Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 October 2015 Pharmacological Influences on the Neolithic Transition
Greg Wadley, Brian Hayden
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

While uncertainty remains as to the relative importance of the factors that propelled Neolithization at different sites, a model is gaining traction that proposes that cereal cultivation was adopted in part to produce alcohol for competitive feasting. The model ties together the emergence of two key phenomena – cereal cultivation and social inequality – and is supported by a variety of archaeological and ethnographic data. However pharmacological theory has not yet been explicitly deployed in the presentation of the model; rather its development has relied on a common-sense understanding of the effects of alcohol and its cross-cultural importance in social life. Our aim in this paper is to bring understandings of drug use from pharmacology and related disciplines to bear upon the challenge of explaining Neolithization. We find that pharmacological theory sheds light on the importance of alcohol and other mood-altering products of Neolithic farming. In particular there is support for some influence of pharmacoactivity on Neolithic social evolution, which might extend beyond a role in feasting to include modulation of responses to status hierarchies, increased residential densities, and more intense work schedules. We propose that pharmacological influences be incorporated into models of the Neolithic transition.

Introduction

The change from a long-standing hunter-gatherer lifestyle to one based on farming and sedentary life in large, hierarchical societies began at a handful of primary sites and spread via movements of people, species, and ideas until it transformed much of the human world. While new data continue to shed light on how and when Neolithization occurred, uncertainty remains about why people chose to undergo this major change in subsistence and social behaviors. Evidence that early cereal farming did not necessarily lead to improved nutrition and population health (Bowles 2011; Cohen 1989) led many researchers to reject the “pull factor” of “a highly desirable and welcome invention providing security and leisure time for prehistoric peoples” (Price and Bar-Yosef 2011:S166) in favor of “demand-, need- or push-based” motives (Cohen 2009:591) such as climate deterioration or population pressure.

A hypothesis that is attracting increasing interest proposes that Epipaleolithic populations exploited and then cultivated cereals, not primarily for food but to brew alcohol for use in competitive feasting. An early partial version of this hypothesis was proposed by J. D. Sauer (Braidwood et al. 1953) and later elaborated by Katz and Voigt (1986). Recently Hayden and colleagues (2012) provided archaeological evidence for this hypothesis, as one component of Hayden’s (1990) “competitive feasting” model. In this view, aggrandizing individuals used alcohol to attract people to feasts and then to manipulate them to acquire political power via reciprocal feasting debts. This “alcohol model” when combined with feasting models explicitly addresses the co-occurrence of two key Neolithic phenomena – cereal agriculture and social inequality – and is supported by a range of archaeological and ethnographic data (Hayden et al. 2012). However, pharmacological theory has not yet been explicitly deployed as part of the model; rather it has relied upon a common-sense understanding of the effects of alcohol and its role in social life. The purpose of this paper is to use insights from pharmacology and related disciplines to explore, in more depth, the role of alcohol and other pharmacologically active comestibles in Neolithization such as coca, poppy, and tobacco. In our view, such crops probably constituted one of the important components of early aggrandizer “toolkits” for creating differential power. Recognizing a pharmacological role in Neolithization helps to explain the choice of these species for cultivation and contributes to a more nuanced understanding of the dynamics of Neolithization.

The Neolithic Transition

The earliest known Neolithic transition and one of the best studied occurred in the Pre-Pottery Neolithic (PPN) in the southern Levant (Kuijt and Goring-Morris 2002). Climate change during the Epipaleolithic may have led to the appearance of large local stands of wild cereals, possibly cultivated by Late Epipaleolithic (Natufian) bands (c. 14,500-11,700 BP) and PPNA communities (11,700 to 10,500 BP) without creating genetic changes (Willcox et al. 2008). During the subsequent PPNB (10,500 to 7800 BP) this area saw the morphologically manifested domestication of cereals, sheep and goats, the concentration of populations into permanent villages that grew dramatically in size, and a reorganization of political and economic structures and processes (Kuijt and Goring-Morris 2002). While particular circumstances and species varied, broadly similar processes occurred at many sites globaly (Cohen 2011).

Neolithization involved not only new subsistence practices, but also a change in the size and structure of human groups. While simple mobile band societies probably consisted of 15-40 individuals living in egalitarian relationships in which food and materials were generally shared, by 12,000 BP some Natufian hunter-gatherer communities had become sedentary or semi-sedentary and were comprised of up to 200-300 people who recognized private property, held wealth, and exhibited socioeconomic differences (Hayden 2004, 2011a; Henry 1989:214; Perrot 1966:477). Subsequent PPNA villages such as Jericho had up to 200 inhabitants or more living close together (Goring-Morris and Belfer-Cohen 2011). Some settlements in the PPNB reached town proportions covering areas up to ten hectares (Twiss 2008) with populations at least in the hundreds and probably much higher (Goring Morris and Belfer-Cohen 2011). However, there were a number of negative effects that accompanied reliance on domesticated plants. Populations increased in spite of the likely detrimental effects of new diets (Cohen 1989). Greater densities must have created scalar stresses (Friesen 1999) and fomented greater competition between and within sites, evidenced by an increase in violence in the PPNB (Belfer-Cohen and Goring-Morris 2011).

Hayden and colleagues (2012) emphasized the risks involved in the long delay between agricultural effort and payoff, such as drought, pests, and theft. Cultivation is a delayed-return investment (Winterhalder and Kennett 2009) whose potential nutritional payoff should be discounted because unforeseen events might reduce its future availability (Cohen 2009; Tucker 2006). Future-discounting is a rational and probably universal practice (Green and Myerson 2004) and it has been suggested that a key difference between forager and farmer lifeways concerns the degree to which each accepts, and is willing to discount, delayed returns (Woodburn 1988).

The most common explanations for the social changes emerging from domestication involve chains of causality in which environmental and population pressures led to crises that forced people to adopt cultivation and participate in complex societies, which then enabled specialization and technological advances, empowering these groups to dominate others (Cohen 1975; Rosenberg 1990). However, the high risks, lower returns, and nutritional disadvantages of cultivation do not seem to support exclusively subsistence-oriented explanations of domestication. Moreover, a link between population pressure and domestication or cultivation is far from certain (Hayden 2000, 2014). The assertion that sedentism produces complex societies is also problematic, since some hunter-gatherers became sedentary without developing social complexity (Kelly 1992), and conversely, there are highly mobile societies, such as pastoral nomads, that have developed chiefdoms and even empires.

Thus we suggest that other factors should be considered which may help explain changes in both sustenance strategies and social structures. Hayden (1990, 2009, 2011b, 2014), for example, has argued from ethnographic and archaeological evidence that cultivation was motivated by the advantages that competitive feasting provides in building political power and pursuing self-interests. Feasting and political gift economies are common among ethnographically described complex human societies including complex hunter-gatherer but not those employing a simple foraging lifestyle characterized by sharing and egalitarianism. Partly through the agency of psychoactive substances, feasts in traditional societies are, and presumably were, used as arenas for inculcating ideologies, creating cohesion and social differentiation within a social group, and introducing new foods and technologies (Dietler 2001; Hayden 2014; Twiss 2008). Feasts require the production and storage ahead of time of large quantities of food and drink, and successful organizers can and do obtain political power and reproductive success (Hayden 2009, 2014). The social competition model proposes that a wealth rather than a dearth of resources enabled people to engage in high risk production activities such as cultivation and domestication. In this scenario, a failure of food production for feasts could not affect basic subsistence, but success would provide important sociopolitical advantages. Neolithic domesticates are proposed, in part, to have been used as costly status symbols, helping to bring an end to egalitarianism largely via feasting.

Since consumption of alcohol is a primary component of Neolithic feasting (Twiss 2008), one might expect that producing alcohol for feasts could have been a significant motivator of attempts first at harvesting, and eventually, at cultivating cereals. Noting the possible cultural significance of cereals for making alcohol in the Epipaleolithic, Katz and Voigt (1986:28) suggested that “any disruption in the supply of these wild foods would have posited a serious problem” which could have been resolved by cultivating cereals. Smalley and Blake (2003) made a similar point regarding the domestication of maize in Neolithic America, arguing that, initially, the primary use was to produce alcohol, the importance of which drove the rapid diffusion of this cereal. Hayden and colleagues have recently presented evidence that Levantine and East Asian pre-farming and farming groups were producing alcohol (Hayden 2011a; Hayden et al. 2012). These authors describe ancient brewing techniques in detail and propose a method for brewing given the available technology and testing it experimentally.

In order to flesh out these models in a broader pharmacological context, we first review the production of alcohol and other psychoactive domesticates by Neolithic farmers with a focus on their behavioral effects. We do not discount the nutritional aspects of agriculture, but acknowledge and attempt to explain the high representation of psychoactive plants among early crops.

Psychoactive Crops

A psychoactive substance influences thought or emotion through biochemical action upon the nervous system. When exogenous they are commonly referred to as “drugs,” but as Sherratt (1995a) has argued, this term carries controversial and variable cultural meanings and a morally neutral term is preferred. In pre-Industrial times, drugs were usually ingested as part of a food or drink, so that effects on mood could be confused with nutritional benefits (Hagen et al. 2013); thus Jankowiak and Bradburd (1996) used the term “drug foods.” However, not all drugs are consumed by an oral route, and we have followed Sherratt in using the term “psychoactive substances,” abbreviating it to PAS.

The use of PAS precedes agriculture and was widespread in forager societies (e.g., Saniotis 2010:481; Thomas 2003). Many PAS were exploited for medicinal purposes (Guthrie and Ho-Yen 2011) or were mild stimulants, including tobacco in America and Australia, and khat and betel in Africa and Asia (all these plants were eventually cultivated by some groups). Much forager PAS use focused on hallucinogens for ritual purposes, such as to induce shamanic trances and communicate with the spirit world (La Barre 1970; Merlin 2003:295; Van Pool 2009). Most hallucinogens are debilitating in high doses, and their powerful effects deter widespread consumption, restricting use largely to infrequent rituals by a few specialists. With domestication, however, the focus shifted from perception-altering to mood-altering, euphoric, or stimulating PAS (Merlin 2003). Domestication enabled the production of large and reliable quantities of such PAS (Saniotis 2010; Sullivan and Hagen 2002). In the primary Neolithic sites, West and East Asian farmers produced alcohol, while American farmers produced alcohol, coca, tobacco, and cacao (see Table 1). European and Asian farmers added opium and cannabis to the Levantine crop complex; in Southern India farmers produced grains for alcohol; in Africa, coffee and kola were major trade items. Sherrat (1995a) and other authors have illustrated the global importance of PAS to farming societies. Table 1 lists the major Neolithic regions and the PAS produced there.

Table 1.

PAS production by early farmers

i0278-0771-35-3-566-t01.tif

Mood-altering PAS stimulate brain reward pathways (Blum et al. 2012). They are highly prized and sought for effects such as amicability, reduction of stress, and feelings of liberation. They are widely used in many cultures, and they have been major trade goods throughout history and prehistory. Psychoactive substances were not the only products of early cultivators, but they were typically the most highly valued and were given religious and social significance. Even today, the majority of adult humans regularly use PAS derived from early domesticates including alcohol, nicotine, caffeine, chocolate, and sugars (Anderson 2006). Ancient users may not have perceived these as drugs in the modern sense but simply as desirable, good tasting, good feeling foods (McGovern 2009; Sullivan and Hagen 2002). In early modern times, PAS played a facilitating role in global colonization, used first to entice indigenous peoples into labor arrangements, and then to reward individuals for labor and production outputs (Courtwright 2009). The effects of PAS upon mood and motivation are critical: “Habitual users tend to develop psychological or physiological dependency on them and, in turn, on the trader or merchant who provides them,” (Jankowiak and Bradburd 1996).

Alcohol

Ethyl alcohol was probably first produced by West Asian Epipaleolithic cereal foragers (Hayden et al. 2012) as well as by complex hunter-gatherers and early farmers in China 9000 years ago (McGovern et al. 2004), in Central America 6,000 years ago (Smalley and Blake 2003), in Incan South America (Staller 2009), and in Neolithic India where the purpose of growing wheat and barley in some areas seems to have been to produce alcohol (Fuller 2005). Alcohol has had an enormous impact on social and religious practices around the world (Sherrat 1995b). It played a major role in public and private life in most of the major early civilizations, where it was consumed at religious, political and social events (Jennings et al. 2005).

Nicotine

Tobacco was gathered and cultivated in America as early as 4000 years ago (Rafferty 2006) and was used for religious and social rituals, as medicine, and for trade and recreation (Groark 2010; Wilbert 1979). While tobacco was probably originally used exclusively by shamans, with cultivation it became more democratized (von Gernet 1995). After domestication in the Andes (Pickersgill 2007), tobacco spread to Central America (Groark 2010) and then into North America as a domesticate along with maize at least two thousand years ago (Rafferty 2006).

Caffeine and theobromine

Caffeine is a psychostimulant and antidepressent (Pechlivanova et al. 2012) and is currently the world’s most widely used PAS (Weinberg 2000). Coffee was first exploited in East Africa at least 1500 years ago (Anthony et al. 2002), kola in West Africa at least 500 years ago (Lovejoy 1980), and tea in China at least 2,000 years ago (Li 1983); all were socially important and were major drivers of agriculture and trade.

A drink of fermented cacao (which contains not only caffeine but the pharmacologically similar theobromine) was in use over 2000 years ago in Central America (Henderson at al. 2007; Powis et al. 2002) and may have been the key trade product that enabled the Olmec to create expansive early complex polities in Mesoamerica. Subsequently, chocolate was used in social and ritual settings by the Mayans and served as a currency and elite drink for the Aztecs.

Cocaine

There is evidence for coca use as early as 8000 years ago in northern Peru, so it was “a feature of the rise of agriculture and social complexity in the region” (Dillehay et al. 2010). By the time of the Incan civilization, coca was a marker of social status and was accorded religious significance (Plowman 1984).

Cannabis

Cannabis was one of the earliest PAS used in India and was part of the early Chinese Neolithic crop complex (Merlin 2003). It was also one of the few domesticates in Early Jomon Japan (Crawford 1983:23-25). Charred seeds found in pots throughout Eastern Europe indicate that cultivation spread there soon after initial domestication (Rudgley 1995).

Opiates

The opium poppy is native to Mediterranean Europe and was probably the first plant added to the crop complex imported from the Levant (Zapata 2004). Poppies, cereals, and pulses formed the plant assemblages of the first farmers in Italy (Merlin 2003), and opioids have been found in the bones of Neolithic miners as well as at burial sites in Iberia (Zapata 2004).

Sucrose

Humans eat sweet, fatty, energy-dense foods in response to depression, stress, and negative affect (Gibson 2012). These “comfort foods” (Dube et al. 2005) induce “a psychologically comfortable and pleasurable state for a person,” (Wansink et al. 2003). Sucrose acts as an analgesic (Blass et al. 1987), reduces stress (Ulrich-Lai et al. 2010), and reduces future discounting (Wang and Dvorak 2010), with repeated consumption leading to tolerance and withdrawal effects (Johnson and Kenny 2010).

Sugar cane appears to have been first used at least 6000 years ago in New Guinea (Daniels and Daniels 1993; Denham 2011), and its cultivation and use later spread throughout Asia (Galloway 2005) into Europe, where it was a major driver of European colonization and trade (Grivet et al. 2004).

Dietary peptides

Both the neurotransmitter serotonin and its precursor tryptophan influence mood (Young 2007). Kerem et al. (2007) showed that chickpea, a Levantine founder crop, contains a high level of tryptophan which increased with domestication, suggesting that early farmers selected and were able to discriminate high tryptophan varieties. Kerem et al. (2007) argued that mood effects motivated cultivation and may have become important in early agricultural communities.

Opioid peptides called exorphins are produced when cereals and milk are digested (Zioudrou et al. 1979), and it has been proposed that this contributed to the attractiveness of some domesticates (Wadley and Martin 1993). Most pharmacological evidence for their effects comes from animal models where exorphins raise pain thresholds (Gritsai et al. 2009), reduce anxiety (Belyaeva et al. 2008), and interfere with satiety signaling leading to increased food intake (Duraffourd et al. 2012). Their function in humans is less understood, though current research is exploring a role in mental health conditions (e.g., Sokolov et al. 2014).

Summary

The extensive early production of psychoactive substances has been widely discussed in anthropological and archaeological literature, yet pharmacological research has rarely been drawn upon for clues as to how PAS might have influenced ancient users. We believe that a comprehensive archaeological explanation of PAS cultivation and domestication should include discussion of the psychosocial roles of these substances, a topic addressed next.

Understanding Psychoactive Substance Use

Psychoactive substances shape behavior by influencing mood and motivation (Nesse and Berridge 1997), future-orientation (Giordano et al. 2002), and social behavior (Velea and Hautefeuille 2007). While early pharmacological research focused on mechanisms underlying compulsive use of psychoactives, recent work has offered a more nuanced understanding, with researchers arguing that much PAS use is not the result of addiction per se, but that it serves instrumental purposes such as coping with stress or fatigue (Morgan et al. 2013), alleviating psychological distress (Pickard 2012), and achieving work or social goals (Muller and Schumann 2011). Benefits include feeling part of a social group, enjoyment, creativity, relief of pain and anxiety, relaxation, control of waking and sleep cycles, improved memory and concentration (Morgan et al. 2013), and, in some cases, maintaining neurotransmitter levels (Hagen et al. 2013). This section reviews current understandings of why PAS are used and how they influence behavior.

Motivation and Emotion

PAS activate brain pathways that regulate appetitive anticipation and consummatory hedonia (satisfaction upon attainment) (Berridge and Robinson 1998; Blum et al. 2012). From a Darwinian perspective, they provide artificial subjective rewards that create “a signal in the brain that indicates, falsely, the arrival of a huge fitness benefit” (Nesse and Berridge 1997). Nevertheless, PAS lead the user to attribute importance to settings and events associated with PAS (Lende and Smith 2010), and to crave repeated use (Robinson and Berridge 1993).

Several avenues of research show that PAS influence individuals’ perceptions of, and abilities to deal with, social situations. Pro-social behavior is facilitated through reduction of anxiety, enhancement of self-confidence, suppression of fatigue, and disinhibition of communication (Muller and Schumann 2011:298). Nicotine (Todd 2004), alcohol (Cooper et al. 2002), and other PAS (Sinha 2001) counter social stress, while alcohol is used for mood enhancement, cognitive performance, and to reduce symptoms of depression (Muller and Schumann 2011).

Empathogens such as MDMA increase sociability and make identification of social threat less accurate (Bedi et al. 2010). Many feasting foods such as alcohol, kava, betel, nicotine, and fatty meat have the same effect (Dietler 2001; Hayden 2014). Enjoyment of social interaction is enhanced by exogenous opioids (Trezza et al. 2011), and alcohol is used to remedy an inability to interact socially (Martinotti et al. 2009).

Psychoactive substances can reduce the subjective stress of living in groups, so that “social constraints become bearable and the individual feels independent while remaining within society” (Velea and Hautefeuille 2007). People and animals become stressed if they feel crowded (Armario et al. 2008; Calhoun 1973; Epstein 1981). Crowding stress is mediated by serotonin (Daniels et al. 2000) and attenuated by anti-depressants (e.g., Naitoh et al. 1992), alcohol (Nagaraja and Jeganathan 2003), opiates (Xigeng et al. 2004), and high-caloric food (Coccurello et al. 2009).

Artificial rewards are particularly attractive to those who have fewer opportunities to receive natural rewards (Nesse 2002) that lead to increased fitness, e.g., people with low socio-economic status (Anderson 2006). Social defeat precipitates use of PAS by animals (Bardo et al. 2013:276-280), including cocaine (Haney et al. 1995) and alcohol (Hilakivi-Clarke and Lister 1992; Wolffgramm and Heyne 1991), and heavy use in humans is likely due to similar situations. In addition, comfort foods are used to address effort-reward imbalance (Kouvonen et al. 2005b) that arises when physical or mental effort is not commensurate with pay and social rewards (Siegrist 1996).

Delay of Gratification and Willingness to Work

Future-discounting, or impulsiveness, refers to an individual’s preference to receive immediate return or instant gratification rather than to apportion effort now for future payoff. Future-discounting is not irrational but represents recognition of the risk that a delayed payoff may not materialize to the extent desired or even at all; thus all individuals future-discount to varying extents (Green and Myerson 2004).

Future-discounting is influenced by PAS in a complex fashion. While some users are more impulsive than non-users (Bickel and Marsh 2001), impulsiveness may both be a cause and a consequence of PAS use (Stein and Madden 2012). In addition, PAS use can increase an individual’s ability to delay gratification by offering an immediate false reward substitute for a delayed real fitness reward. PAS can reduce impulsiveness by inducing a feeling of power over the delivery of reward (Soubrie 1986). For example, opioid users who are currently sated discount less than do those currently deprived (Giordano et al. 2002), and individuals who have recently consumed sucrose-laden drinks are more likely to accept delayed rewards (Wang and Dvorak 2010).

PAS have been used throughout history to inhibit work-related stress, to overcome muscle and mental fatigue, to adapt to work environments, and to enhance work performance (Courtwright 2009; Dube et al. 2005; Grunberg et al. 1999; Jankowiak and Bradburd 1996; Kivimäki et al. 2006; Kouvonen et al. 2005a; Muller and Schumann 2011). Tellingly, Muller and Schumann (2011) consider high workloads in industrial societies to be a major driver of contemporary PAS use.

Summary

Psychoactive substance use does not represent merely a problematic behavior indulged in by a minority; rather it is an important and routine shaper of behavior for the vast majority of post-Neolithic humans. Is this mere coincidence, or is there a causal link between widespread adoption of mood altering PAS and the emergence of Neolithic cultures? PAS induce pro-social behavior and conformity to social constraints, and reduce scalar, work, and status-related stress. Thus, PAS should promote tolerance of larger, more impersonal social structures and the behaviors required of individuals participating in them (Muller and Schumann 2011; Nesse 2002; Velea and Hautefeuille 2007). They provide substitute rewards in the present that facilitate commitments to work for uncertain future rewards, and they are used to elicit appropriate emotional responses during work. Over the millennia, sociopolitical and economic organizers have promoted their use by workers.

A Pharmacological View of the Neolithic Transition

Considering the prominence of crops that could produce PAS in early farming assemblages, and the profound influences that PAS have upon social and individual behavior, we now explore the potential for pharmacological theory as an explanatory factor in understanding Neolithization. Our exploratory model has two facets: 1) that cultivation was adopted in part to establish more reliable supplies of PAS, and 2) that the PAS thus obtained helped to facilitate changes in social structures and behaviors that characterize the Neolithic phenomena.

A Motive for Cultivation

The alcohol model of Braidwood et al. (1953), Katz and Voigt (1986), and Hayden et al. (2012) proposes that a desire for the hedonic effects of this PAS and its enabling role in feasting and ritual drove intensive gathering and then cultivation of plants from which alcohol is derived. It is often assumed that cereals were cultivated in order to make bread, although the use of maize in South America is often viewed as having been adopted primarily to produce beer (Tykot and Staller 2002; Benz and Staller 2006). The same may have been true in the Neolithic Levant, although bread may have also had mood altering qualities due to its high glycemic index and other characteristics (Hayden et al. In press). The alcohol hypothesis is supported by pharmacological theories of artificial reward (Nesse and Berridge 1997), and is easily extensible to the cultivation of other psychoactive cultigens such as poppy, coca, coffee, cacao, tobacco, and cannabis. It is very likely that these plants were cultivated primarily for their pharmacological effects. Cultivation made psychoactive substances more available, more regularly and more reliably, to more people. Demand for PAS would have increased substantially with the appearance of competitive feasting among complex hunter-gatherers, where PAS would have been used to attract and obligate people in ways that promoted the power of successful feasters and their supporters. Space constraints limit our documentation of competitive feasting among complex hunter-gatherers prior to domestication, but the Northwest Coast potlatch provides a well-known example. Detailed documentation is provided in Hayden (2014).

Cultivators whose efforts were focused on crops with great PAS effects but little nutritional value would have had to rely on foraging and hunting for food, in some cases for very long periods; this is consistent with the archaeological record (Flannery 1973; Fuller et al. 2009; Smith 2001; Tanno and Willcox 2011). However, in the long run, crop complexes that yielded both psychocoactive and nutritional products, such as the Levantine, East Asian, and American cereal-based systems, ultimately were able to dominate entire regions.

Social Structures and Behavior

There can be little doubt regarding a correlation between PAS use and increasing social complexity, nor of the importance that early civilizations placed upon the consumption of PAS (Goodman et al. 1995; Groark 2010). The question we address is whether there is a causal relationship between social complexity and PAS use.

Politically ambitious individuals may have promoted regular PAS use, and PAS use could have substantially advanced political agendas. An increased ability to manipulate people would be a powerful tool in the hands of an aspiring leader, and ambitious leaders probably competed in the use of such tools, offering more, better quality, and newer varieties of PAS (cf. King et al. 2009). In particular, because PAS were highly desired by many people, the ability to supply PAS could facilitate the ambitions of individuals by helping to lure people into relationships involving debts, favors, and political power as part of competitive feasting (Hayden 2014). Increased compliance stemming from PAS use among some of a community, combined with intense competition by a few for leadership positions, could act to engender a hierarchy of political classes. Once political control began to appear in this context, PAS use could amplify social differences by attracting people to obtain PAS, creating acquiescence, and providing solace via PAS use for those holding lower status positions in a society that was becoming increasingly hierarchical.

Hayden (1993, 2003) has argued that the distinctive human adaptation lies not in physiology or intelligence, nor even tool use, but in an emotional adaptation to social living and the use of social skills to access food, security, dominance, mates, and success in competitive interactions. This perspective is also advocated for by Gowlett et al. (2012). The use of psychoactive substances to manipulate social relationships is likely to be part of this distinctive adaptation. Ethnographic accounts almost always associate PAS use with social acts: often the bringing together of family, friends, factions, allies, or other support personnel (e.g., Goodman et al. 1995).

Conflict is a significant contributor to forager mortality (Gat 2006), and it is no surprise that conflict also afflicted agricultural settlements as they reached higher densities (Belfer-Cohen and Goring-Morris 2011); yet within-group conflict and violence among agriculturalists was not so great as to prevent stable sedentary polities from persisting. PAS use would have helped communities to grow and become more stable by increasing people’s tolerance of large group conditions (Coccurello et al. 2009; Nagaraja and Jeganathan 2003; Xigeng et al. 2004). By enhancing pro-social behavior, PAS use would help to bind a larger group together, including inducing commitments to future physical confrontations with other groups. This would help agricultural societies to compete more effectively than foraging groups who lacked the organizational framework and group sizes critical for winning conflicts.

Complex Labor Arrangements

The idea that the provision of PAS facilitates complex labor arrangements is not new. Staller (2009) noted that maize beer was used to pay workers and was a key element in keeping the Incan state functioning. Morris (1979) quoted a chief’s 1556 testimony that “the main reason that the people obey their leaders here is through the custom that they have to give the people drink.” Aztec leaders too provided alcohol to their subjects and controlled the quantities consumed (Mitchell 2004). Egyptian, Sumerian, and probably most other Near Eastern states paid their workers partly in beer (Jennings et al. 2005). Jankowiak and Bradburd (1996) and Courtwright (2009) argued that drug foods have likewise been a globally important tool of colonizers, operating to induce natives to engage in labor and then used to increase their work efficiency, intensity, and duration. Contemporary Amazonian elders provide coca to farm workers in a manner comparable to the coffee or cigarette break that characterizes labor in the West (Hugh-Jones 1995). Cameron and Jones (1985) described a political function for “drugs of solace” in terms of maintaining working class compliance. Our model simply locates the origin of this strategy earlier within the Neolithic and at a smaller, more localized scale.

Discussion and Conclusion

Extensive archaeological evidence shows that a significant portion of the effort of early cultivators globally was probably directed to the production of psychoactive substances (PAS), including the use of cereals, poppies, coca, sugar cane, tobacco, and cannabis. We propose a model involving PAS in two phases of Neolithication. First, people were attracted to PAS, and accorded them prestige because of their rewarding, mood-altering properties. Second, as cultivation enabled PAS use to become widespread and regular, the power of PAS to influence social behavior helped to create and reinforce complex social structures by inducing acceptance of crowded conditions, heirarchical power, and regular, hard work for others. Hayden et al. (2012) concluded that "feasting and brewing very likely provided a key link between increasing “complexity” and the adoption of cereal cultivation." The pharmacological theory presented here supports and enriches this hypothesis and supplies a mechanism for the link. Importantly, it gives the model broader range, as it can be applied to PAS other than alcohol, and thus to more cases of Neolithization.

Enhancing models of Neolithization with pharmacological theory provides compelling causality for the cultivation of PAS crops, and some crops, like cereals, that are usually assumed to have had only subsistence value. When combined with the emergence of complex hunter-gatherers exhibiting feasting and other strategies for controlling labor (ancestor worship, bride prices, material ritual requirements), pharmacology can provide a more powerful explanatory framework for the emergence of increasing social complexity.

Although foreshadowed by developments in complex hunter-gatherers, we propose that a major component of the Neolithic transition was a pharmacological transition consisting of a change in the kinds of psychoactive substances consumed (from shamanic use of hallucinogens to mood-altering PAS), the quantity and frequency of consumption, the ways they were produced, and the range of people who consumed them. The production of PAS crops represented supplemental work beyond mere subsistence needs, and therefore generally would have been undertaken by those with subsistence surpluses and greater access to labor. For example, the production of beer from grain was predicated on a surplus production of grain beyond subsistence needs that only the relatively wealthy could afford (Arthur 2003; Dietler 1990; Jennings et al. 2005). Thus, it would have predominantly been wealthy and highly motivated individuals who produced, controlled, and used PAS to advance their political strategies, generally via cultivation and the hosting of feasts.

Our reasoning is applicable to both fully agricultural and mixed-strategy groups, and can help to explain both primary Neolithization and its spread from initial sites of cultivation and domestication. It is compatible with the proposed extension of optimal foraging models to include the prestige associated with particular items including special foods (Hayden 2009, 2014; Winterhalder and Kennett 2006). Finally it can help explain why only crop complexes that produced foods both with PAS and subsistence value resulted in the formation of large-scale polities. Thus pharmacology belongs, we believe, among the factors from which accounts and models of early cultivation are constructed.

Acknowledgments

Greg Wadley would like to thank Prof. Angus Martin of the University of Melbourne for discussion and advice. Brian Hayden’s research on feasting was supported by the Canadian Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council.

References Cited

1.

P. Anderson 2006. Global Use of Alcohol, Drugs and Tobacco. Drug and Alcohol Review 25:489–502. Google Scholar

2.

F. Anthony M. C. Combes C. Astorga B. Bertrand G. Graziosiand P. Lashermes 2002. The Origin of Cultivated Coffea arabica L. Varieties Revealed by AFLP and SSR Markers. Theoretical and Applied Genetics 104:894–900. Google Scholar

3.

A. Armario J. M. Castellanosand J. Balasch 2008. Effect of Crowding on Emotional Reactivity in Male Rats. Neuroendocrinology 39:330–333. Google Scholar

4.

J. W. Arthur 2003. Brewing Beer: Status, Wealth and Ceramic Use Alteration Among the Gamo of South-Western Ethiopia. World Archaeology 34:516–528. Google Scholar

5.

M. T. Bardo J. L. Neisewanderand T. H. Kelly 2013. Individual Differences and Social Influences on the Neurobehavioral Pharmacology of Abused Drugs. Pharmacological Reviews 65:255–290. Google Scholar

6.

G. Bedi D. Hymanand H. de Wit 2010. Is Ecstasy an “Empathogen”? Effects of MDMA on Prosocial Feelings and Identification of Emotional States in Others. Biological Psychiatry 68:1134–1140. Google Scholar

7.

A. Belfer-Cohenand A. N. Goring-Morris 2011. Becoming Farmers: The Inside Story. Current Anthropology 52:S209–S220. Google Scholar

8.

Yu. A. Belyaeva V. A. Dubynin I. S. Stovolosovand A. A Kamensky 2008. Neurotropic Activity of Exorphins with Different Affinity to the Opioid Receptors of m and d-Types. Neurochemical Journal 2:1–2. Google Scholar

9.

B. F. Benzand J. E. Staller 2006. The Antiquity, Biogeography, and Culture History of Maize in the Americas. In Histories of Maize: Multidisciplinary Approaches to the Prehistory, Linguistics, Biogeography, Domestication, and Evolution of Maize, edited by J. Staller R. Tykotand B. Benz pp. 647–673. Left Coast Press, Walnut Creek, CA. Google Scholar

10.

K. C. Berridgeand T. E. Robinson 1998. What is the Role of Dopamine in Reward: Hedonic Impact, Reward Learning, or Incentive Salience? Brain Research Reviews 28:309–369. Google Scholar

11.

W. K. Bickeland L. A. Marsch 2001. Toward a Behavioral Economic Understanding of Drug Dependence: Delay Discounting Processes. Addiction 96:73–86. Google Scholar

12.

E. Blass E. Fitzgeraldand P. Kehoe 1987. Interactions Between Sucrose, Pain and Isolation Distress. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 26:483–9. Google Scholar

13.

K. Blum A. L. Chen J. Giordano J. Borsten T. J. Chen M. Hauser T. Simpatico J. Fernino E. R. Bravermanand D. Barh 2012. The Addictive Brain: All Roads Lead to Dopamine. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs 44:134–143. Google Scholar

14.

S. Bowles 2011. Cultivation of Cereals by the First Farmers Was Not More Productive Than Foraging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108:4760–4765. Google Scholar

15.

R. J. Braidwood J. D. Sauer H. Helbaek P. C. Mangelsdorf H. C. Cutler C. S. Coon R. Linton J. Stewardand A. L. Oppenheim 1953. Symposium: Did Man Once Live By Beer Alone? American Anthropologist 55:515–26. Google Scholar

16.

J. B. Calhoun 1973. Death Squared: The Explosive Growth and Demise of a Mouse Population. Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine 66:80–88. Google Scholar

17.

D. Cameronand I. G. Jones 1985. An Epidemiological and Sociological Analysis of the Use of Alcohol, Tobacco and Other Drugs of Solace. Journal of Public Health 7:18–29. Google Scholar

18.

R. Coccurello F. R. D’Amatoand A. Moles 2009. Chronic Social Stress, Hedonism and Vulnerability to Obesity: Lessons from Rodents. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33:537–550. Google Scholar

19.

D. J. Cohen 2011. The Beginnings of Agriculture in China. Current Anthropology 52:S273–S293. Google Scholar

20.

M. N. Cohen 1975. Population Pressure and the Origins of Agriculture: An Archaeological Example from the Coast of Peru. In Population, Ecology and Social Evolution, edited by S. Polgar pp. 79–121 The Hague, Mouton. Google Scholar

21.

M. N. Cohen 1989. Health and the Rise of Civilization. Yale University Press, New Haven. Google Scholar

22.

M. N. Cohen 2009. Introduction: Rethinking the Origins of Agriculture. Current Anthropology 50:591–595. Google Scholar

23.

M. L. Cooper M. Russell J. B. Skinner M. R. Froneand P. Mudar 1992. Stress and Alcohol Use: Moderating Effects of Gender, Coping, and Alcohol Expectancies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 101:139. Google Scholar

24.

D. T. Courtwright 2009. Forces of Habit: Drugs and the Making of the Modern World. Harvard University Press, Cambridge. Google Scholar

25.

G. Crawford 1983. Paleoethnobotany of the Kameda Peninsula Jomon. Museum of Anthropology, University of Michigan, Anthropological Paper 73. Google Scholar

26.

J. Danielsand C. Daniels 1993. Sugarcane in Prehistory. Archaeology in Oceania 28:1–7. Google Scholar

27.

W. M. Daniels C. Y. Pietersen M. E. Carstens S. Dayaand D. Stein 2000. Overcrowding Induces Anxiety and Causes Loss of Serotonin 5HT-1a Receptors in Rats. Metabolic Brain Disease 15:287–295. Google Scholar

28.

T. Denham 2011. Early Agriculture and Plant Domestication in New Guinea and Island Southeast Asia. Current Anthropology 52:S379–S395. Google Scholar

29.

M. Dietler 1990. Driven by Drink: The Role of Drinking in the Political Economy and the Case of the Early Iron Age in France. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 9:352–406. Google Scholar

30.

M. Dietler 2001. Theorizing the Feast: Rituals of Consumption, Commensal Politics, and Power in African Contexts. In Feasts, edited by M. Dietlerand B. Hayden pp. 65–114. Smithsonian Books, Washington, D.C. Google Scholar

31.

O. Dietrich M. Heun J. Notroff K. Schmidtand M. Zarnkow 2012. The Role of Cult and Feasting in the Emergence of Neolithic Communities: New Evidence from Göbekli Tepe, South-Eastern Turkey. Antiquity 86:674–695. Google Scholar

32.

T. D. Dillehay J. Rossen D. Ugent A. Karathanasis V. Vásquezand P. J. Netherly 2010. Early Holocene Coca Chewing in Northern Peru. Antiquity 84:939–953. Google Scholar

33.

L. Dubé J. L LeBeland J. Lu 2005. Affect Asymmetry and Comfort Food Consumption. Physiology and Behavior 86:559–567. Google Scholar

34.

C. Duraffourd F. De Vadder D. Goncalves F. Delaere A. Penhoat B. Brussetand G. Mithieux 2012. Mu-opioid Receptors and Dietary Protein Stimulate a Gut-Brain Neural Circuitry Limiting Food Intake. Cell 15:377–388. Google Scholar

35.

Y. M. Epstein 1981. Crowding Stress and Human Behavior. Journal of Social Issues 37:126–144. Google Scholar

36.

K. Flannery 1973. The Origins of Agriculture. Annual Review of Anthropology 2:271–310. Google Scholar

37.

D. Q. Fuller 2005. Ceramics, Seeds and Culinary Change in Prehistoric India. Antiquity 79:761–777. Google Scholar

38.

D. Q. Fuller L. Qin Y. Zheng Z. Zhao X. Chen L. A. Hosoyaand G. P. Sun 2009. The Domestication Process and Domestication Rate in Rice: Spikelet Bases from the Lower Yangtze. Science 323:1607–1610. Google Scholar

39.

T. M. Friesen 1999. Resource Structure, Scalar Stress, and the Development of Inuit Social Organization. World Archaeology 31:21–37. Google Scholar

40.

G. J. Fritzand T. R. Kidder 1993. Recent Investigations Into Prehistoric Agriculture in the Lower Mississippi Valley. Southeastern Archaeology 12:1–14. Google Scholar

41.

J. H. Galloway 2005. The Sugar Cane Industry: An Historical Geography from Its Origins To 1914. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Google Scholar

42.

A. Gat 2006. War In Human Civilization. Oxford University Press, Oxford. Google Scholar

43.

E. L. Gibson 2012. The Psychobiology of Comfort Eating: Implications for Neuropharmacological Interventions. Behavioral Pharmacology 23:442–460. Google Scholar

44.

L. A. Giordano W. K. Bickel G. Loewenstein E. A. Jacobs L. Marschand G. J. Badger 2002. Mild Opioid Deprivation Increases the Degree That Opioid-Dependent Outpatients Discount Delayed Heroin and Money. Psychopharmacology 163:174–182. Google Scholar

45.

J. Goodman P. E. Lovejoyand A. Sherratt eds. 1995. Consuming Habits: Drugs in History and Anthropology. Routledge, London. Google Scholar

46.

A. N. Goring-Morrisand A. Belfer-Cohen 2011. Neolithization Processes in the Levant: The Outer Envelope. Current Anthropology 52:S195–S208. Google Scholar

47.

J. Gowlett C. Gambleand R. Dunbar 2012. Human Evolution and the Archaeology of the Social Brain. Current Anthropology 53:693–723. Google Scholar

48.

L. Greenand J. Myerson 2004. A Discounting Framework for Choice with Delayed and Probabilistic Rewards. Psychological Bulletin 130:769. Google Scholar

49.

O. B. V. A. Gritsai Z. D. Dubynin V. E. Bespalovaand V. E Pilipenko 2009. Effects of Several Exorphins and Endorphins on the Escape Reaction of the Cockroach Periplaneta americana Under Elevated Temperature Conditions. Journal of Evolutionary Biochemistry and Physiology 45:476–483. Google Scholar

50.

L. Grivet C. Daniels J. C. Glaszmannand A. D'Hont 2004. A Review of Recent Molecular Genetics Evidence for Sugarcane Evolution and Domestication. Ethnobiology Research and Applications 2:9–17. Google Scholar

51.

K. P. Groark 2010. The Angel in the Gourd: Ritual, Therapeutic, and Protective Uses of Tobacco (Nicotiana tabacum) Among the Tzeltal and Tzotzil Maya of Chiapas, Mexico. Journal of Ethnobiology 30:5–30. Google Scholar

52.

L. Grunberg S. Moore R. Anderson-Connollyand E. Greenberg 1999. Work Stress and Self-Reported Alcohol Use: The Moderating Role of Escapist Reasons for Drinking. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology 4:29. Google Scholar

53.

J. S. Guthrieand D. O. Ho-Yen 2011. Alcohol and Cholera. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine 104:98. Google Scholar

54.

E. H. Hagen C. J. Rouletteand R. J. Sullivan 2013. Explaining Human Recreational Use of ‘Pesticides.’. Frontiers in Psychiatry 4:142. Google Scholar

55.

M. Haney S. Maccari M. Le Moal H. Simonand P. Vincenzo Piazza 1995. Social Stress Increases the Acquisition of Cocaine Self-Administration in Male and Female Rats. Brain Research 698:46–52. Google Scholar

56.

B. Hayden 1990. Nimrods, Piscators, Pluckers and Planters: The Emergence of Food Production. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 9:31–69. Google Scholar

57.

B. Hayden 1993. Archaeology: The Science of Once and Future Things. Freeman, New York. Google Scholar

58.

B. Hayden 2000. On Territoriality and Sedentism. Current Anthropology 41:109–112. Google Scholar

59.

B. Hayden 2003. Shamans, Sorcerers, and Saints: The Prehistory of Religion. Smithsonian Books, Washington, D.C. Google Scholar

60.

B. Hayden 2004. Sociopolitical Organization in the Natufian: A View from the Northwest. In The Last Hunter-Gatherer Societies In the Near East, edited by C. Delage pp. 263–308. BAR International Series, Oxford. Google Scholar

61.

B. Hayden 2009. The Proof is in the Pudding: Feasting and the Origins of Domestication. Current Anthropology 50:597–601. Google Scholar

62.

B. Hayden 2011a. Feasting and Social Dynamics in the Epipaleolithic of the Fertile Crescent. In Guess Who’s Coming To Dinner: Feasting Rituals in the Prehistoric Societies of Europe and the Near East, edited by G. Aranda S. Monton-Subiasand M. Sanchez pp. 30–63 Oxbow Books, Oxford. Google Scholar

63.

B. Hayden 2011b. Rice: The First Asian Luxury Food? In Why Cultivate? Anthropological and Archaeological Approaches To Foraging-Farming Transitions In Southeast Asia, edited by G. Barkerand M. Janowski pp. 75–94. McDonald Institute Monograph, Cambridge. Google Scholar

64.

B. Hayden 2014. The Power of Feasts. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Google Scholar

65.

B. Hayden N. Canueland J. Shanse 2012. What Was Brewing in the Natufian? An Archaeological Assessment of Brewing Technology in the Epipaleolithic. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory 20:102–150. Google Scholar

66.

B. Hayden L. Nixon-Darcusand L. Ansell In press. The Origins of Beer and Bread. In The Materiality of Foodstuffs, edited by L. Steel editor. Routledge, London. Google Scholar

67.

J. S. Henderson R. A. Joyce G. R. Hall W. J. Hurstand P. E. McGovern 2007. Chemical and Archaeological Evidence for the Earliest Cacao Beverages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104:18937–18940. Google Scholar

68.

D. O. Henry 1989. From Foraging to Agriculture: The Levant at the End of the Ice Age. University of Pennsylvania Press, Philadelphia. Google Scholar

69.

L. Hilakivi-Clarkeand R. G. Lister 1992. Social Status and Voluntary Alcohol Consumption in Mice: Interaction With Stress. Psychopharmacology 108:276–282. Google Scholar

70.

G. Hillman R. Hedges A. Moore S. Colledgeand P. Pettitt 2001. New Evidence of Late Glacial Cereal Cultivation a Abu Hureyra on the Euphrates. Holocene 11:383–393. Google Scholar

71.

S. Hugh-Jones 1995. Coca, Beer, Cigars and Yage: Meals and Anti-Meals in an Amerindian Community. In Consuming Habits: Drugs In History and Anthropology, edited by J. Goodman P. E. Lovejoyand A. Sherratt pp. 47–65. Routledge, London. Google Scholar

72.

W. Jankowiakand D. Bradburd 1996. Using Drug Foods to Capture and Enhance Labor Performance: A Cross-Cultural Perspective. Current Anthropology 37:717–720. Google Scholar

73.

J. Jennings K. Antrobus S. Atencio E. Glavich R. Johnson G. Lofflerand C. Luu 2005. Drinking Beer in a Blissful Mood: Alcohol Production, Operational Chains, and Feasting in the Ancient World. Current Anthropology 46:275–303. Google Scholar

74.

P Johnsonand P. J. Kenny 2010. Dopamine D2 Receptors in Addiction-Like Reward Dysfunction and Compulsive Eating in Obese Rats. Nature Neuroscience 13:635–41. Google Scholar

75.

S. H. Katzand M. M. Voigt 1986. Bread and Beer: The Early Use of Cereals In the Human Diet. Expedition 28:23–34. Google Scholar

76.

R. L. Kelly 1992. Mobility/Sedentism: Concepts, Archaeological Measures, and Effects. Annual Review Of Anthropology 21:43–66. Google Scholar

77.

Z. Kerem S. Lev-Yadun A. Gopher P. Weinbergand S. Abbo 2007. Chickpea Domestication in the Neolithic Levant Through the Nutritional Perspective. Journal of Archaeological Science 34:1289–1293. Google Scholar

78.

A. J. King D. P. Johnsonand M. Van Vugt 2009. The Origins and Evolution of Leadership. Current Biology 19:R911–R916. Google Scholar

79.

M. Kivimäki J. Head J. E. Ferrie M. J. Shipley E. Brunner J. Vahteraand M. G. Marmot 2006. Work Stress, Weight Gain and Weight Loss: Evidence For Bidirectional Effects Of Job Strain on Body Mass Index in the Whitehall II Study. International Journal of Obesity 30:982–987. Google Scholar

80.

V. J. Knight 2001. Feasting and the Emergence Of Platform Mound Ceremonialism In Eastern North America. In Feasts: Archaeological and Ethnographic Perspectives on Food Politics, and Power, edited by M. Dietlerand B. Hayden pp 311–333., Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, DC. Google Scholar

81.

A. Kouvonen M. Kivimäki M. Virtanen J. Penttiand J. Vahtera 2005a. Work Stress, Smoking Status, and Smoking Intensity: An Observational Study of 46 190 Employees. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 59:63–69. Google Scholar

82.

A. Kouvonen M. Kivimäki S. J. Cox T. Coxand J. Vahtera 2005b. Relationship Between Work Stress and Body Mass Index Among 45,810 Female and Male Employees. Psychosomatic Medicine 67:577–583. Google Scholar

83.

I. Kuijtand N. Goring-Morris 2002. Foraging, Farming, and Social Complexity in the Pre-Pottery Neolithic of the Southern Levant: A Review and Synthesis. Journal of World Prehistory 16:361–440. Google Scholar

84.

La Barre. 1970. The Ghost Dance: Origins of Religion. Doubleday, Garden City, NY. Google Scholar

85.

D. H. Lendeand E. O. Smith 2002. Evolution Meets Biopsychosociality: An Analysis of Addictive Behavior. Addiction 97:447–458. Google Scholar

86.

H. L. Li 1974. The Origin and Use of Cannabis in Eastern Asia: Linguistic-Cultural Implications. Economic Botany 28:293–301. Google Scholar

87.

H. L. Li 1983. The Domestication of Plants in China: Ecogeographical Considerations. In The Origins of Chinese Civilization, edited by D. N. Keightley pp. 21–63. University of California Press, Berkeley. Google Scholar

88.

O. Lindauerand J. H. Blitz 1997. Higher Ground: The Archaeology of North American Platform Mounds. Journal of Archaeological Research 5(2):169–207. Google Scholar

89.

P. E. Lovejoy 1980. Kola in the History of West Africa. Cahiers d'Etudes africaines 20:97–134. Google Scholar

90.

G. Martinotti M. D. Nicola D. Tedeschi S. Cundariand L. Janiri 2009. Empathy Ability is Impaired in Alcohol-Dependent Patients. The American Journal on Addictions 18:157–161. Google Scholar

91.

P. E. McGovern 2009. Uncorking the Past: The Quest for Wine, Beer, and Other Alcoholic Beverages. University of California Press, Berkeley. Google Scholar

92.

P. E. McGovern J. Zhang J. Tang Z. Zhang G. R. Hall R. A. Moreauand C. Wang 2004. Fermented Beverages of Pre- and Proto-Historic China. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 101:17593–17598. Google Scholar

93.

M. D. Merlin 2003. Archaeological Evidence for the Tradition of Psychoactive Plant Use in the Old World. Economic Botany 57:295–323. Google Scholar

94.

T. J. Mitchell 2004. Intoxicated Identities: Alcohol's Power In Mexican History and Culture. Routledge, New York. Google Scholar

95.

C. J. Morgan L. A. Noronha M. Muetzelfeldt A. Fieldingand H. V. Curran 2013. Harms and Benefits Associated with Psychoactive Drugs: Findings of an International Survey of Active Drug Users. Journal of Psychopharmacology 27:497–506. Google Scholar

96.

C. Morris 1979. Maize Beer in the Economics, Politics, and Religion of the Inca Empire. In Fermented Foods in Nutrition, edited by C. Gastineau W. Darbyand T. Turner pp. 21–34. Academic Press, New York. Google Scholar

97.

C. P. Müllerand G. Schumann 2011. Drugs as Instruments: A New Framework for Non-Addictive Psychoactive Drug Use. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34:293–310. Google Scholar

98.

H. Naitoh S. Nomura Y. Kunimiand K. Yamaoka 1992. “Swimming-Induced Head Twitching” in Rats in the Forced Swimming Test Induced by Overcrowding Stress: A New Marker in the Animal Model of Depression? The Keio Journal of Medicine 41:221. Google Scholar

99.

H. S. Nagarajaand P. S. Jeganathan 2003. Effect of Acute and Chronic Conditions of Overcrowding on Free Choice Ethanol Intake in Rats. Indian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology 47:325–331. Google Scholar

100.

R. M. Nesseand K. C. Berridge 1997. Psychoactive Drug Use in Evolutionary Perspective. Science 278:64–65. Google Scholar

101.

R. M. Nesse 2002. Evolution and Addiction. Addiction 97:470–471. Google Scholar

102.

D. M. Pechlivanova J. D. Tchekalarova L. H. Alova V. V. Petkov R. P. Nikolovand K. S. Yakimova 2012. Effect of Long-Term Caffeine Administration on Depressive-Like Behavior in Rats Exposed to Chronic Unpredictable Stress. Behavioral Pharmacology 23:339–347. Google Scholar

103.

J. Perrot 1966. Le gisement Natoufien de Mallaha (Eynan) Israel. L’Anthropologie 70:437–483. Google Scholar

104.

H. Pickard 2012. The Purpose in Chronic Addiction. AJOB Neuroscience 3:40–49. Google Scholar

105.

B. Pickersgill 2007. Domestication of Plants in the Americas: Insights from Mendelian and Molecular Genetics. Annals of Botany 100:925–940. Google Scholar

106.

T. Plowman 1984. The Origin, Evolution, and Diffusion of Coca, Erythroxylum spp., in South and Central America in Pre-Columbian Plant Migration. Papers of the Peabody Museum of Archaeology and Ethnology Cambridge 76:125–163. Google Scholar

107.

T. G. Powis F. Valdez Jr T. R. Hester W. J. Hurstand S. M. Tarka Jr 2002. Spouted Vessels and Cacao Use Among the Preclassic Maya. Latin American Antiquity 13:85–106. Google Scholar

108.

T. D. Priceand O. Bar-Yosef 2011. The Origins of Agriculture: New Data, New Ideas. Current Anthropology 52:S163–S174. Google Scholar

109.

S. M. Rafferty 2006. Evidence of Early Tobacco in Northeastern North America? Journal of Archaeological Science 33:453–458. Google Scholar

110.

E. A. Reber 2006. A Hard Row to Hoe: Changing Maize Use in the American Bottom and Surrounding Areas. In Histories of Maize: Multidisciplinary Approaches to the Prehistory, Linguistics, Biogeography, Domestication, and Evolution of Maize, edited by J. Staller R. Tykotand B. Benz pp. 236–248. Left Coast Press, Walnut Creek, CA. Google Scholar

111.

T. E. Robinsonand K. C. Berridge 1993. The Neural Basis of Drug Craving: An Incentive-Sensitization Theory of Addiction. Brain Research Reviews 18:247–291. Google Scholar

112.

J. Roach 2005. 9000-Year-Old Beer Recreated from Chinese Recipe. National Geographic News [web page].  http://news.nationalgeographic.com/news/2005/07/0718_050718_ancientbeer.html. Accessed on 19th June, 2015. Google Scholar

113.

M. Rosenberg 1990. The Mother of Invention: Evolutionary Theory, Territoriality, and the Origins of Agriculture. American Anthropologist 92:399–415. Google Scholar

114.

R. Rudgley 1995. The Archaic Use of Hallucinogens in Europe: An Archaeology of Altered States. Addiction 90:163–4. Google Scholar

115.

A. Saniotis 2010. Evolutionary and Anthropological Approaches Towards Understanding Human Need for Psychotropic and Mood Altering Substances. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs 42:477–484. Google Scholar

116.

A. Sherratt 1995a. Introduction: Peculiar Substances. In Consuming Habits: Drugs in History and Anthropology, edited by J. Goodman P. E. Lovejoyand A. Sherratt pp. 1–10. Routledge, London. Google Scholar

117.

A. Sherratt 1995b. Alcohol and Its Alternatives: Symbol and Substance in Pre-Industrial Cultures. In Consuming habits: drugs in history and anthropology, edited by J. Goodman P. E. Lovejoyand A. Sherratt pp. 11–46. Routledge, London. Google Scholar

118.

J. Siegrist 1996. Adverse Health Effects of High-Effort/Low-Reward Conditions. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology 1:27. Google Scholar

119.

R. Sinha 2001. How Does Stress Increase Risk of Drug Abuse and Relapse? Psychopharmacology 158:343–359. Google Scholar

120.

J. Smalleyand M. Blake 2003. Sweet Beginnings: Stalk Sugar and the Domestication of Maize. Current Anthropology 44:675–703. Google Scholar

121.

B. D. Smith 2001. Low-Level Food Production. Journal of Archaeological Research 9:1–43. Google Scholar

122.

O. Sokolov N. Kost O. Andreeva E. Korneeva V. Meshavkin Y. Tarakanova A. Dadayan Y. Zolotarev S. Grachev I. Mikheeva O. Varlamov A. Zozulya 2014. Autistic Children Display Elevated Urine Levels of Bovine Casomorphin-7 Immunoreactivity. Peptides 56: 68–71. Google Scholar

123.

P. Soubrié 1986. Reconciling the Role of Central Serotonin Neurones in Human and Animal Behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9:319–364. Google Scholar

124.

J. Staller 2009. The Social, Symbolic, and Economic Significance of Zea mays L. in the Late Horizon Period. In Histories of Maize: Multidisciplinary Approaches to the Prehistory, Linguistics, Biogeography, Domestication, and Evolution of Maize, edited by J. Staller R. Tykotand B. Benz pp. 449–467. Left Coast Press, Walnut Creek, CA. Google Scholar

125.

I. S. Steinand G. I. Madden 2012. Delay Discounting and Drug Abuse: Empirical, Conceptual, and Methodological Considerations. In The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Addiction Psychopharmacology, edited by J. MacKillopand H. De Wit pp. 165. John Wiley, Chichester. Google Scholar

126.

R. J. Sullivanand E. H. Hagen 2002. Psychotropic Substance-Seeking: Evolutionary Pathology or Adaptation? Addiction 97:389–400. Google Scholar

127.

K. Tannoand G. Willcox 2011. Distinguishing Wild and Domestic Wheat and Barley Spikelets from Early Holocene Sites in the Near East. Vegetation History and Archaeobotany 21:107–115. Google Scholar

128.

B. Thomas 2003. The Psychoactive Flora of Papua New Guinea. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs 35:285–293. Google Scholar

129.

M. Todd 2004. Daily Processes in Stress and Smoking: Effects of Negative Events, Nicotine Dependence, and Gender. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 18:31–39. Google Scholar

130.

M. Touw 1981. The Religious and Medicinal Uses of Cannabis in China, India and Tibet. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs 13:23–34. Google Scholar

131.

B. Tucker 2006. A Future Discounting Explanation for the Persistence of a Mixed Foraging Horticulture Strategy among the Mikea of Madagascar. In Behavioral Ecology and the Transition to Agriculture, edited by D. J. Kennettand B. Winterhalder pp. 22–40. University of California Press, Berkeley. Google Scholar

132.

V. Trezza R. Damsteegt E. M. Achterbergand L. J. Vanderschuren 2011. Nucleus Accumbens μ-opioid Receptors Mediate Social Reward. The Journal of Neuroscience 31:6362–6370. Google Scholar

133.

S. Tushingham D. Ardura J. Eerkens M. Palazoglu S. Shahbazand O. Fiehn 2013. Hunter-Gatherer Tobacco Smoking: Earliest Evidence from the Pacific Northwest Coast of North America. Journal of Archaeological Science 40(2):1397–1407. Google Scholar

134.

K. C. Twiss 2008. Transformations in an Early Agricultural Society: Feasting in the Southern Levantine Pre-Pottery Neolithic. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 27:418–442. Google Scholar

135.

R. Tykotand J. Staller 2002. The Importance of Early Maize Agriculture in Coastal Ecuador: New Data from La Emerenciana. Current Anthropology 43:666–77. Google Scholar

136.

Y. M. Ulrich-Lai A. M. Christiansen M. M. Ostrander A. A. Jones K. R. Jones D. C. Choiand J. P. Herman 2010. Pleasurable Behaviors Reduce Stress Via Brain Reward Pathways. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107:20529–20534. Google Scholar

137.

C. S. VanPool 2009. The Signs of the Sacred: Identifying Shamans Using Archaeological Evidence. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 28:177–190. Google Scholar

138.

D. Veleaand M. Hautefeuille 2007. Empathogenic Agents: Their Use, Abuse, Mechanism of Action and Addiction Potential. In Empathy In Mental Illness, edited by T. Farrowand P. Woodruff pp. 289–309. Cambridge University Press, Berkeley. Google Scholar

139.

A. Von Gernet 1995. Nicotian Dreams: The Prehistory and Early History Of Tobacco in Eastern North America. In Consuming Habits: Drugs In History and Anthropology, edited by J. Goodman P. E. Lovejoyand A. Sherratt pp. 67–87. Routledge, London. Google Scholar

140.

G. Wadleyand A. Martin 1993. The Origins of Agriculture: A Biological Perspective and a New Hypothesis. Australian Biologist 6:96–105. Google Scholar

141.

X. T. Wangand R. D. Dvorak 2010. Sweet Future: Fluctuating Blood Glucose Levels Affect Future Discounting. Psychological Science 21:183–188. Google Scholar

142.

B. Wansink M. M. Cheneyand N. Chan 2003. Exploring Comfort Food Preferences Across Age and Gender. Physiology and Behavior 79:739–747. Google Scholar

143.

B. Weinberg 2000. The World Of Caffeine: The Science and Culture of the World's Most Popular Drug. Routledge, New York. Google Scholar

144.

J. Wilbert 1979. Magico-Religious Use of Tobacco among South American Indians. In Spirits, Shamans, and Stars. Perspectives from South America, edited by D. L. Browman pp. 13–38. Mouston, The Hague. Google Scholar

145.

G. Willcox 2007. The Adoption of Farming and the Beginnings of the Neolithic in the Euphrates Valley. In The Origins and Spread of Domestic Plants in Southwest Asia and Europe. Edited by S. Colledgeand J. Conolly pp. 21–36. Left Coast Press, Walnut Creek, CA. Google Scholar

146.

G. Willcox S. Forniteand L. Herveux 2008. Early Holocene Cultivation before Domestication in Northern Syria. Vegetation History and Archaeobotany 17:313–325. Google Scholar

147.

G. Willcox R. Buxoand L. Herveux 2009. Late Pleistocene and Early Holocene Climate and the Beginnings of Cultivation in Northern Syria. The Holocene 19:151–158. Google Scholar

148.

B. Winterhalderand D. J. Kennett 2006. Behavioral Ecology and the Transition from Hunting and Gathering to Agriculture. In Behavioral Ecology and the Transition To Agriculture, edited by D. J. Kennettand B. Winterhalder pp. 1–21. University of California Press, Berkeley. Google Scholar

149.

B. Winterhalderand D. J. Kennett 2009. Four Neglected Concepts with a Role to Play in Explaining the Origins of Agriculture. Current Anthropology 50:645–648. Google Scholar

150.

J. Wolffgrammand A. Heyne 1991. Social Behavior, Dominance, and Social Deprivation of Rats Determine Drug Choice. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 38:389–399. Google Scholar

151.

J. Woodburn 1988. African Hunter-Gatherer Social Organization: Is it Best Understood as a Product of Encapsulation? In Hunters and Gatherers 1: History, Evolution and Social Change, edited by T. R. Ingoldand J. Woodburn pp. 31–64. Berg, New York. Google Scholar

152.

D. A. Wymer 1994. The Social Context of Early Maize in the Mid-Ohio Valley. In Corn & Culture in the Prehistoric New World, edited by S. Johannessenand C. A. Hastorf pp. 414–426, Westview Press, Boulder, CO. Google Scholar

153.

Z. Xigeng L. Yonghui L. Xiaojing X. Lin W. Dongmei L. Jieand S. Nan 2004. Social Crowding Sensitizes High-Responding Rats to Psychomotor-Stimulant Effects of Morphine. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 79:213–218. Google Scholar

154.

S. N. Young 2007. How to Increase Serotonin in the Human Brain Without Drugs. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience 32:394–399. Google Scholar

155.

L. Zapata L. Peña-Chocarro G. Pérez-Jordáand H. P. Stika 2004. Early Neolithic Agriculture in the Iberian Peninsula. Journal of World Prehistory 18:283–325. Google Scholar

156.

C. Zioudrou R. Streatyand W. Klee 1979. Opioid Peptides Derived from Food Proteins: The Exorphins. Journal of Biological Chemistry 254:2446–9. Google Scholar

157.

D. Zohary M. Hopfand E. Weiss 2012. Domestication of Plants in the Old World: The Origin and Spread of Domesticated Plants. In Southwest Asia, Europe, and the Mediterranean Basin. Oxford University Press, Oxford. Google Scholar
© 2015 Society of Ethnobiology
Greg Wadley and Brian Hayden "Pharmacological Influences on the Neolithic Transition," Journal of Ethnobiology 35(3), 566-584, (1 October 2015). https://doi.org/10.2993/etbi-35-03-566-584.1
Published: 1 October 2015
KEYWORDS
alcohol
complex societies
Feasting
NEOLITHIC TRANSITION
pharmacology
Back to Top