004; Hu et al. 2012). Females of S. guani prefer to parasitize mid-instars; late instars have strong defensive behaviors and are difficult to parasitize, whereas early instars are vulnerable to mortality during parasitism (Ma et al. 2010). Because larvae of M. alternatus live under pine bark in dark galleries filled with sawdust and frass, the mechanism by which S. guani locates and selects suitable hosts warrants clarification.

Parasitoid wasps may use olfactory, visual, or acoustic signals to locate concealed hosts and parasitize them (Hou & Yan 1997; da Silva Torres et al. 2005; Wang & Yang 2008; Wölfling & Rostás 2009). Parasitoids evolve to focus on the most reliable of available cues for host location and may exhibit preferences for a particular host species or odor based on previous experience (van Alphen & Vet 1986; Vet & Dicke 1992; Papaj et al. 1994; Takasu & Lewis 2003; Fatouros et al. 2008; Li et al. 2009). In the present study, 3 main questions were addressed: 1) What is the most attractive chemical cue that guides S. guani females in host location? 2) Which instar of M. alternatus is the most attractive? 3) Does experience affect subsequent host location behavior by S. guani females? In order to answer these questions, S. guani females were permitted to choose between various habitat-derived odor sources and different instars of M. alternatus larvae, and the components of these odors were analyzed by gas chromatography/ mass spectrometry (GC/MS). Wasp odor preferences also were investigated in S. guani that had been reared on different hosts. It was reasoned that clarification of the kairomones used by S. guani in host location, and the role of learned cues, would improve our understanding of the tritrophic relationship among host pine, M. alternatus, and S. guani and potentially contribute to improved biological control of M. alternatus with S. guani.

Materials and Methods

INSECT PREPARATION

Larvae of M. alternatus (n ≥ 200) were collected as 2nd and 3rd instars from infested pine trees in Hangzhou City, Zhejiang Province, China, in Jul 2006. The larvae were isolated in plastic vials (5.0 cm height × 3.0 cm diameter), held in complete darkness under ambient temperature in our laboratory, and reared on an artificial diet (provided by the Chinese Academy of Forestry). Following adult emergence, beetles were transferred to cages and provided with fresh pine branches as described by Song et al. (2008). After 2 wk, 1st and 2nd instars of M. alternatus could be found under the bark of branches near oviposition scars. Mixtures of wood and frass were collected from their larval galleries and stored at -20 °C for subsequent analysis.

Adults of S. guani were obtained from a colony maintained on larvae of Saperda populnea (L.) (Coleoptera: Cerambycidae) on the Beijing Xishan Forest Farm. Pupae of the mealworm Tenebrio molitor L. (Coleoptera: Tenebrionidae) were used as a factitious host for rearing S. guani in all experiments. Mealworm larvae were purchased from the Beijing Guanyuanqiao pet market and reared on artificial diet (Zhang & Liu 2005) at 26.0 °C, a 16:8 h L:D photoperiod, and 70% RH until pupation. Pupae of T. molitor (100–160 mg) were held at 0.0 °C for 48 h before use in order to immobilize them for parasitism. Pupae were then placed into glass vials (10.0 cm height × 3.0 cm diameter) with mated females of S. guani in a parasitoid-to-pupa ratio of 2:1. Each vial contained a cotton ball with 10% honey solution and was plugged with a segment of cotton tampon. Parasitoids were removed after cocoons formed, and vials were held in a climate-controlled chamber under the same environmental conditions described above until adult parasitoid emergence.

Parasitoids were reared on larvae of M. alternatus by confining 3 mated S. guani females with 1 third instar (25–30 mm long) in a glass vial (5.5 cm height × 2.0 cm diameter) at 8.0 °C for 48 h. A 10% honey solution was provided on a ball of cotton, and the vials were plugged with cotton tampons. Insects were then kept in a climatic chamber at 27.0 °C, a 16:8 h L:D photoperiod, and 60% RH for adults collection. Once the 2nd generation of S. guani adults emerged, mated females were collected, kept in glass vials (5.5 cm height × 2.0 cm diameter) at 25 °C, a 16:8 h L:D photoperiod, and 60% RH, and supplied with 10% honey solution for 5 to 6 d to ensure oogenesis before use in experiments.

RESPONSES OF NAÏVE S. GUANI TO HOST-DERIVED ODORS

Mated female adults of S. guani (6 d old post-emergence) had no contact with any host (i.e., they were naïve) when they were tested in a glass Y-tube olfactometer modified according to de Kogel et al. (1999). Tests were carried out at 25.0 °C in a dark room with a fluorescent lamp hanging directly above the experimental apparatus. A black sheet was used to cover the whole apparatus to prevent access to other visual stimuli. All tests were performed between 9:00 and 10:30 a.m. or between 3:30 and 5:00 p.m., when the parasitoid is most active. Naïve S. guani females, 1 at a time, were released into the main arm and walked upwind to choose between 2 odor sources. A clock was started when each wasp entered the branch point of the main arm and direct observations were made for 5 min. Choices were recorded when a wasp walked more than 1.0 cm beyond the branch point and remained there for at least 10 s, or reached the end of the branch arm; otherwise no choice was recorded. Sixty repetitions were conducted for each set of odor choices, and each wasp was used only once. The Y-tube was reversed after each observation and replaced with a new tube after 10 observations. All tubes were cleaned with 95% ethanol before reuse. The following odor sources were tested: 10.0 mg of the wood diet used for feeding of M. alternatus (W), 10.0 mg of mixtures of wood diet and frass of M. alternatus (WF), 10.0 mg of sawdust produced by M. alternatus larval boring (S), 10 larvae of M. alternatus starved for 3 h (L), and a clean air control (A). WF and S were obtained from habitat occupied by 10 M. alternatus larvae for 1 wk.

ORIENTATION TO HOST INSTARS

First, third, and overwintering fifth instars of M. alternatus were all tested versus clean air with naïve, 6-d-old S. guani females that were isolated after emergence without any host or odor experience. The same protocol was used as described above for other odor sources.

VOLATILE COLLECTION AND ANALYSIS

Volatiles were collected separately from 10 M. alternatus 3rd instars that had been starved for 3 h and from 10.0 mg samples of the wood diet and frass mixture, using a dynamic headspace air collection device (QC-1, Beijing Municipal Institute of Labor Protection, Beijing, China) as in the study of Tang et al. (2012). Tenax-TA absorbent (150 mg) was connected to an oven bag (Wegmans Food Market, USA) where the volatiles were delivered. Air was delivered into the collection device after passage through activated charcoal and distilled water and through the bag containing odor sources. Volatile samples were collected for 24 h, at 25.0 °C in the dark with an airflow rate of 50 mL/min and subsequently analyzed using thermal-desorption cold-trap injector GC/MS (TCT-GC/MS) (CP-4010 PTI/TCT, Varian, Bergen op Zoom, Belgium; Thermo Finnigan Voyager GC/MS with Trace 2000 GC, ThermoQuest, United Kingdom). The TCT had a trap injection temperature of 200.0 °C, a trap temperature of -130.0 °C, a desorption temperature of 250.0 °C, and a desorption time of 10 min. A DB-5 Low Bleed Column (60 m × 0.32 nm × 0.5 µm) was used for GC/MS analysis, with a column temperature of 40.0 °C for 3 min increasing at 6.0 °C/min to 270.0 °C. Helium was used as carrier gas, the separator temperature was 280.0 °C, and the scan range of the MS monitor was 19 to 435 m/z for ion monitoring. The relative abundance and total ion current of MS for each sample were thus obtained. Compound identification was conducted by comparing the retention time of each component in the NIST 11 Mass Spectral Library (NIST/2011/EPA/NIH). The relative ratio of each compound in samples was calculated using the area normalization method.

EXPERIENCE AND ODOR SELECTION BY S. GUANI

Females of S. guani were subjected to various pre-trial experience treatments that included 1) no odor or oviposition experience (N); 2) contact with sawdust collected from the M. alternatus larval habitat (S); or 3) exposure to volatiles of M. alternatus larvae, but without oviposition (L). Naïve S. guani females (as described above) were held in glass vials (5.5 cm height × 2.0 cm diameter) sealed with fine mesh gauze and placed into cages together with 10 starved 3rd instars of M. alternatus (L) or 10.0 mg of sawdust (S) for 30 min. Wasps could not contact host larvae or sawdust samples directly but were exposed to only their odors. These wasps were used in olfactometer tests which offered M. alternatus larvae versus clean air as described above. Scleroderma guani colonies were reared separately either on pupae of T. molitor or larvae of M. alternatus for at least 5 generations and were then tested for effects of the natal host on odor responses. Females (n = 60) of S. guani from each colony were then offered a choice between the odor of 10 pupae of T. molitor (TP) versus 10 larvae of M. alternatus (ML).

STATISTICAL ANALYSES

Cochran's Q test was used to compare the relative preference of S. guani females for different odor sources, using SPSS Statistics 17.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, USA). Stacked vertical bars were constructed to depict differences in volatile components between different extracted samples with SigmaPlot 10.0 (Systat Software, Inc., California, USA).

Results

RESPONSES OF NAÏVE S. GUANI TO HOST-DERIVED ODORS

Naïve S. guani females expressed no preference between odor pairs that lacked M. alternatus larvae (Fig. 1): (W vs. WF, Q = 0.176, P = 0.674; S vs. A, Q = 0.209, P = 0.647; WF vs. A, Q = 0.020, P = 0.888; W vs. A, Q = 0.556, P = 0.456). However, host larvae were preferred to clean air (Q = 6.149, P = 0.013), to wood diet (Q = 21.333, P < 0.001) and to sawdust (Q = 5.255, P = 0.022, n = 55).

VOLATILE COMPONENTS OF ODOR SOURCES

Fourteen compounds were identified in volatiles derived from M. alternatus larvae (Fig. 2), including 11 different hydrocarbons (nonane 3.23%, decane 1.61%, limonene 9.08%, dodecane 5.44%, tetradecane 7.83%, dotriacontane 9.25%, ethylbenzene 2.70%, 1,3-dimethyl benzene 5.70%, biphenyl 7.97%, octane 1.58%, and hexadecane 11.39%), 2 alcohols (dimethyl hexadecanol 2.18% and borneol 16.02%), and 1 aldehyde (nonaldehyde 16.05%). Collectively, hydrocarbons were the main components and comprised 65.75% of the total. Six compounds were detected in volatiles from mixtures of wood diet and frass of M. alternatus larvae, including 2 alcohols (borneol 24.30% and 2,3-butanediol 35.53%), 3 organic acids (2-methylbutyric acid 4.13%, pentanoic acid 3.36%, and hexanoic acid 22.36%), and 1 ester (2-ethylhexyl acetate 10.33%), of which the alcohols comprised the largest proportion of the total (59.83%), followed by the acids (29.85%). Borneol was detected in both volatile samples but was present in higher proportion in the volatiles of wood and frass than in those of larvae.

Fig. 1.

Numbers of naïve Scleroderma guani females responding to various odor sources presented in pairs in a Y-tube olfactometer. W = wood diet, WF = mixture of wood diet and Monochamus alternatus frass, S = sawdust from M. alternatus galleries, L = 10 M. alternatus 3rd instars, and A = clean air. *: P ≤ 0.05, **: P ≤ 0.01. Numbers indicate numbers of wasps responding.

f01_1182.jpg

Fig. 2.

Relative proportions of organic compounds identified in headspace volatiles from Monochamus alternatus larvae and a mixture of wood diet, sawdust, and frass.

f02_1182.jpg

ORIENTATION TO HOST INSTARS

Females of S. guani showed no preference for the odor of overwintering M. alternatus 5th instars compared with clean air (Q = 0.021, P = 0.884), but did respond to odors of 1st instars (Q = 5.898, P = 0.015) and even more strongly to those of 3rd instars (Q = 11.520, P = 0.001; Fig. 3).

EXPERIENCE AND ODOR SELECTION BY S. GUANI

Female S. guani exposed to volatile chemicals from host larvae without opportunity to oviposit showed no significant orientation to M. alternatus larvae (Q = 1.923, P = 0.166), but those exposed to odors of sawdust did (Q = 5.255, P = 0.022). Naïve females did orient to M. alternatus larvae (Q = 11.520, P = 0.001), but the number of non-responding individuals was also highest in this test (Fig. 4).

Females of S. guani showed no preference for either host type whether they were reared on pupae of T. molitor pupae (TP, n = 48, Q = 0.750, P = 0.386) or on M. alternatus larvae (ML, n = 52, Q = 0.692, P = 0.405), although those reared on the factitious host were less responsive in the apparatus.

Fig. 3.

Numbers of naïve Scleroderma guani females responding to the odor of 10 Monochamus alternatus 1st, 3rd, or 5th instars (L) versus clean air (A) in a Y-tube olfactometer. *: P ≤ 0.05, **: P ≤ 0.01.

f03_1182.jpg

Discussion

Naïve S. guani females responded to odors of M. alternatus larvae, but not to other odor sources from the larval habitat. Neonate M. alternatus larvae feed on phloem in the endothelial layer of the pine bark, leaving coarse, powdery frass outside feeding locations. As larvae grow, they penetrate deeper into the xylem creating galleries full of sawdust and frass that are sometimes detectable from the bark surface (Tang et al. 2005). We infer that sawdust and frass on the surface of pine bark are important visual cues to searching S. guani females. Once in the host gallery, volatiles from sawdust and frass mix with those of M. alternatus larvae, but it is the latter that guide S. guani to its host.

Fig. 4.

Numbers of Scleroderma guani females responding to the odor of Monochamus alternatus 3rd instars (L) versus clean air (A) in a Y-tube olfactometer. Females were either naïve (N), previously exposed to gallery sawdust (S), or with previous exposure to the odor of M. alternatus larvae (L). *: P ≤ 0.05, **: P ≤ 0.01.

f04_1182.jpg

The hydrocarbons that appeared to be important cues in host location were associated with M. alternatus larvae but were absent from the volatiles of wood and frass. Similarly, linear and monomethyl- branched alkanes were the major components of residues left by larvae that were utilized in host location by the wasp Cotesia marginiventris Cresson (Hymenoptera: Braconidae) (Wölfling & Rostás 2009). Upon host contact, non-volatile hydrocarbons present in insect cuticles can be important in host recognition and elicitation of oviposition behavior (Vinson 1976; Morehead & Feener 1999). We infer that volatile hydrocarbons emanating from the body of M. alternatus larvae are probably responsible for guiding host location in S. guani females in the darkness of M. alternatus larval galleries.

Previous research demonstrated that S. guani prefers to parasitize M. alternatus larvae in mid-instars (Ma et al. 2010). In the present study, females of S. guani responded significantly to 1st and 3rd instars of M. alternatus, the latter appearing especially attractive, but did not respond to overwintering 5th instars. Early instars have a thin, soft cuticle and high metabolic activity associated with rapid growth, both of which probably facilitate high rates of volatile emission. In contrast, overwintering 5th instars have a much lower metabolic rate due to energy conservation requirements, and they possess a thick cuticle, both of which probably contribute to low levels of volatile emission and reduce attractiveness to S. guani.

After a period of coevolution, female parasitoids may evolve innate preferences for cues associated with their specific host or its microhabitat, while learning experiences continue to fine-tune responses to secondary cues (Menzel 1983; Vet & Groenewold 1990; Vinson 1998; Takasu & Lewis 2003; Giunti et al. 2015). Successful oviposition can serve to reinforce associative learning of secondary cues (Papaj & Vet 1990), whereas the lack of this reward can also change behavioral responses (Papaj 1990; Real 1991). In the present study, naïve S. guani females showed the strongest preference for host larvae in olfactometer trials, although they were also the most likely to not respond in tests. Females with sawdust experience also showed no orientation towards host larval odor, although fewer wasps failed to respond. In contrast, exposure to volatiles of host larvae alone did not cause females to respond to these odors in the olfactometer. Some previous studies have shown that parasitoids may prefer host species from which they emerged, although such preferences may be altered by learning experiences (Papaj & Vet 1990; Hoedjes et al. 2011; Giunti et al. 2015). Successful foraging or oviposition experiences may establish or enhance parasitoid responses to host-related cues, whereas an unrewarding experience may cause the reverse effect, as has been demonstrated in Sclerodermus pupariae Yang and Yao (Hymenoptera: Bethylidae) (Wei et al. 2013) and Leptopilina heterotoma (Thomson) (Hymenoptera: Figitidae) (Papaj et al. 1994).

Because S. guani females prefer early to mid-instars of M. alternatus, the best period for S. guani release will be from late summer to autumn, when these particular instars predominate. Later releases could be compensated by increasing the number of females released, as more parasitoids will be needed to obtain the same level of parasitism in an older, larger cohort of M. alternatus larvae. Because rearing S. guani on T. molitor pupae had no impact on subsequent responses to host odors, this species is suitable as a factitious host for mass production of the parasitoid, given its ready commercial availability and low cost. Further analysis of the activity of volatile hydrocarbons emanating from M. alternatus larvae using electrophysiological techniques such as simultaneous GC-electroantennogram observations could further elucidate the specific compounds guiding the host location process in S. guani.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the National Pear Industry System (No. CARS-29-08).

References Cited

1.

T Aikawa , T Kikuchi , H Kosaka. 2006. Population structure of Bursaphelenchus xylophilus within single Pinus thunbergii trees inoculated with two nematode isolates. Forest Pathology 36: 1–13. Google Scholar

2.

J Chen , H Cheng. 2000. Advances in applied research on Scleroderma spp. Chinese Journal of Biological Control 16: 166–170. (In Chinese) Google Scholar

3.

CSA da Silva Torres , RW Matthews , JR Ruberson , WJ Lewis. 2005. Role of chemical cues and natal rearing effect on host recognition by the parasitic wasp Melittobia digitata. Entomological Science 8: 355–362. Google Scholar

4.

WJ de Kogel , EH Koschier , JH Visser. 1999. Y-tube olfactometer to determine the attractiveness of plant volatiles to western flower thrips. Proceedings of the Section Experimental and Applied Entomology of the Netherlands Entomological Society 10: 131–135. Google Scholar

5.

Y Ding , C Lu , B Han , H Pu , M Wu. 2001. Relationship among growth potential of pine, population density of Monochamus alternatus, and pathogenicity of Bursaphelenchus xylophilus. Chinese Journal of Applied Ecology 12: 351–354. (In Chinese) Google Scholar

6.

NE Fatouros , M Dicke , R Mumm , T Meiners , M Hilker. 2008. Foraging behavior of egg parasitoids exploiting chemical information. Behavioral Ecology 19: 677–689. Google Scholar

7.

G Giunti , C Canale , RH Messing , E Donati , C Stefanini , JP Michaud , G Benelli . 2015. Parasitoid learning: current knowledge and implications for biological control. Biological Control: in press. DOI:10.1016/j.biocontrol.2015.06.007. Google Scholar

8.

B Han , C Piao , L Wang , Y Li. 2007. Development status of pinewood nematode disease and its management strategies in China. Chinese Agricultural Science Bulletin 23: 146–150. (In Chinese) Google Scholar

9.

KM Hoedjes , HM Kruidhof , ME Huigens , M Dicke , LEM Vet , HM Smid. 2011. Natural variation in learning rate and memory dynamics in parasitoid wasps: opportunities for converging ecology and neuroscience. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 278: 889–897. Google Scholar

10.

Z Hou , F Yan. 1997. Progress in the study on host selection behaviors of parasitic wasps. Acta Entomologica Sinica 40: 94–107. (In Chinese) Google Scholar

11.

Z Hu , X Zhao , Y Li , X Liu , Q Zhang . 2012. Maternal care in the parasitoid Sclerodermus harmandi (Hymenoptera: Bethylidae). PLoS One 7: e51246. Google Scholar

12.

JT Jones , M Moens , M Mota , H Li , T Kikuchi. 2008. Bursaphelenchus xylophilus: opportunities in comparative genomics and molecular host-parasite interactions. Molecular Plant Pathology 9: 357–368. Google Scholar

13.

L Li , DR Miller , J Sun. 2009. The influence of prior experience on preference and performance of a cryptoparasitoid Scleroderma guani (Hymenoptera: Bethylidae) on beetle hosts. Ecological Entomology 34: 725–734. Google Scholar

14.

Z Li , X Liu , Y Chu , Y Wang , Q Zhang , X Zhou. 2011. Cloning and characterization of a 2-Cys peroxiredoxin in the pine wood nematode, Bursaphelenchus xylophilus, a putative genetic factor facilitating the infestation. International Journal of Biological Sciences 7: 823–836. Google Scholar

15.

H Lu , D Cai , J Fu , X Ma , T Yang , H Liu. 2013. Research advance on Scleroderma guani to control Monochamus alternatus. Modern Agricultural Science and Technology 9: 117–120. (In Chinese) Google Scholar

16.

L Ma , Y Zhu , C Cao , J Wen , Z Xu , D Xiong , W Tao. 2010. Utilization the Pyemotes sp. and Scleroderma guani Xiao et Wu to control the larvae of Semanotus bifasciatus. Forest Research 23: 313–316. Google Scholar

17.

R Menzel. 1983. Neurobiology of learning and memory: the honeybee as a model system. Naturwissenschaften 70: 504–511. Google Scholar

18.

SA Morehead , DH Feener Jr. 2000. Visual and chemical cues used in host location and acceptance by a dipteran parasitoid. Journal of Insect Behavior 13: 613–625. Google Scholar

19.

DR Papaj. 1990. Interference with learning in pipevine swallowtail butterflies: behavioral constraint or possible adaptation? Symposia Biological Hungarica 39: 89–101. Google Scholar

20.

DR Papaj , LEM Vet. 1990. Odor learning and foraging success in the parasitoid, Leptopilina heterotoma. Journal of Chemical Ecology 16: 3137–3150. Google Scholar

21.

DR Papaj , H Snellen , K Swaans , LEM Vet. 1994. Unrewarding experiences and their effect on foraging in the parasitic wasp Leptopilina heterotoma (Hymenoptera: Eucoilidae). Journal of Insect Behavior 7: 465–481. Google Scholar

22.

X Qiu , X Wu , L Huang , M Tian , J Ye . 2013. Specifically expressed genes of the nematode Bursaphelenchus xylophilus involved with early interactions with pine trees. PLoS One 8: e78063. Google Scholar

23.

L Real. 1991. Animal choice behavior and the evolution of cognitive architecture. Science 253: 980–986. Google Scholar

24.

L Song , X Liu , Y Zhang , Q Zhang , Z Zhao. 2008. The cloning and expression of α-tubulin in Monochamus alternatus. Insect Molecular Biology 17: 495–504. Google Scholar

25.

K Takasu , WJ Lewis. 2003. Learning of host searching cues by the larval parasitoid Microplitis croceipes. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 108: 77–86. Google Scholar

26.

C Tang , J Huang , Q Chen , H Song , W Kang , X He. 2005 Biological characteristics of Monochamus alternatus II: life habit. Entomological Journal of East China 14: 209–213. (In Chinese) Google Scholar

27.

R Tang , J Zhang , Z Zhang . 2012. Electrophysiological and behavioral responses of male fall webworm moths (Hyphantria cunea) to herbivory-induced mulberry (Morus alba) leaf volatiles. PLoS One 7: e49256. Google Scholar

28.

JJM van Alphen , LEM Vet. 1986. An evolutionary approach to host finding and selection, pp. 23–61 In JK Waage , D Greathead [eds.], Insect Parasitoids. Academic Press, London, United Kingdom. Google Scholar

29.

LEM Vet , M Dicke. 1992 Ecology of infochemical use by natural enemies in a tritrophic context. Annual Review of Entomology 37: 141–172. Google Scholar

30.

LEM Vet , AW Groenewold. 1990. Semiochemicals and learning in parasitoids. Journal of Chemical Ecology 16: 3119–3135. Google Scholar

31.

SB Vinson. 1976. Host selection by insect parasitoids. Annual Review of Entomology 21: 109–133. Google Scholar

32.

SB Vinson. 1998. The general host selection behavior of parasitoid Hymenoptera and a comparison of initial strategies utilized by larvaphagous and oophagous species. Biological Control 11: 79–96. Google Scholar

33.

L Wang. 2004. Study on the biological characteristic of Monochamus alternatus Hope. Journal of Fujian Forestry Science and Technology 31: 23–26. (In Chinese) Google Scholar

34.

X Wang , Z Yang. 2008. Behavioral mechanisms of parasitic wasps for searching concealed insect hosts. Acta Ecologica Sinica 28: 1257–1269. Google Scholar

35.

K Wei , Y Tang , X Wang , Z Yang , L Cao , J Lu , E Liu , G Liu. 2013. Effects of learning experience on behavior of the generalist parasitoid Sclerodermus pupariae to novel hosts. Journal of Applied Entomology 137: 469–475. Google Scholar

36.

M Wölfling , M Rostás. 2009. Parasitoids use chemical footprints to track down caterpillars. Communicative and Integrative Biology 2: 353–355. Google Scholar

37.

F Xu , K Xu , C Xie , P Zhang , S Shin , Y Cheong. 2008. Studies on Scleroderma guani to control the pine sawyer beetle, Monochamus alternatus , pp. 379–388 In MM Mota , P Vieira [eds.], Pine Wilt Disease: A Worldwide Threat to Forest Ecosystems. Springer, Dordrecht, The Netherlands. Google Scholar

38.

K Xu , F Xu , M Wang , J Zhao , Q Jiang , P Zhang , D Xu , R He , X Jiang. 2002. The techniques of Scleroderma guani Xiao et Wu to control pine sawyer beetles. Journal of Nanjing University 26: 48–52. (In Chinese) Google Scholar

39.

W Yao , Z Yang. 2008. Studies on biological control of Anolophora blabripennis (Coleoptera: Cerambycidae) with a parasitoid, Sclerodermus guani (Hymenoptera: Bethylidae). Journal of Environmental Entomology 30: 127–134. (In Chinese) Google Scholar

40.

S Zhang , G Liu. 2005. Study on artificial diet of yellow mealworm (Tenebrio molitor L.). Jianggxi Plant Protection 28: 154–157. (In Chinese) Google Scholar

41.

W Zhang , X Sun , A Qu , Y Liu. 2004. The oviposition behavior of Scleroderma guani Xiao et Wu. Natural Enemies of Insects 26: 28–33. Google Scholar
Zhen Li, Baishu Li, Zhenjie Hu, J. P. Michaud, Jie Dong, Qingwen Zhang, and Xiaoxia Liu "The Ectoparasitoid Scleroderma guani (Hymenoptera: Bethylidae) Uses Innate and Learned Chemical Cues to Locate Its Host, Larvae of the Pine Sawyer Monochamus alternatus (Coleoptera: Cerambycidae)," Florida Entomologist 98(4), 1182-1187, (1 December 2015). https://doi.org/10.1653/024.098.0425
Published: 1 December 2015
KEYWORDS
biological control
Control biológico
experience
experiencia
host location
hydrocarbones volatiles
localizacion de huesped
Back to Top