Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 May 2011 A Review of Chemosensation and Related Behavior in Aquatic Insects
José G. Crespo
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Insects that are secondarily adapted to aquatic environments are able to sense odors from a diverse array of sources. The antenna of these insects, as in all insects, is the main chemosensory structure and its input to the brain allows for integration of sensory information that ultimately ends in behavioral responses. Only a fraction of the aquatic insect orders have been studied with respect to their sensory biology and most of the work has centered either on the description of the different types of sensilla, or on the behavior of the insect as a whole. In this paper, the literature is exhaustively reviewed and ways in which antennal morphology, brain structure, and associated behavior can advance better understanding of the neurobiology involved in processing of chemosensory information are discussed. Moreover, the importance of studying such group of insects is stated, and at the same time it is shown that many interesting questions regarding olfactory processing can be addressed by looking into the changes that aquatic insects undergo when leaving their aquatic environment.

Introduction

Aquatic insects are an arbitrary group that includes insects that are associated with an aquatic or semiaquatic environment in one or more of their life stages (Daly 1984). Although only a small percentage (∼ 3%) of insects are aquatic, representatives are found in 13 insect orders (Daly 1984; Williams and Feltmate 1992) suggesting that partial aquatic lifestyle might be advantageous to a wide array of insects. Generally, aquatic insects are nymphs/larvae of terrestrial adults (see the section on Coleoptera below for an exception, Brown 1987) that spend some time in terrestrial environments during certain stage/s of their life cycle (see Plecoptera exception, Jewett 1963). These insects have been, in some cases, well studied because they are vectors of several diseases (e.g. Malaria, see Cook 1997), can be environmental quality biosensors (e.g. Kashian et al. 2007), and are utilized for understanding aquatic communities (e.g. Benke 1979; Waters 1979) and several other areas of ecology such as predator-prey interactions, competition, population dynamics, etc. (Resh and Rosenberg 1984).

According to the fossil record, aquatic insects appeared in the Triassic (Zherikhin in Belayeva et al. 2002), more than 150 MY after the appearance of insects (Gaunt and Miles 2002; Engel and Grimaldi 2004). This fact, along with the presence of a tracheal system in nearly all aquatic insects (Chapman 1998), supports the idea that these animals secondarily adapted to living in water (Resh and Solem 1984; Ross 1967; Pritchard et al. 1993). During the immature stages, insect behavior changes during development, which is interesting from an evolutionary point of view because there are several examples of species of different taxa that have secondarily adapted to living in water.

Insects that have adapted to aquatic habitats face a vast range of physical and chemical conditions that differ from those adapted to a terrestrial environment and thus, affect their physiology and behavior (Denny 1993; Vogel 1994). Obviously, sensory systems are also affected and since insects, like most invertebrates, rely on chemoreception as their main sensory modality (Hildebrand and Shepard 1997), this review will center on the current knowledge of their main chemoreceptor structures, i.e. their antennae, and chemosensory—associated behavior.

Chemoreception in aquatic insects is the perception of chemicals that originate in organic or inorganic sources, and if in aqueous solution, are perceived by gustatory sensilla or if airborne, perceived by olfactory sensilla. In aquatic insects, as in other aquatic animals, this distinction between taste and olfaction is vague, but is still used based on the structure, response, or particular location of the sensilla or the animal's behavioral response (Zacharuk 1980). As when released in air, chemical cues that propagate in water form a plume that in theory is well preserved at great distances from the source (Murlis et al. 1990). This, in addition to environmental conditions (e.g. turbidity of water, reduced light transmission, high habitat complexity, etc.), prompts aquatic insects to use chemical cues for foraging and in predator-prey interactions (Brönmark and Hansson 2000; Wisenden 2000).

The focus of this review is on the chemosensory adaptations of insects that live in an aquatic environment as nymphs/larvae before moving to a terrestrial setting as adults. Thus, the semiaquatic insects (e.g. Leptysma marginicollis [Order Orthoptera, Family Acrididae], Pentacora signoreti [Order Hemiptera, Family Saldidae], Simyra sp. [Order Lepidoptera, Family Noctuidae]), insects that live in interstices of the soil (e.g. Hydraena sp. [Order Coleoptera, Family Hydraenidae]), parasitoids of some aquatic insects (e.g. Hydrophylita aquivolans [Order Hymenoptera, Family Chalcidoidea]), and insects that live their whole lives in water (e.g. Laccophilus sp. [Order Coleoptera, Family Dytiscidae], and Belostoma bakeri [Order Hemiptera, Family Belostomatidae]) are not included. Of all the insect orders that have this dual way of life, only three hemimetabolous orders (Ephemeroptera, Odonata, and Plecoptera) and two holometabolous orders (Trichoptera and Diptera) have been studied so far.

In this review three aspects of chemosensation in insects are concentrated on: 1) the morphology of the antennae and its sensilla, which are the principal chemosensory organs; 2) the brain structures associated with processing chemical information (e.g. antennal lobes, mushroom bodies); and 3) the behavioral responses associated with chemical sensation (in particular olfaction). At the end of each section a short conclusion is provided on the current knowledge of each particular order. Finally, the importance of understanding how sensory information is encoded in the brain of these animals, how the data being collected will allow for better comprehension of aspects of neurobiology that remain uncertain, and the challenges that these animals face by switching from an aquatic to a terrestrial environment are discussed.

Hemimetabolous orders

Three orders of hemimetabolous insects, i.e. insects that undergo an incomplete metamorphosis, have been studied regarding their sensory biology, namely Ephemeroptera, Odonata, and Plecoptera. These are described in the first part of this review.

Ephemeroptera

Antennal morphology and types of sensilla Nymphs

A pair of filiform antennae, usually arising anterior or ventral to the eyes, are present in the nymphs. The scape and pedicel are usually well developed and the flagellum varies in the number of articles according to the species. For example, Ephemera danica has a flagellum with 26–27 segments (Rebora and Gaino 2008) while Baetis rhodani has 42–45 flagellar articles (Gaino and Rebora 1998). In this last species, a distal border of triangularshaped lobes is also characteristic of each antennal flagellar segment (Gaino and Rebora 1996). In general, the antennae have a great diversity of types of sensilla (Appendix 1), which are probably in concert with these animals' sensory requirements. The length of the antennae varies from short (or less than the head's width) to long (twice as long as the head's width; Edmunds et al. 1976). In the several species investigated so far, the following structures have been described: scolopidia, sensilla trichodea, chaetica, campaniformia, placodea, coeloconica, coeloconic-like, basiconica, and a new type of sensilla basiconica called “flat-tipped”. Although not all of the sensilla mentioned here, in the supplementary information and in following sections, have been reported to be chemosensory, they are included in this review because they are part of the antennal morphology. Furthermore, physiological data that show a non-chemosensory function (at least in the hemimetabolous orders) are not available.

Adults

After emergence from water, a radical modification of the nymph antennae occurs as the insect transforms into a subimago. In this stage, the winged mayfly is still not sexually mature and will ultimately molt to the reproductive stage (i.e. the imago stage). The small antennae of the subimago consist of a short scape, a well developed pedicel, and a thin filiform flagellum (Edmunds et al. 1976). The first two segments of the antennae are uniformly covered with microtrichia (Gaino and Rebora 1997, 1998) and the unsegmented flagellum presents cuticular ribs (Gaino and Rebora 1997). Although the subimago stage lasts for a short period of time and its antennae are not well developed, several authors have documented the presence of sensilla trichodea, coeloconica, campaniformia and basiconica (Appendix 1).

Ephemeroptera is the only order of insects that has two winged stages, i.e. a subimago and imago stage, even though some species do not molt into the imago stage (Peters and Peters 1977). The imago is the sexually mature stage and has antennae similar in form to the subimago. The antenna usually lacks sensilla in the scape and pedicel (e.g. Gaino and Rebora 1997, Gupta 1998, Slifer 1977) and cuticular scales replace the microtrichia in the scape and pedicel or only the pedicel of several species (Gaino and Rebora 1997, 1998). Gaino and Rebora (1997) suggested that these cuticular scales might indeed be squatiform sensilla, although they have not been described as such, and may perform a sensory function. These authors also reported that the unsegmented flagellum presents fanlike cuticular projections or a honeycomb-like structure.

In comparison to the nymphs, adults have less diversity of sensory structures in the antennae than would be expected from these animals' life style. Only three types of sensilla (i.e. trichodea, coeloconica, and campaniformia) have been described so far (Appendix 1).

Brain morphology

The brain morphology of Ephemeropteran nymphs has not been studied, but some information regarding the adults is available. Adults of Ephemeroptera, as well as Odonata, are considered to be primarily anosmic with respect to volatile odors because they lack the glomerular antennal lobes characteristic of Neoptera (Strausfeld et al. 1998). However, the absence of glomerular structures does not necessarily indicate (although strongly suggests) that olfaction is not a relevant sense for the animal (see Kristoffersen et al. 2008). With the present information, the suggested anosmic condition in Ephemeroptera is still debatable and studies on the electrophysiology of sensilla and detailed brain structure work are needed to resolve this issue.

Another important characteristic of the brain of these insects is the fact that the mushroom bodies present no evidence of a neopteran-like calyx (Strausfeld et al. 2009). Instead, a cluster of microglomeruli is localized in the most distal part of the pedunculus in the position where the calyx is normally located. Along with information from other phylogenetically relevant invertebrates, this observation led to the suggestion that the calyces receive olfactory input and that various sensory modalities reach the mushroom bodies indirectly through other protocerebral neuropils, not just olfactory ones (Strausfeld et al. 1998, 2009). Apparently, the role of the mushroom bodies in the palaeopteran insects would be that of integrating mechano- and optosensory rather than olfactory information, which might be correlated to the ancient environment in which they evolved (Strausfeld et al. 1998).

Behavior

Nymphs

Mayfly nymphs can be deposit-feeders, filter feeders, shredders, and/or scrapers that feed on detritus (e.g. leaf litter and associated microbioita), bacteria, diatoms, animal fragments (micro- or macroinvertebrates), and algae present on stone surfaces (Wiilliams and Feltmate 1992; Edmunds 1984). Besides gathering food, nymphs need to be able to avoid both invertebrate and vertebrate predators. Drift (i.e. the passive downstream transport of stream invertebrates of the benthos) has been repeatedly documented as a predator avoidance response (e.g. Corkum and Pointing 1979; Walton 1980; Corkum and Clifford 1980; Malmqvist and Sjöström 1987; Lancaster 1990; Flecker 1992; Culp and Scrimgeour 1993; Forrester 1994). The sensory mechanisms mediating drift and other avoidance behaviors (e.g. active swimming) when in the presence of predators (e.g. stoneflies, caddisflies, crustaceans, and fish) have been investigated on several occasions (see below). These studies have relied on static (e.g. freezing or showing tail curl behavior) and active (e.g. swimming, drifting, or crawling) behaviors to record mayflies' sensory capabilities when confronting an invertebrate or vertebrate predator.

Mayfly response to invertebrate predators

There are several invertebrate predators of mayfly nymphs. Stonefly nymphs are the most studied, but predatory mayflies and some crustaceans prey on mayfly nymphs as well. Several studies have attempted to demonstrate the nature of the predator cues that elicit the different behavioral responses in mayflies. For example, when measuring the number of nymphs in the region of highest stimulus concentration of an observation box, Peckarsky (1980) recorded that the number of individuals of certain species (e.g. Ephemerella subvaria), but not others (e.g. Baetis phoebus), decreased in the presence of chemical stimuli from a stonefly predator and later increased after the predator's removal. Furthermore, some nymphs (e.g. Baetis bicaudatus) were able to discriminate between predatory stoneflies (e.g. Megarcys signata) and a similar size omnivorous stonefly (Pteronarcella badia; Peckarsky and Dodson 1980) suggesting a chemical, tactile, or chemotactile mechanism of differentiation. Additionally, none of the mayfly species tested (seven species in total) reacted to the presence of the predators by visual cues alone indicating the importance of chemical information. These results were reinforced by the observations of Williams (1987), which showed that the same species studied by Peckarsky (1980) utilized a close-range (probably in the order of a few millimeters) chemodetection mechanism to sense the stonefly Dinocras cephalotes. The fact that only some species responded to predatory stonefly chemical cues was also found in the behavior of B. rhodani and Rhithrogena nubile, suggesting a species specific response to stonefly odors (Malmqvist 1992). In other cases, chemical cues sensed by mayflies have been shown to emanate from injured conspecifics (Huryn and Chivers 1999), which are supposed to be indirect cues of stonefly feeding, and even enhance the response to predator tactile stimuli, as in the case of Paraleptophlebia adoptiva (Ode and Wissinger 1993).

Alternatively, predator avoidance was also suggested to be a response to hydrodynamic cues. For example, Peckarsky (1987) and Peckarsky and Penton (1989a) suggested that B. bicaudatus utilizes its cerci as sensory structures in the presence of Kogotus modestus and that noncontact responses were probably due to pressure wave disturbances, i.e. hydrodynamic cues, created by the movement of the predator. Further evidence established that the mean predator-prey distance to elicit an evasive response was around 1–2 cm and the cue was again suggested to be hydrodynamic (Peckarsky 1996). However, in all these cases, chemical cues have not been discounted and recently the relevance of other arthropods' chemical cues, i.e. besides stoneflies chemical cues, in mayfly behavior was tested. Huryn and Chivers (1999) reported that Siphlonurus mirus reduced its levels of movement when exposed to chemicals from the predator mayfly, Siphlonisca aerodromia, and when Stenonema sp. was exposed to crayfish (Orconectes rusticus) conditioned water more mayflies displayed a “tail-curl” behavior, which is believed to serve as a mechanism of intimidation (Richmond and Lasenby 2006). Thus, hydrodynamic and chemical cues are probably both important for describing the mayfly response to invertebrate predators.

Mayfly response to vertebrate predators

Among the vertebrate predators of mayfly, fish (e.g. trout, sculpins, dace, and minnows) have been shown to greatly affect mayfly nymphs' behavior. Several studies have shown that mayflies use chemical cues to detect fish predators. Without rejecting other detection mechanisms, Kohler and McPeek (1989) suggested that the presence of chemicals from the mottled sculpin, Cottus bairdi, affected the feeding behavior of Baetis tricaudatus. Later on, it was found that, besides the effect of presence or absence of trout odor, mayflies adjust their behavior depending on experience (i.e. coming from a fishless or fish stream) and the time of the day (McIntosh and Peckarsky 1996). Similar to what was reported for stoneflies, these authors suggested that fish odor sensitized mayflies to the risk of predation. When the trout Oncorhynchus mykiss was added in a fishless stream, not only did Baetis coelestis reduce its daytime drift, but this behavioral response was also detected within 24 hours (Douglas et al. 1994) accounting for its persistence through time. Finally, the importance of the fish diet was demonstrated when Salvelinus fontinalis fed with S. mirus conspecifics, but not brine shrimp (a control), decreased the mayfly's movement activity (Huryn and Chivers 1999).

The fact that almost all the predator fishes utilized in these experiments fed mostly during the day (e.g. McIntosh and Toensend 1995) putting larger nymphs at higher risk than smaller ones, at least during this time of the day, persuaded researchers to look for behavioral differences among different mayfly sizes. Although, Huhta et al. (1999) found that large and small B. rhodani turn to nocturnal drift in the presence of minnow odor (Phoxinus phoxinus), size-related differences were recorded in natural environments by the addition of fish or its odor to a fish stream, i.e. a stream where a fish background odor is presumed to be present. A decrease in drift of large nymphs during the night and an increase in drift of small nymphs drift during day and night was the result of mayflies exposed to higher concentrations of fish odors, suggesting that mayflies discriminate between dissimilar concentrations of fish odor in natural environments (McIntosh et al. 1999) and that drift behavior changes depending on the developmental stage. Moreover, this behavioral response was observed within five minutes of exposure demonstrating how informative chemicals can be for these animals. However, other authors concluded that mayfly night drift probably occurred due to hydrodynamic, rather than chemical, cues from the predator (Culp et al. 1991; Tikkanen et al. 1994). Interestingly, some researchers have shown that brook trout odor can even induce morphological plasticity (e.g. develop longer caudal filaments) in mayflies, probably reducing predation rates on these insects (Dahl and Peckarsky 2002). These longer caudal filaments would improve predator detection, but have also been suggested to account for lower fitness and ultimately to a great reduction in mayfly biomass (Peckarsky et al. 2001; Peckarsky et al. 2002).

On the other hand, B. rhodani and R. nubile showed no behavioral changes to the predator Cottus gobio (Malmqvist 1992) and Callibaetis ferrugineus and seemed not to perceive brook trout odors (Caudill and Peckarsky 2003), suggesting again that evasive behaviors mediated by chemosensation may be species specific. In addition, the length of exposure to the stimulus is probably relevant. For example, Tikkanen et al. (1996) found that an immediate behavioral response in B. rhodani could be elicited only by actively foraging P. phoxinus, but not by its chemical cues alone or in combination with a fish model. However, when the fish model and chemicals were presented continuously (i.e. up to 17h.), an increase in the use of upper surfaces of tiles (where the food is located) peaked sharply in the first hours after dark. These results may indicate that mayflies use more than one type of cue to detect a predator and even invertebrate-vertebrate predator interactions cues (or maybe this interaction indirectly affects mayflies). Peckarsky and McIntosh (1998) studied the complex multiple-species interactions that occur between the mayfly, B. bicaudatus; the brook trout, S. fontinalis; and the nocturnal stonefly predator, M. signata. These authors concluded that both predators' odors reduced mature mayfly size and that while stoneflies increased night drift dispersal, trout suppressed feeding at night and drift. An interesting result was that fish odor changed the effect of stoneflies on Baetis drift in addition to reducing its drift directly, also indicating the importance of multiple prey-predator interactions (see also Soluk and Collins 1988).

As a final point, other types of odors (e.g. conspecific odors) alone or presented together with other types of stimuli have also been shown to elicit a behavioral response in mayflies. For example, Scrimgeour et al. (1994) studied the stimuli initiating changes in drift rates and position in substratum surfaces of three species of mayfly (Ephemerella aurivilli, Paraleptophlebia heteronea, and B. tricaudatus) and showed that the first two species responded to chemical stimuli alone, i.e. either predator or conspecific odors, and all three species responded to the hydrodynamic stimuli produced by the predator models, i.e. longnose dace and stonefly models, alone or in addition to chemical cues. This shows the importance of different types of stimuli on mayfly behavior when simultaneously presented. Furthermore, the behavioral response to chemical cues alone depended on the type of chemical stimulus in some species (e.g. Ephemerella responded to predator odors, but not to conspecifics odors) and was also species specific in others (e.g. Paraleptophlebia responded to conspecific odors, but Baetis and Ephemerella did not). The authors also showed that mayflies may be able to sense their own chemical stimuli, but do not respond unless other cues are also present (e.g. in the case of Baetis).

Adults

It is generally accepted that mayfly adults do not feed, although intake of water may occur (Takemon 1993), and they usually live for a very short period of time. Mating, oviposition and, in some cases, dispersal are the main functions of this stage. Unfortunately, the sensory mechanisms involved in these behaviors have been scarcely studied. Vision, by means of positive polarotaxis (e.g. Kriska et al. 2007), seems to be the predominant sensory modality involved in mating (Brink 1956; Brittain 1982) and oviposition (e.g. Kriska et al. 1998) in the majority of the species. However, McCafferty and Bloodgood (1989) described a distinctive copulating system and its associated reproductive structures in Tortopus, speculating that females could use a pheromone to attract males. Their speculation was also based on the observations that Tortopus and Campsurus mate at night and males have relatively small eyes. Mating attraction was further studied to find that, although first perception of females by males was visual, non-volatile chemical substances might be important after close physical contact (Landolt et al. 1997). Until now, the use of chemical signals for sex attraction has been speculative in Ephemeroptera and the antennae have not been the sensory structures suggested to be involved in it.

Conclusion

Ephemeropteran nymphs are equipped with chemoreceptors that allow them to, at least in some species, sense predators and injured conspecifics. On the other hand, adults apparently are anosmic (e.g. they lack a glomerular antennal lobe and mushroom bodies calyces) and all the current data indicate that they are visually driven animals. Thus, even though nymphs use chemical cues throughout their life stage, adults seem to be deprived of a chemical sense. These data and lack of chemoreception in adults begs us to ask why there is a loss of the chemical sense. Wouldn't it be advantageous for the adult to be able to select the best possible oviposition site (e.g. fish-free site) to ensure the success of its offspring? Or it may be difficult to find predation free areas, and so it would be more advantageous to spread the risk and oviposit in different locations.

Odonata

Antennal morphology and types of sensilla Nymphs

The order Odonata is comprised of the suborders Zygoptera (Damselflies) and Anisoptera (Dragonflies). Species of both suborders have been the focus of several studies regarding the sensory biology of these insects and although the visual sense has been reported to be the primary sensory modality involved in prey detection and studied in detail (e.g. Sherk 1977), the nymphs of some species are less dependent on vision for prey capture. The antennae of the most phylogenetic basal forms of odonates are usually thick and show little differentiation between the base (scape and pedicel) and the apex (flagellum; Needham and Westfall 1955). In the most phylogenetically derived odonates, the antennae usually are seven jointed like in Libellula depressa (Gaino and Rebora 2001), but can also have fewer segments (e.g. Epiophlebia superstes, Faucheux 2007) and are of the filiform type (i.e. slender, cylindric, and greatly elongated). Sometimes the number of flagellomeres differs from one antenna to the other in the same individual and ornamentations can be present on the whole length of the antenna, as in E. superstes (Faucheux 2007). Besides the sensilla described in other parts of the body of the nymphs (e.g. Pritchard 1965b; Bassemir and Hansen 1980), sensilla trichodea, filliformia, basiconica, coeloconica, chaetica, campaniformia, and ampulliformia have been observed in the antennae (Appendix 2).

Adults

Even though adults are visual predators with exceptional vision (Sherk 1978) and the antennae of these animals undergo regressive development during the final molt (Needham and Westfall 1955), the antennal structures seem to be functional in the adult stage. In a comparative study of six species of zygopterans and 11 species of anisopterans, Slifer and Sekhon (1972) reported that the length of the antennae could be as short as 0.6 mm (in Argia fumipennis, Zygoptera) and as long as 2.1 mm (in Anax junius, Anisoptera); and they also reported that the flagellum of Zygoptera is strongly sculptured and undivided while the flagellum of Anisoptera is relatively smooth and composed of 2–5 segments. Among the sensilla described in several species are: ampulliformia, coeloconica, styloconica, and campaniformia (Appendix 2).

Brain morphology

Larvae have been almost neglected when it comes to the brain morphology and physiology associated with olfaction (for a review on other aspects of Odonata neurobiology see Mill 1982). Svidersky and Plotnikova (2004) describe the structural and functional organization of the mushroom bodies in last instar larvae, but they do so because the central nervous system is basically identical to that of the imago. Besides this report, only Plotnikova and Isavnina (2006) have studied the input of the antennal nerve to the brain in last instar nymphs of Aeshna sp. These authors found that the antennal nerve is connected to the lateral lobe of the protocerebrum and that the arborizations of such neurons are similar to those found in glomerular antennal tracts of Musca domestica. This result indicates that the lateral protocerebrum might at least be partly involved in the same type of processing that the antennal area is in other insects.

Adults are considered to be anosmic and they lack defined antennal lobes and calyces in the mushroom bodies. Nevertheless, the mushroom bodies are massive structures (Strausfeld 2009) and have been shown to receive afferents from the optic lobes (Svidersky and Plotnikova 2004). From the data available today, there seems to be little doubt about the poorly developed or actual existence of the olfactory system in adult odonates (but see Svidersky and Plotnikova 2006).

Behavior

Nymphs

Vision has been described as the most highly developed sense in many species of odonates, especially in visual predators like aeshnids and lestids, but others are tactile predators during the first nymphal instars or even throughout the whole larval development as in the case of Calopteryx virgo (Sherk 1977; Corbet 1999). The size and movement of the prey, in contrast to its shape, color, and odor (Tenebrio extract), have been observed to be important in stimulating feeding behavior (Pritchard 1965a). The use of mayfly and mayfly dummies (Rebora et al. 2004), other types of dummies (Etienne 1972), or immobilized tadpoles (Kanou and Shimozawa 1983) reinforced the idea that mechanical cues alone (Richards and Bull 1990) or mechanical and visual cues can elicit the release of the predatory labial strike. Although this mechanism has only been studied in Anisoptera, zygopterans presumably behave in a similar way.

As seen in Ephemeroptera, predator-prey interactions of odonates span a wide range (e.g. Caldwell et al. 1980) and in some lakes (usually in fishless lakes) these can be the top predators. However, odonates have been reported to have invertebrate (e.g. other odonates) and vertebrate (e.g. fish) predators in their natural habitats too (see below).

Dragonfly response to invertebrate predators

The main predators of odonatan nymphs are other Anisoptera, including larger conspecifics feeding on smaller ones (Corbet 1999), but other insects (e.g. aquatic heteroptera) also prey upon them. Predators can have important effects on mortality and growth of aquatic insects, including odonates (McPeek and Peckarsky 1998). Johansson (1993) showed that odonatan nymphs could detect and respond accordingly to the presence of an invertebrate predator. While the presence of large eyes might indicate visual stimulus to elicit an anti-predator response, several studies have shown that this is not the case. Ischnura elegans (Zygoptera) was able to detect the presence of the heteropteran predator Notonecta glauca in darkness by presumably using hydrodynamic or chemical cues, or even differentiate between this predator and a detritus feeding heteropteran (Corixa punctata; Heads 1985, 1986). Koperski (1997) found that the chemical cues of this same predator influenced prey consumption in Enallagma cyathigerum reinforcing the importance of chemical signals. When the behavior and hunting success of E. cyathigerum (Zygoptera) in the presence or absence of Aeshna juncea (Anisoptera) was studied, a marked response to visual and chemical cues from the predator was observed (Jeffries 1990). Also, Pyrrhosoma nymphula (Zygoptera) decreased its foraging activity when chemical stimuli alone and chemical and visual stimuli together of the predator A. juncea (Anisoptera) were provided (McBean et al. 2005). Furthermore, these authors demonstrated that predators fed with conspecifics significantly reduced their foraging activity, suggesting that this behavioral response occurs due to alarm pheromones released by conspecifics rather than by visual cues from the predator (Stoks 2001). This shows that vision is not the only important sense at least under these particular conditions. Hopper (2001) concluded that waterborne cues alone can cause Pachydiplax longipennis (Anisoptera) larvae to change their behavior in presence of different types of predators, and later on Mortensen and Richardson (2008) found that Enallagma antennatum (Zygoptera) foraging response is finely adjusted to predator/prey chemical signal combination (e.g. predator diet cues from Tubifex sp. and cues from injured Tubifex sp. elicit different responses). An even more interesting finding was that of the use of chemical and visual cues by small Plathemis lydia (Anisoptera) to detect larger cannibalistic conspecifics (Ferris and Rudolf 2007). However, these authors observed an opposite effect when compared to other studies, i.e. an increase in activity, spatial movement, and feeding behavior.

On the other hand, when I. elegans (Zygoptera) was presented with a caged anisopteran predator (Anax imperator) that allowed for chemical cues to be the main stimuli perceived, its foraging activity was not significantly reduced (Schaffner and Anholt 1998). Since a free-swimming predator did elicit a reduction in the feeding activity of I. elegans, the authors concluded that this response was probably due to visual cues. It is worth noting that this is the only paper that concluded that chemical cues are probably not involved and some authors have argued that the fact that certain animals do not perceive a high predator threat (like the presence of a caged predator) could explain the lack of behavioral response. For example, lamellae autotomy (i.e. the sacrifice of the lamellae to escape a predator) has been shown to influence escape behavior when nymphs were presented with fish kairomones (Gyssels and Stoks 2005, 2006). Thus, it may be the case that in addition to detecting the presence of a predator, these insects can evaluate the risk of being consumed according to the predator's spatial and temporal distribution. In addition, several authors also reported that the habitat background of odonates, i.e. coming from a fish or fishless lake, is a variable that has to be taken into consideration (e.g. McPeek 1990).

Dragonfly response to vertebrate predators

In lakes where fish are present, these can feed on several aquatic invertebrates, and odonatan nymphs have been reported to detect and avoid fish predators (Pierce 1988). Although this author suggested that probably visual and/or mechanical cues are the basis for predator detection, several later studies support a different type of predator detection mechanism in these animals. For one, E. cyathigerum responded to fish chemical stimuli by altering their feeding rate and diet composition (Koperski 1997). In other studies, the diet of the predator fish (i.e. if the fish was fed mealworms, damselflies, or fathead minnows) was shown to change the frequency of feeding bites, head bends, and moves in two Enallagma species (Chivers et al. 1996). Furthermore, the use of pike-naïve as well as pre-exposed to pike damselflies illustrated that nymphs can learn to identify predators through diet-related stimuli and that a single exposure to the chemical cues was enough to elicit the response (see also Wisenden et al. 1997).

Besides, as shown for ephemeropterans, the size of odonatan nymphs also seems to play a role in the response to fish (Dixon and Baker 1988) and even morphological changes (e.g. longer abdominal spines) have been observed to occur under predator selective pressure (Johansson and Samuelsson 1994; Johansson 2002; Johansson and Wahlström 2002; McCauley et al. 2008). For example, in Leucorrhinia dubia (Anisoptera), the growth of longer and wider abdominal spines due to the presence of perch waterborne chemicals (Arnqvist and Johansson 1998) seems to be an adaptation to life in a hostile environment. These spines may function as a defense mechanism or as a warning for predators, and demonstrate once more the importance of chemical signals in the lives of these insects.

Adults

Since newly emerged males are not sexually mature, they disperse before going back to the breeding sites. During this time adults feed until sexual maturity is attained. It is well known that adult odonates are visual predators and predominantly feed on flying insects according to their movement, size, and shape (Corbet 1980). It has also been established that at least some species of Anisoptera and Zygoptera possess a dorsal rim area in the compound eye that is sensitive to polarized light; indicating that orientation in their habitat and even flight directionality during migration could involve visual cues (Meyer and Labhart 1993). Besides vision, no reference to any other type of stimuli involved in hunting has been reported so far.

Mature males may defend their breeding territory to gain access to females and depending on the environmental and population conditions, both sexes have been shown to exhibit flexibility in their reproductive strategies (e.g. female passive choice in Irusta and Araújo 2007). Copulation can occur during flight or while resting and female acceptance may vary depending on male persistence to mate (e.g. Cordero and Andrés 2002). In addition to vision being the primary sense involved in mating, only a tactile recognition system involving the mesostigmal plates of the female and the inner surface of the male superior appendage during the tandem position seems to play a role (Robertson and Paterson 1982). No experimental data on the role of the antennae of adults in mating has been published so far, but due to their small size and lack of diversity and abundance of sensilla, chemical senses (at least involving the antennae) seem to be unimportant.

Conclusion

Odonates have well developed eyes, but being a primarily visual animal does not mean that other senses are not needed when exploring their environment. Nymphs have been shown to use infochemicals from a variety of predators and even learn to associate them with the presence of predators. In contrast, adults are considered to be anosmic (even though two types of antennal sensilla have been suggested to be chemosensory) and utilize vision for prey capturing and mating. Hence, once again it seems pertinent to ask what occurs with the chemical information that is so important in the immature stages, during the imago stage. Furthermore, if nymphs are able to learn, is this memory still present in the adult brain? As asked for Ephemeroptera, is the oviposition site decided according to the chemical information gathered during the immature stages? It is very interesting that both palaeopteran orders, i.e. Ephemeroptera and Odonata, have a chemosensory sensitive nymph but become anosmic as adults. The observation that these two palaeopteran orders drink water raises the possibility that water sampling might take place when choosing an oviposition site, implying that even though the adult loses the sense of smell, its sense of taste may be retained and employed during feeding and oviposition.

Plecoptera

Antennal morphology and types of sensilla Nymphs

A pair of filliform antennae, formed by a scape, pedicel, and a long flagellum, is present in the nymphs. For example, in Paragnetina media the flagellum possesses 76–81 segments; and the first segment, called meriston, is the product of the fusion of several segments making it larger than the rest (Kapoor 1985). Plecopteran nymphs require a variety of sensilla to obtain information regarding temperature, mechanical, and chemical stimuli from the aquatic environment. Sensilla trichodea, basiconica, coeloconica, campaniformia, coniform complexes, and an unrecognized type of sensilla were observed in several plecopterans (Appendix 3).

Adults

The adults possess long fine antennae consisting of several segments that are covered with short setae (Williams and Feltmate 1992). As in the nymph, the adult antennae have similar shape and consist of a scape, pedicel, and a long flagellum. In Allocapnia recta, the 4 mm flagellum of the female is 1 mm longer than that of the male and, as seen in the nymph of P. media, its first segment is partially divided (Slifer 1979). This species also presents a different number of segments in the right and left antennae, and no differences were found between sexes. So far, in the species investigated, sensilla trichodea, chaetica, coeloconica, campaniformia, and an unrecognized type of sensilla have been found (Appendix 3).

Brain morphology

Until now no report regarding Plecoptera brain morphology or physiology has been published. Even though both nymphs and adults generally have well-developed antennae, no data are available on the structure of the antennal lobes or other parts of the brain of these animals.

Behavior

Nymphs

The diet of plecopteran nymphs varies depending on the species, developmental instar, or time of the day (Harper and Stewart 1984). Some species are herbivores-detritivores, others are omnivores-carnivores, and yet several change their feeding habit during development. Although many species are predators, other organisms (e.g. fish) predate on them too, and similar interactions to those reported for Ephemeroptera also occur. However, significantly less information is available regarding the importance of the different sensory modalities in predator-prey interactions and food searching.

Stoneflies have been reported to use their antennae to locate prey (e.g. Hynes 1941; Brinck 1949) and their efficiency depends on whether foraging occurs on surfaces more exposed to predators or not (Kovalak 1978). In the presence of predators, the nymphs' avoidance response may change their foraging behavior by confining them to a “safer” substratum (e.g. dark substratum; Feltmate and Williams 1989) where food may not be as abundant. Even if prey is abundant, the presence of a predator may increase the metabolic rate of the stonefly and reduce the efficiency of food assimilation as suggested by Duvall and Williams (1995). While the sensory cues involved in avoidance of fish predators have not been studied (except to a certain extent by Martinez 1987), some researchers have reported on the cues involved in food detection. The fact that some nymphs (e.g. Perlidae) have activity peaks during the crepuscule, or when very limited light is available (Hynes 1941; Brink 1949), is in accordance with the use of non-visual modalities to detect food. However, there are many animals that are adapted to see in low light intensity environments. When K. modestus and M. signata foraged with their eyes occluded or their antennae removed, Martinez (1987) showed that the antennae, but not the eyes, were necessary for feeding. This author also showed that chemical cues associated with competitors and prey elicited the appropriate response to each cue, stressing the importance of chemical information for these insects. On the other hand, detritivorous stoneflies, like Pteronarcys pictetii, were also shown to utilize chemical cues (in this case gustatory cues) when discriminating among food sources (Motyka et al. 1985).

In contrast to these findings, other authors found that mechanosensory cues were involved in the detection of prey items. The result that the stereotyped escape behavior of the ephemeropteran Baetis, but not that of Heptageniids or Ephemerelids, was attractive to stoneflies (Peckarsky and Penton 1989b) was shown to be the hydrodynamic stimulus K. modestus sensed when attacking its prey (Peckarsky and Wilcox 1989). Furthermore, these authors used a plastic model simulating the mayfly swimming wave patterns to test the importance of mechanosensory cues and at the same time eliminated any possible chemical or “normal” visual cues (it is important to note that these results contradict those of Martinez (1987) obtained with the same species). Two other stoneflies (M. signata and D. cephalotes) were shown to attack their ephemeropteran prey after antennal contact, suggesting that also mechanosensory stimuli initiate this behavior (Sjöström 1985; Peckarsky and Penton 1989a). So as in other examples presented above, different species seem to have evolved different detection mechanisms for their prey.

Adults

Although the antennae of adults are well developed, to my knowledge no research has been reported on the sensory biology of these structures. Reproduction of these animals has been reported to be primarily by vibrational communication (e.g. Stewart 1997; Virant-Doberlet and Cokl 2003; Sandberg and Stewart 2006), and regarding feeding habits very little is known. Many short-lived species do not feed (but do drink water) and many long-lived species feed on the green encrusting growth of bark, rotten wood (probably for the fungi component of it), or even honeydew; and in several species the intake of food is necessary to produce eggs (Hynes 1976). However, nothing is known about the sensory modalities used by the species that do feed as adults when searching for this food. It would be very surprising if those well-developed antennae had been retained without being advantageous for the insect's life.

Conclusion

In several species, the antennae of both nymphs and adults are conspicuous structures that, at least in nymphs, have been shown to be involved in feeding and predator-avoidance behaviors. Until now, no data have been collected regarding the importance of odor-mediated adult behaviors, brain morphology, or sensory physiology. Nevertheless, assuming that adults can perceive chemical Stimuli (based on the presence of several possible chemosensilla), what morphological changes take place in the antennae and sensilla of the nymph after developing into an adult? Are the same sensory neurons connected to the same brain structures in both stages? What kind of brain reorganization occurs in the adult and how does this restructuration affect the animal's biology? Although much more research is needed, especially in the imago, Plecoptera is the only order of the three hemimetabolous orders presented here that can possibly answer many of these questions because both the nymphal and adult instars have well developed antennae.

Holometabolous orders

Two orders of holometabolous insects, i.e. insects that undergo complete metamorphosis, have been studied regarding their sensory biology, namely Trichoptera and Diptera. As mentioned before, even though several studies have concentrated on aquatic Coleopterans, these insects are restricted to an aquatic life both as larvae and adults. Thus, these are not included in this review and only Trichoptera and Diptera will be covered in the second part of this review.

Trichoptera

Antennal morphology and types of sensilla Larvae

In general the antennae of larvae of Trichoptera are not well developed and are represented by one or two apical papillae (Ross 1967). Denis (1984) reported that the basic structure of the antennae of several species was cylindrical with a large and rounded lobe, minute warts, and two tubercles in the middle. While in Nectopsyche albida the antenna consists of a short scape, long pedicel and a highly reduced flagellum (Tozer 1982), in Melampophylax mucoreus this has been described as a small peg positioned at the dorsolateral frontal edge of the head near the insertion of the mandibles (Spanhoff et al. 2005). In this last species, the tip of the single segmented antenna is laterally canted off and presents a plate structure, which could be interpreted as a multiporous plate sensillum by comparison to a similar structure in the larva of Homoeosoma nebulella (Lepidoptera: Pyralidae; Faucheux 1995).

Although other types of sensilla (e.g. basiconica and styloconica) have been described in the mouthparts of the larvae (Motyka et al. 1985; Spanhoff et al. 2005), only trichodea seem to be present on the antennae. Denis (1984) observed a single trichoid seta between the two tubercles on the medial side of the antennae of several species (except for Goeridae pilosa) and Tozer (1982) observed a similar structure in the apical portion of each antenna of N. albida. This last author suggested that the trichoid sensillum might be a mechanosensillum useful in locomotion and feeding activities as in Lepidopteran larvae. Thus, Trichopteran larvae seem to be deprived of antennal chemosensilla.

Adults

Quite opposite to what happens in the larvae, the antennae of the adult are very conspicuous and may vary in length according to the species and sex. For example, in N. albida the length of the whole antenna (i.e. the bulbous scape, relatively short pedicel, and long filiform flagellum) is about 32 mm in males and 16 mm in females (Tozer 1982), while in Frenesia missa the flagellum (probably the first two segments are very short) varied from 7–10 mm in both sexes (Slifer and Sekhon 1971). In the first species, sensilla are present on both sides of the flagellum and sexual dimorphism is also apparent in the size and shape of the scales, the presence of annulated sensilla in the male, and in the number of flagellar subdivisions (Tozer 1982). On the other hand, F. missa does not present antennal sexual dimorphism (but see sensilla description in Appendix 4) and the thread-like flagellum is divided into 45–50 subsegments in both sexes (Slifer and Sekhon 1971). In this last species, the flagellar subsegments are covered with microtrichia and large, flattened, sharp-tipped hairs (probably similar to the scales seen in Lepidoptera) are the most conspicuous structures. The following are the type of sensilla described in these two species (since two different types of classification were used by different authors, some of these categories could be actually the same): chaetica, campaniformia, squamiformia, thick-walled chemoreceptor pegs, thin-walled chemoreceptor pegs, thin-walled pegs in a depression, and a special type of thin-walled chemoreceptor called plate organs.

Brain morphology

Even though there is a close phylogenetic relationship between Trichoptera and Lepidoptera (Grimaldi and Engel 2005), and morphological similarities are known to exist, no information about the Trichoptera brain structure (e.g. antennal lobes, mushroom bodies, etc.) is available. Only two species of adult Trichoptera were mentioned in a paper on the phylogeny of a serotonin neuron in the antennal lobes of several insect orders (Dacks et al. 2006).

The extensive knowledge on Lepidoptera brain morphology and function and the available electrophysiological and histological techniques will most certainly prove to be very useful in future research in Trichoptera because of presumed similarities among these two groups. This will most definitely serve as a basis for comparison.

Behavior

Larvae

These insects are probably best known for the attractive caddis that some larvae build as shelters. Most trichopteran larvae feed on plant materials (although some are predaceous) and even though they are not very selective, they are greatly specialized for food acquisition (Wiggins 1984). Shredders have been observed to feed more heavily on leaves that are microbially colonized than on uncolonized ones, leading Motyka et al. (1985) to test for the response of larvae of Pycnopsyche guttifer (note that he also used plecopteran larvae of P. pictetii) to noncontact chemical compounds released by hickory and ash leaves. This species seemed to prefer colonized leaves after contact was already established indicating that prolonged arrestment on the chosen food might be triggered by gustatory cues. Later on Spanhoff et al. (2005) tested whether the antennae of M. mucoreus were involved in long-range food finding. Their results not only demonstrate that larvae with amputated antennae behave the same as those with intact antennae, but also suggest that contact chemoreception for identification of food patches may be achieved by sensilla in the maxillary palps and galea. Regarding predaceous larvae, experiments with Plectrocnemia conspersa show that vibrations of their irregular catching net (used to trap invertebrates) are transmitted to the larva and depending on the frequency, elicit feeding behavior (Tachet 1977).

Predator avoidance responses have not been studied so far. However, it may be the case that since a caddis protects some of the trichopteran species, these did not evolve a kairomone-mediated predator detection mechanism (suggested by Tachet 1977) as seen in other aquatic larvae.

Adults

Feeding habits in adult Trichoptera have been overlooked mainly because of the belief that they do not feed; but some species have been seen to feed on plant nectar (Crichton 1957) and functional mouthparts modified for sucking have been reported in six species (representing four families; Frings and Frings 1956). In contrast, Trichoptera mating behavior has received much more attention. In the search for exocrine glands that could secrete sex pheromones, Roemhild (1980) found secretory glands in the head and thorax of nine species of microcaddisflies (Hydroptilidae). Since these glands were observed only in males at the sexually active stage, these glands were suggested to be sex-pheromone production structures. However, Solem (1985), without finding the actual glands, demonstrated that the fourth abdominal sternite of Rhyacophila nubila was attractive to males. Exocrine glands were found to be associated with this abdominal sternite (Löfstedt et al. 1994), and Resh and Wood (1985) reported the presence of paired glands in the fifth abdominal sternite of Dicosmoecus gilvipes and two Gumaga species. The presence of exocrine glands was also found in Hydropsyche angustipennis, Rhyacophila fasciata (Löfstedt et al. 1994), Molanna angustata (Löfstedt et al. 2008), and in half of the 26 Trichoptera examined by Nielsen (1980). Interestingly, in all the species studied both females and males had a homologous gland system but its secretion was shown to be, at least in some cases, sex specific (e.g. Ansteeg and Dettner 1991). This suggests that these glands may produce compounds that have diverse roles (e.g. sexual in females and aggregational in males; Valeur et al. 1990 in Ansteeg and Dettner 1991).

Some authors suggested that caddisfly glands secrete defensive compounds against invertebrate predators (Duffield et al. 1977; Duffield 1981) and Ansteeg and Dettner (1991) found that some of these compounds had a very high toxicity for ants. Nevertheless, after Wood and Resh (1984) first demonstrated a chemically mediated sexual communication system in Gumaga griseola, many other reports showing similar results followed (e.g. Resh et al. 1987; Solem and Petersson 1987), suggesting a widespread use of this form of communication among sexes (see Ivanov 1993 for other basic communication signals between sexes). The timing of mate attraction and flight activity found in some species also reinforced these observations (e.g. Jackson and Resh 1991). Further confirmation came from electrophysiological experiments with identified compounds from the exocrine glands (e.g. Bergmann et al. 2004) that elicited significant responses in the male's antenna (Löfstedt et al. 1994, 2008; Jewett et al. 1996; Bjostad et al. 1996; Larsson and Hansson 1998; Bergmann et al. 2001). However, in some cases, females also respond to the active compounds (e.g. Jewett et al. 1996) and males also produce the active compounds that elicit the electroantennographic response in the male antenna (e.g. Bergmann et al. 2001). All together, these data point towards a less specific role of the Trichoptera exocrine glands when compared to Lepidoptera (Löfstedt et al. 1994). In Lepidoptera, males are usually the ones adapted to sense very small amounts of female pheromone compounds and thus, sexual communication is more specific.

Conclusion

While trichopteran larvae have very small antennae with only one type of non-chemical sensilla, adults have well developed antennae with a wide array of sensilla. In addition, several researchers have been investigating chemodetection in caterpillars and its contrast to the adult counterpart. Thus, it would be of interest to compare the extensive findings that have already been published on several lepidopterans with those of trichopterans. For example, does the fact that the larva is anosmic reflect changes in the brain structure and physiology of the adult stage? If so, how do these changes compare with those of lepidopterans? These questions can shed light on the encoding of chemosensory modalities and, thus, on the behavioral repertoires that these animals exhibit in their sexual communication.

Diptera

In contrast to the orders discussed before, the order Diptera has been extensively researched and mosquitoes, due to their role as vectors of human diseases have been particularly well examined (Clements 1999). The following section is simply a summary of some examples (mainly mosquitoes) that are relevant for this review. In contrast to adults, larvae have been the focus of fewer studies and for this reason more in-depth information is provided on the larval instar.

Antennal morphology and type of sensilla Larvae

The order Diptera is comprised of the suborders Nematocera (thread-horned flies) and Brachycera (short-horned flies). Of the nematoceran families with at least some aquatic larvae only the following have had their antennal sensilla described so far: Culicidae (mosquitoes), Simuliidae (blackflies), Chaoboridae (phantom midges), and Psychodidae (drainflies). The speciose Chironomidae (non-biting midges) and Ceratopogonidae (biting midges) have not been described so far. Although variation in antennae structure and sensilla occurs in these families, basic characteristics can be recognized (e.g. the presence of a cone organ in almost all the families). The general structure of the reduced antenna of the mosquito larva consists of a ring-like scape, fused pedicel and flagellum. In Culicidae, the antennae of Aedes aegypti (Zacharuk et al. 1971; McIver 1982; Gaino and Rebora 1999) and Toxorhynchites brevipalpis (Jez and McIver 1980; McIver 1982; Gaino and Rebora 1999) have been extensively studied. In these species, the antenna consists of a single tubular piece or a cylinder ending in a terminal membranous region where the rest of the sensilla are situated. Six types (10 in total) and five types (8 in total) of sensilla are present in Ae. aegypti and T. brevipalpis respectively; and in both species, a cone organ and a basiconic peg sensillum have been described. In Simulidae, the two-segment antenna is tubular with a membranous base that possesses a bacteria covered multiporous sensillum (Craig and Batz 1982; Gaino and Rebora 1999). Among the five types of sensilla that are present, the chemosensory cone sensillum is worth noting. In Chaoborus crystallinus (Chaoboridae), the highly modified prehensile antenna articulates on the anterior tip of the rostrum and consists of seven types of sensilla (Nicastro et al. 1995; Gaino and Rebora 1999). Lastly, Psychoda cinerea (Psychodidae) has multimodal receptor fields on the anterior part of the head and each one of these contains eight morphologically different types of sensilla (Gaino and Rebora 1999). More detail on the different types of sensilla is given in Appendix 5.

Adults

Although there are reports on the morphology and distribution of sensory receptors on the antenna of non-mosquito nematocerans (e.g. Cribb 1997; Felippe-Bauer and Bauer 1990), research has been strongly biased towards Culicidae. The structure and ultrastructure of the sensilla in the antennae of mosquitoes, as well as their electrophysiological properties, have been extensively studied and compiled elsewhere (e.g. McIver 1982; Sutcliffe 1994; Clements 1999). Due to their importance as disease vectors that afflict human beings, the chemosensitive sensilla in the antenna of female Ae. aegypti (e.g. Ghaninia et al. 2007; Ghaninia et al. 2008), Anopheles gambiae (e.g. Qiu et al. 2006) and Culex quinquefasciatus (e.g. Hill et al. 2009), as well as the odor ligands of the olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs) housed in them, have been particularly examined. Appendix 5 summarizes the adult sensilla of mosquitoes and their sensory modalities, but the reader is encouraged to go to the references mentioned in this summary (and the references therein) for a more detailed description on the topic.

Brain morphology

Mosquitoes, in particular Ae. aegypti and An. gambiae, have been the focus of studies on brain morphology and physiology within the Nematocera. Although limited information is available on the olfaction of the larvae (see below), extensive research has been conducted on the adult stage. However, only a summary of the adult's olfactory processes is provided.

While in Ae. aegypti, 49 male antennal glomeruli and 50 female antennal glomeruli have been described (fewer glomeruli were reported by Anton 1996) in the antennal lobe, in An. gambiae 61 are present in males and 60 in females. A detailed description on the neuronal architecture of the mosquito deutocerebrum (including the antennal lobe and the antennal mechanosensory and motor center) along with a partial functional map of the antennal lobe of the female mosquito is found in Ghaninia (2007). After the publication of the genome of An. gambiae (Holt et al. 2002) and Ae. aegypti (Nene et al. 2007), the identification of candidate odorant receptors (ORs) in these two species (as well as in Cu. quinquefasciatus) has been undertaken by several research groups. In An. gambiae, 79 candidate odorant receptors (ORs) and 76 candidate gustatory receptors were identified by Hill et al. 2002. Among these ORs, AgOr7 is a highly conserved receptor gene that is expressed in the majority of the ORNs, as is Dor83b in Drosophila melanogaster, and is supposed to be necessary for the functioning of other ORs expressed in ORNs (Pitts et al. 2004). After AgOr7 was characterized, orthologs in Ae. aegypti (i.e. AaOr7; Melo et al. 2004) and Cu. quinquefasciatus (i.e. CqOr7; Xia and Zwiebel 2006) were also identified. Moreover, the recent study of the ORs in adult An. gambiae has shed light on the specific responses of these AgOrs to biologically relevant odors (Wang et al. 2010; Carey et al. 2010) and also allowed for a comparison to D. melanogaster taking into account their different ecological needs (Carey et al. 2010). However, after the identification of pheromone binding proteins in moths (Vogt and Riddiford 1981), researchers realized that these proteins (more generally known as odor binding proteins or OBP's) bind and carry odor molecules to the ORs. Today, several OBPs have been identified in several species of mosquitoes (e.g. Ishida et al. 2002; Xu et al. 2003; Sengul and Tu 2008, 2010; Pelletier and Leal 2009) and in at least one case, the ligand established (Biessmann et al. 2010). It is interesting that even though the need for odorant transport is presumably unique to terrestrial animals (Vogt et al. 1991), OBPs have been found in Ae. aegypti (Biron et al. 2005), An. gambiae (Xu et al. 2003) and Anopheles stephensi (Sengul and Tu 2010) larvae.

The selective expression of AgOr7 in a particular tissue is indirect evidence of its chemosensory function that has been used to show that trichoid sensilla in the adult antenna and the distal part of the antennae in the larva are indeed chemosensory structures (Pitts et al. 2004). Interestingly, of the 23 ORs found to be expressed in the larvae of Ae. aegypti, eight are also expressed in the antenna of males and females (Bohbot et al. 2007). Xia et al. (2008) found that 12 ORNs labeled with AgOr7 antibody project into the sensory cone of the larva of An. gambiae and of 33 odorants tested, larvae responded to 11 (10 of which are aromatics and some products of organic decay). Moreover, some of these compounds (e.g. indole and 4-methylcyclohexanol) have also evoked strong electrophysiological response in the antenna of the female (Blackwell and Johnson 2000). Thus, as suggested by Xia et al. (2008), these compounds might be involved in larval development (by means of food detection) in addition to oviposition site selection in adults.

Behavior

Larvae

Nematocera species feed on an extremely broad range of plant, animal, and detrital material (Teskey 1984). Information on three families, specifically Chaoboridae, Chironomidae, and Culicidae is available regarding the sensory modalities involved in food searching and predator avoidance. Chaoborus has been reported to be predaceous and to probably use mechanical stimuli to detect its prey. Winner and Greber (1980) concluded that Chaoborus punctipennis most probably discriminates swimming prey items by detecting differences in water vibrations. In addition, experiments with a vibrating rod or probe determined the preferred distance at which a midge would attack and which frequencies and intensities are more likely to elicit such an action (Guiguère and Dill 1979). Since these experiments were done in the dark and the probe had no odor, the results point towards a mechanically-mediated hunting behavior. However, as in other immature insect stages, Chaoborids may change their behavior in presence of fish (Luecke 1986) and this behavior seems to involve fish kairomones. Chemicals produced by the three-spined stickleback Gasterosteus aculeatus affected the vertical migration of Chaoborus flavicans and this fish effect persisted for more than 15 days (Dawidowicz et al. 1990). In another experiment with C. flavicans, an increase in the tube depth (where these insects retreat) was observed when fish odor was added, but only in larvae that came from a fish lake (as opposed to those from fishless lakes); and the effects faded after some time suggesting microbial degradation of the chemical cues (Oda and Hanzato 2008).

Two reports have documented chemosensation in chironomid larvae. The first one showed that digging, burrowing, and foraging behavior changed after fish-conditioned water was added to a larval tank (Holker and Stief 2005). In these experiments, the response was seen 120 min after exposure and the chironomids were able to assess different infochemical concentrations which are probably indicative of different fish densities. The second set of experiments reported the first insect pheromone acting in an aquatic environment. Naik et al. (2006) demonstrated that the larval cuticle of the non-biting midge Chironomus ramosus contained farnesol (among other compounds) and that this “pheromone-like” compound had attractive properties for the larvae implying that farnesol is an aggregation pheromone.

Finally, included in the Culicidae family, several species of some subfamilies have been investigated (see some examples below). Within Toxorhynchitinae, Toxorhynchites amboinensis (predator of Ae. aegypti and others), have been shown to be attracted to water in which prey have been reared and also to the movement of living prey (Barber and Hirsch 1984). Using a vibrating probe, McIver and Beech (1986) observed that the 3rd instar larvae of T. brevipalpis attacked the probe in response to vibrations alone. These authors discarded vision as a factor because the compound eyes are nonfunctional at this stage (Sato 1961 in McIver and Beech 1986) and also chemical cues were discarded due to the fact that the probe is not associated with any prey odors. However, instead of proposing the antennae to be involved in the detection mechanism, they proposed that the main setae in the thorax and abdomen might be responsible for prey sensing, and Magnuson and Baerwald (1987) suggested that the hair sensilla on the head detected prey movement. Within Culicinae, one example of a detritivorous larva attracted to chemical cues from mucilaginous seeds was reported for Culex pipiens quinquefasciatus (Page and Barber 1975). Surprisingly predator avoidance has not received the same attention as in other aquatic insects, but the above evidence suggests that, as other aquatic insects, these have evolved to respond to a diverse range of stimulants.

Adults

Since the biology of the adult stage of mosquitoes is well studied (e.g. McIver 1982; Clements 1999), only a brief summary based on a review of mosquito sensory responses written by Clements (1999) supplemented with later citations is given.

Male and female mosquitoes need three kinds of resources, namely sugar from plants, mates, and resting sites; in addition, females need blood meals as a protein source and oviposition places. First, experiments done with Anopheles arabiensis and Ae. aegypti showed that mosquitoes could detect floral odors (Clements 1999) and green leaf volatiles (reviewed by Takken and Knols 1999) and also that these insects fly upwind towards the source (Clements 1999). Second, mating attraction at long distance has been shown to occur in response to sound stimulus, i.e. the male responds to the wing beat frequency of the female (e.g. Warren et al. 2009), but there is also evidence for a contact sex pheromone emanating from the legs of the female Culiseta inornata (Lang and Foster 1976 in Clements 1999; Lang 1977 in Clements 1999). Third, regarding host searching in females, chemical cues that have been shown to elicit attraction are: 1) expired breath (CO2 and water vapor); 2) substances secreted by the eccrine (sweat), apocrine (protein, carbohydrates and ammonia), and sebaceous (sebum) glands; 3) epidermal secretions and their bacterial decomposition products; 4) flatus; and 5) urinary and faecal associated contaminants and their bacterial decomposition product (Clements 1999). Besides this, aggregational pheromones released during feeding have also been suggested (e.g. in the sandfly Phlebotomus papatasi) to be involved along with other cues (e.g. Kennedy 1938). Lastly, oviposition preference in water where larvae occur (Ikeshoji 1966b in Clements 1999; Rejmánková et al. 2005) and electrophysiological responses in the female antenna when tested with larvae-water chemo-attractants have been confirmed (Blackwell and Johnson 2000). The presence of fish or tadpoles can also affect oviposition preference in gravid females (e.g. Petranka and Fakhoury 1991). These authors suggested that Anopheles and Chaoborus (phantom midge) females might chemically sample water with their tarsi while they are about to lay eggs. Focks and Hall (1977) found that female Toxorhynchites rutilus rutilus (predator of the larvae of Ae. aegypti) preferred to oviposit in water previously used to rear Ae. aegypti. There is also evidence for an oviposition aggregational pheromone for Cu. quinquefasciatus and Culex tarsalis used as an indicator of where egg rafts have already been laid (Clements 1999). Apparently, gravid females of these two species respond to a chemical that is released from droplets that become visible at the apices of the eggs soon after they have been laid. Since mosquito oviposition pheromones could be used to lure gravid females, much research has been focused on identifying these odors (e.g. Millar et al. 1992; Du and Millar 1999; Olagbemiro et al. 2004; Lindh et al. 2008) and on elucidating how mosquitoes detect them (e.g. Leal et al. 2008; Pelletier et al. 2010a, 2010b). All these data illustrate that, at least in Nematocera (but probably in many other insect orders), adults have evolved to search for optimal environmental conditions in which their offspring would be most likely to survive.

Conclusion

Mosquitoes have been extensively studied and present a unique opportunity to understand the connection between the aquatic larval stage and the terrestrial adult. Because both adults and larvae sense and use chemical cues (including some of the same chemistries) and the molecular biology has been worked out, mosquitoes are a good model to study changes in the olfactory system between an aquatic larva and its terrestrial adult. Although D. melanogaster is currently the best-studied insect, this model insect is not suited to answer questions about the sensory adaptations to aquatic and terrestrial environments. Recent data on the ORs and OBPs expressed in larvae and adult mosquitoes, together with the finding that some of these are shared by immature and mature stages, will be useful in answering the question of how adults may utilize information that is relevant during the larval stages. Furthermore, the fact that some chemical compounds were behaviorally important in the larva and adult, gives support to the idea that female mosquitoes may be sensing the water before ovipositing and by this ensuring a better environment for their brood (e.g. where more food or less natural predators exist). It would be interesting to see if females of other aquatic insect orders exhibit a similar behavior.

Discussion

On the one hand, chemosensation has been extensively studied in terrestrial adult insects (e.g. dipterans, lepidopterans, etc.) in terms of the external and internal antennal morphology and the organization of the antennal lobes and higher centers of the brain. On the other hand, the role of chemical cues in insects that inhabit an aquatic environment during either the adult stage (e.g. aquatic coleopterans and heteropterans) or the larval stage (e.g. ephemeropterans and plecopterans) has been studied to a much lesser extent. Besides expanding knowledge on the biology of one of the most successful animal classes, the study of sensory sensation in insects is relevant to understand how information is acquired, stored, and used to elicit particular behaviors. Animals perceive a subjective representation of the world as a consequence of a vast array of sensations ultimately resulting in decision-making. Even though these behaviors can be very elaborate, they must be based on the activity of neural circuits. Therefore, studying the animal brain helps researchers comprehend how biological neural networks process sensory information (see Chittka and Brockmann 2005).

The larval sensilla of holometabolous insects are present in the embryo and are typically replaced during metamorphosis by adult-specific sensilla derived from imaginal discs (reviews: Levine et al. 1995; Truman 1996; Tissot and Stocker 2000). In contrast, as exemplified by Rhodnius prolixus, every time hemimetabolous insects molt, the cuticular surface increases and new sensilla appear (Wigglesworth 1940 in Keil 1997). This may well be the case for plecopterans also, but the antennae of both ephemeropterans and odonates are reduced in size after the last molt. Since hemimetabolous insects do not undergo complete metamorphosis as holometabolous insects do, modifications in brain structures associated with change of environment, i.e. from water to land, could be first analyzed in these insects.

The divergence in the role of neurons seen in different species can arise from modifications in the connectivity of those neurons (Katz 2007). Neurons, as the basic units of a neural network, can be connected to elicit a particular behavior (e.g. upwind flight of a male moth in response to sex pheromones) or can be plastic in response (e.g. olfactory learning; Bargmann 2006). Since the physiological properties of neuronal dendrites largely depend on their morphology, characteristics such as size, branching patterns, and relative position of an arbor are indicative of the particular role the cell has (Williams and Truman 2005). In holometabolous insects, during metamorphosis, the central nervous system (CNS) undergoes major remodeling which includes: neurons that function in the larval CNS, but die during metamorphosis; larval-and pupal-born neurons that are only functional in the adult CNS; and neurons that function in both the larval and adult system (by dendrite and axon reorganization during metamorphosis; Tissot and Stocker 2000; Truman 1990). Since the physiology and morphology of neural structures in Lepidoptera has been extensively studied, by comparing these with those of Trichoptera, hypotheses about how well neural networks are preserved across phylogenetic orders and the selective pressure that impacts these nervous systems can be addressed. Katz and Harris-Warrick (1999) suggested that neural circuitries might evolve more slowly than behavior does, and thus, natural selection could modify the array of behaviors produced by a circuit by altering its inputs or by altering how it handles those inputs.

Even though, in holometabolous insects, the adult antennal lobe (AL) develops from a different brain area than that of the larval antennal lobe (Jefferis et al. 2004), at least 15 glomeruli of the AL of D. melanogaster adults are innervated by remodeled embryonic projection neurons (PNs; Gerber and Stocker 2007). A similar phenomenon was documented for γ neurons of the mushroom bodies (MB; Gerber and Stocker 2007) and a synchronized remodeling between these MB neurons and persistent projection neurons seems to occur (Marin et al. 2005). If these PNs and MB neurons have a similar function in both stages, this could also provide insights into the gathering and integration of chemical information in the insect brain. Moreover, the order Diptera presents a unique opportunity for comparison between mostly terrestrial larval and adult insects (i.e. flies) and dual-lifestyle insects (i.e. mosquitoes) in this regard. The rewiring of the nervous system, like the other alterations that take place during metamorphosis, occurs in an endocrine environment that has rising titers of ecdysone and 20-hydroxyecdysone (Brown et al. 2005) and has been shown to involve intracellular local and global changes of calcium (Williams and Truman 2005). Once the factors that influence the final form of a neuron are elucidated, our knowledge of how morphology and physiology relate to each other will allow us to understand the evolution of the CNS (see for example Stollewerk and Simpson 2005).

Finally, there are several invertebrate and vertebrate animals that are amphibious and are able to sense chemical cues in aquatic and terrestrial environments. For example, in the beetle Dytiscus marginalis, the same concentrations of coumarin and synthetic musk elicited a similar response in water and in air (Schaller 1926 in Jensen and Zacharuk 1991) and by recording from single sensilla, Behrend (1971) found similar results (in Jensen and Zacharuk 1991). Furthermore, behavioral studies in Laccophilus maculosus showed that in air, these beetles responded to about 10% of the concentration required to produce a similar response in water (Hodgson 1951, 1953). These results led Jensen and Zacharuk (1991) to propose that multiporous sensilla on the antennae of Graphoderus occidentalis are probably olfactory, but may also be sensitive to lower concentrations of chemicals in both air and water. This observation in addition to the finding that some OBPs are found in the antenna of the larva and adult stage of mosquitoes (Xu et al. 2003; Biron et al. 2005; Sengul and Tu 2010) might indicate that the sensing of odors in both media is based on similar molecular and physiological processes. Moreover, the nasal cavity of anurans reorganizes during metamorphosis allowing these animals to continue sensing waterborne odorants while also being able to sense airborne odorants (Belanger and Corkum 2009). This is possibly due to the expression of fish-like and mammalian-like receptor genes utilized in selective recognition of water-soluble or airborne odorants, respectively, in two different compartments of the frog's nose (Freitag et al. 1998). It would be of interest to compare the sensory mechanism of amphibious vertebrates with those of invertebrates and establish the morphological properties of sensory organs that allow for the same sensory modality in two different media.

This review has summarized many studies that demonstrate that many aquatic insects are adapted to sense chemical cues from different sources in their environment and to adjust their behaviors in response to specific cues. Chemosensation is a topic of much interest because of its complexity in stimuli composition when compared to other senses. Insects have been used as models to unravel the intricacy of biological neural networks and have proven to be very useful due to their simplified CNS. The physiological and neurological transformations that aquatic insects undergo throughout their lives are so extreme, that they may be a better model to understand how and why neural remodeling occurs. The vast array of molecular and genetic techniques used to study D. melanogaster will soon be available for mosquitoes too, and thus, a comparison between these two closely related insects will provide information about the developmental changes required for a dual lifestyle. Furthermore, Ephemeroptera and Odonata, being lineages of basal insect orders and having nymphs with chemosensory capability in contrast to that of the adult, may offer special cases to help us understand the evolution of sensory parts of the brain and the associated behaviors.

Acknowledgements

I thank C. G. Farmer, F. Goller, J. C. Martin, L. I. Terry, and N. J. Vickers for helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

Abbreviations:

AL,

antennal lobe;

CNS,

central nervous system;

MB,

mushroom bodies;

OR,

odorant receptor;

ORN,

olfactory receptor neuron;

PN,

projection neuron

References

1.

O Ansteeg , K. Dettner 1991. Chemistry and possible biological significance of secretions from a gland discharging at the 5th abdominal sternit of adult caddisflies (Trichoptera). Entomologia Generalis 15(4): 303–312. Google Scholar

2.

S. Anton 1996 Central olfactory pathways in mosquitoes and other insects. In: GR Bock , C Gail , Editors. Ciba Foundation Symposium 200 - Olfaction in Mosquito-Host Interactions , pp. 184–196. Ciba Foundation. Google Scholar

3.

G Arnqvist , F. Johansson 1998. Ontogenetic reaction norms of predator-induced defensive morphology in dragonfly larvae. Ecology 79(6): 1847–1858. Google Scholar

4.

JT Barber , MS. Hirsch 1984. Prey location in Toxorhynchites amboinensis (Diptera: Culicidae). Annals of the Entomological Society of America 77(1): 115–118. Google Scholar

5.

CI. Bargmann 2006. Comparative chemosensation from receptors to ecology. Nature 444(7117): 295–301. Google Scholar

6.

U Bassemir , K. Hansen 1980. Single-pore sensilla of damselfly-larvae: representatives of phylogenetically old contact chemoreceptors? Cell & Tissue Research 207(2): 307–320. Google Scholar

7.

RM Belanger , LD. Corkum 2009. Review of aquatic sex pheromones and chemical communication in anurans. Journal of Herpetology 43(2): 184–191. Google Scholar

8.

NV Belayeva , AP Rasnitsyn , DLJ. Quicke 2002. History of insects , 1st edition. Kluwer Academic Publishers. Google Scholar

9.

AC. Benke 1979. A modification of the hynes method for estimating secondary production with particular significance for multivoltine populations. Limnology and Oceanography 24(1): 168–171. Google Scholar

10.

J Bergmann , C Löfstedt , VD Ivanov , W. Francke 2001. Identification and assignment of the absolute configuration of biologically active methyl-branched ketones from Limnephilid Caddis flies. European Journal of Organic Chemistry 2001(16): 3175–3179. Google Scholar

11.

J Bergmann , C Löfstedt , VD Ivanov , W. Francke 2004. Identification and synthesis of new bicyclic acetals from caddisflies (Trichoptera). Tetrahedron Letters 45(18): 3669–3672. Google Scholar

12.

H Biessmann , E Andronopoulou , MR Biessmann , V Douris , SD Dimitratos , E Eliopoulos , et al. 2010. The Anopheles gambiae odorant binding protein 1 (AgamOBP1) mediates indole recognition in the antennae of female mosquitoes. PLoS ONE 5(3): e9471. Google Scholar

13.

DG Biron , P Agnew , L Marché , L Renault , C Sidobre , Y. Michalakis 2005. Proteome of Aedes aegypti larvae in response to infection by intracellular parasite Vavraia culicis. International Journal for Parasitology 35(13):1385–1397. Google Scholar

14.

LB Bjostad , DK Jewett , DL. Brigham 1996. Sex pheromone of caddisfly Hesperophylax accidentalis (Banks) (Trichoptera: Limnephilidae). Journal of chemical Ecology 22(1): 103–121. Google Scholar

15.

A Blackwell , SN. Johnson 2000. Electrophysiological investigation of larval water and potential oviposition chemoattractans for Anopheles gambiae S.S. Annals of Tropical Medicine & Parasitology 94(4): 389–398. Google Scholar

16.

J Bohbot , RJ Pitts , H-W Kwon , M Rützler , HM Robertson , LJ. Zwiebel 2007. Molecular characterization of the Aedes aegypti odorant receptor gene family. Insect Molecular Biology 16(5): 525–537. Google Scholar

17.

P. Brinck 1949. Studies on Swedish stoneflies. Opuscula Entomologica 11: 1–250. Google Scholar

18.

P. Brinck 1956. Reproductive system and mating in Ephemeroptera Opuscula Entomologica 22: 1–37. Google Scholar

19.

JE. Brittain 1982. Biology of mayflies. Annual Review of Entomology 27: 119–147. Google Scholar

20.

C Brönmark , L-A. Hansson 2000. Chemical communication in aquatic systems: an introduction. Oikos 88(1): 103–109. Google Scholar

21.

HLD Brown , L Cherbas , P Cherbas , JW. Truman 2005. Use of time-lapse imaging and dominant negative receptors to dissect the steroid receptor control of neuronal remodeling in Drosophila. Development 133(2): 275–285. Google Scholar

22.

HP. Brown 1987. Biology of riffle beetles. Annual Review of Entomology 32: 253–273. Google Scholar

23.

JP Caldwell , JH Thorp , TO. Jervey 1980. Predator-prey relationships among larval dragonflies, salamanders, and frogs. Oecologia 46(3): 285–289 Google Scholar

24.

AF Carey , G Wang , C-Y Su , LJ Zwiebel , JR. Carlson 2010. Odorant reception in the malaria mosquito Anopheles gambiae. Nature 464(7285):66–72. Google Scholar

25.

CC Caudill , BL. Peckarsky 2003. Lack of appropriate behavioral or developmental responses by mayfly larvae to trout predators. Ecology 84(8): 2133–2144. Google Scholar

26.

RF. Chapman 1998. The insects: structure and function , 4th edition. Cambridge University Press. Google Scholar

27.

L Chittka , A. Brockmann 2005. Perception space-The final frontier. PLoS Biology 3(4): e137. Google Scholar

28.

DP Chivers , BD Wisenden , RJF. Smith 1996. Damselfly larvae learn to recognize predators from chemical cues in the predator's diet. Animal Behavior 52(2): 315–320. Google Scholar

29.

AN. Clements 1999. The biology of mosquitoes, Volume 2: Sensory reception and behavior , 1st edition. CABI Publishing. Google Scholar

30.

GC. Cook 1997. Ronald Ross (1857–1932): 100 years since the demonstration of mosquito transmission of Plasmodium spp.- on 20 August 1897. Transactions of the Royal Society of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene 91(4): 487–488. Google Scholar

31.

PS. Corbet 1980. Biology of odonata. Annual Review of Entomology 25: 189–217. Google Scholar

32.

PS. Corbet 1999. Dragonflies: Behavior and ecology of Odonata , 1st edition. Cornell University Press. Google Scholar

33.

A Cordero , JA. Andrés 2002. Male coercion and convenience polyandry in a calopterygid damselfly. Journal of Insect Science , 2:14, Available online,  http://www.insectscience.org/2.14Google Scholar

34.

LD Corkum , HF Clifford . 1980. The importance of species associations and substrate types to behavioural drift. Advances in Ephemeroteran Biology. Proceedings of the third Conference of Ephemeroptera , pp. 331–341. Google Scholar

35.

LD Corkum , PJ. Pointing 1979. Nymphal development of Baetis vagans McDunnough (Ephemeroptera: Baetidae) and drift habits of large nymphs. Canadian Journal of Zoology 57(12): 2348–2354. Google Scholar

36.

DA Craig , H. Batz 1982. Innervation and fine structure of antennal sensilla of Simuliidae larvae (Diptera: Culicomorpha). Canadian Journal of Zoology 60(4): 696–711. Google Scholar

37.

BM. Cribb 1997. Antennal sensilla of the female biting midge: Forcipomyia (Lasiohelea) townsvillensis (Taylor) (Diptera: Ceratopogonidae). International Journal of Insect Morphology and Embryology 25(4): 405–425. Google Scholar

38.

MI. Crichton 1957. The structure and function of the mouth parts of adult caddis flies (Trichoptera). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 241(677): 45–91. Google Scholar

39.

JM Culp , NE Glozier , GJ. Scrimgeour 1991. Reduction of prédation risk under the cover of darkness: Avoidance responses of mayfly larvae to a benthic fish. Oecologia 86(2): 163–169. Google Scholar

40.

JM Culp , GJ. Scrimgeour 1993. Size-dependent diel foraging periodicity of a mayfly grazer in streams with and without fish. Oikos 68(2): 242–250. Google Scholar

41.

AM Dacks , TA Christensen , JG. Hildebrand 2006. Phylogeny of a serotonin-immunoreactive neuron in the primary olfactory center of the insect brain. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 498(6): 727–746. Google Scholar

42.

J Dahl , BL. Peckarsky 2002. Induced morphological defenses in the wild: predator effects on a mayfly, Drunella coloradensis. Ecology 83(6): 1620–1634. Google Scholar

43.

HV. Daly 1984. General classification and key to the orders of aquatic and semiaquatic insects. In: RW Merritt , KW Cummins , Editors. An Introduction to the Aquatic Insects of North America , pp. 76–81. Kendall/Hunt Publishing Company. Google Scholar

44.

P Dawidowicz , J Pijanowska , K. Ciechomski 1990. Vertical migration of Chaoborus larvae is induced by the presence of fish. Limnology and Oceanography 35(7): 1631–1637. Google Scholar

45.

C. Denis 1984. Fine structure of case-making larvae (Trichoptera). Proceedings of the fourth International Symposium on Trichoptera , pp. 105–114. Google Scholar

46.

MW. Denny 1993. Air and Water: the biology and physics of life's media , 1st edition. Princeton University press. Google Scholar

47.

SM Dixon , RL. Baker 1988. Effects of size on predation risk, behavioural response to fish, and cost of reduced feeding in larval Ischnura verticalis (Coenagrionidae: Odonata). Oecologia 76(2): 200–205. Google Scholar

48.

PL Douglas , GE Forrester , SD. Cooper 1994. Effects of trout on the diel periodicity of drifting in baetid mayflies. Oecologia 98(1): 48–56. Google Scholar

49.

Y-J Du , JG. Millar 1999. Electroantennogram and oviposition bioassay responses of Culex quinquefasciatus and Culex tarsalis (Diptera: Culicidae) to chemicals in odors from Bermuda grass infussions. Journal of Medical Entomology 36(2): 158–166. Google Scholar

50.

RM. Duffield 1981. 2-Nonanol in the exocrine secretion of the nearctic caddisfly, Rhyacophila fuscula (Walker) (Rhyacophilidae: Trichoptera). Proceedings of the Entomological Society of Washington 83(1): 60–63. Google Scholar

51.

RM Duffield , MS Blum , JB Wallace , HA Lloyd , FE. Regnier 1977. Chemistry of the defensive secretion of the caddisfly Pycnopsyche scabripennis (Trichoptera: Limnephilidae). Journal of Chemical Ecology 3(6): 649–656. Google Scholar

52.

CJ Duvall , DD. Williams 1995. Individuality in the growth of stonefly nymphs in response to stress from a predator. Archiv für Hydrobiologie 133(3): 273–286. Google Scholar

53.

GFJ. Edmunds 1984. Ephemeroptera. In: RW Merritt , KW Cummins , Editors. An Introduction to the Aquatic Insects of North America , pp. 94–125. Kendall/Hunt Publishing Company. Google Scholar

54.

GFJ Edmunds , JC Jensen , L. Berner 1976. The mayflies of North and Central America , 1st edition. University of Minesota Press. Google Scholar

55.

MS Engel , DA. Grimaldi 2004. New light shed on the oldest insect. Nature 427(6975): 627–630. Google Scholar

56.

AS. Etienne 1972. The behaviour of the dragonfly larva Aeschna cyanea M. after a short presentation of a prey. Animal Behavior 20(4): 724–731. Google Scholar

57.

MJ. Faucheux 1995. Sensilla on the antennae, mouthparts, tarsi and ovipositor of the sunflower moth, Homoeosoma electellum (Hulster) (Lepidoptera, Pyralidae): a scanning electron microscopic study. Annales des Sciences naturelles, Zoologie 16(4): 121–136. Google Scholar

58.

MJ. Faucheux 2007. Multiporous and aporous sensilla on the larval antennae of the relict dragonfly Epiophlebia superstes (Selys, 1889) (Odonata: Anisozygoptera: Epiophlebiidae). Entomologie 77: 121–128. Google Scholar

59.

ML Felippe-Bauer , PG. Bauer 1990. Sensilla ampullacea on the antennae of Culicoides paraensis (Goeldi, 1905) with notes on other Culicoides (Diptera: Ceratopogonidae). Memorias do Instituto Oswaldo Cruz 85(2): 235–237. Google Scholar

60.

BW Feltmate , DD. Williams 1989. Influence of rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss) on density and feeding behaviour of a perlid stonefly. Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 46(9): 1575–1580. Google Scholar

61.

G Ferris , VHW. Rudolf 2007. Responses of larval dragonflies to conspecific and heterospecific predator cues. Ecological Entomology 32(3): 283–288. Google Scholar

62.

AS. Flecker 1992. Fish predation and the evolution of invertebrate drift periodicity: evidence from neotropical streams. Ecology 73(2): 438–448. Google Scholar

63.

DA Focks , DW Hall , JA. Seawright 1977. Laboratory colonization and biological observations of Toxorhynchites rutilus rutilus. Mosquito News 37(4): 751–755. Google Scholar

64.

GE. Forrester 1994. Diel patterns of drift by five species of mayfly at different levels of fish predation. Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 51(11): 2549–2557. Google Scholar

65.

J Freitag , G Ludwing , I Andreini , P Rössler , H. Breer 1998. Olfactory receptors in aquatic and terrestrial vertebrates. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 183(5): 635–650. Google Scholar

66.

H Frings , M. Frings 1956. The location of contact chemoreceptors sensitive to sucrose solutions in adult trichoptera. Biology of Bulletin 111(1): 92–100. Google Scholar

67.

E Gaino , M. Rebora 1996. Fine structure of flat-tipped antennal sensilla in three species of mayflies (Ephemeroptera). Invertebrate Biology 115(2): 145–149. Google Scholar

68.

E Gaino , M. Rebora 1997. Antennal cuticular sensilla in some mayflies (Ephemeroptera). In: P Landolt , M Sartori , Editors. Ephemeroptera and Plecoptera: Biology-Ecology-Systematics , pp. 317–325. 12th International symposium on Plecoptera, Lausanne Switzerland, 1995. Google Scholar

69.

E Gaino , M. Rebora 1998. Ultrastructure of the antennal sensilla of the mayfly Baetis rhodani (Pictet) (Ephemeroptera: Baetidae). International Journal of Insect Morphology and Embryology 27(2): 143–149. Google Scholar

70.

E Gaino , M. Rebora 1999. Larval antennal sensilla in water-living insects. Microscopy Research and Technique 47(6): 440–457. Google Scholar

71.

E Gaino , M. Rebora 2001. Apical antennal sensilla in nymphs of Libellula depressa (Odonata: Libellulidae). Invertebrate Biology 120(2): 162–169. Google Scholar

72.

MW Gaunt , MA. Miles 2002. An insect molecular clock dates the origin of the insects and accords with palaeontological and biogeographic landmarks. Molecular Biology and Evolution 19(5): 748–761. Google Scholar

73.

B Gerber , RF. Stocker 2007. The Drosophila larva as a model for studying chemosensation and chemosensory learning: a review. Chemical Senses 32(1): 65–89. Google Scholar

74.

M. Ghaninia 2007. Olfaction in mosquitoes: Neuroanatomy and Electrophysiology of the olfactory system. Ph.D. thesis. Faculty of Landscape Planning, Horticulture and Agricultural Science - Department of Plant Protection Biology, Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences. Google Scholar

75.

M Ghaninia , R Ignell , BS. Hansson 2007. Functiional classification and central nervous projections of olfactory receptor neurons housed in antennal trichoid sensilla of female yellow fever mosquitoes, Aedes aegypti. European Journal of Neuroscience 26(6): 1611–1623. Google Scholar

76.

M Ghaninia , M Larsson , BS Hansson , R. Ignell 2008. Natural odor ligands for olfactory receptor neurons of the female mosquito Aedes aegypti: use of gas chromatography-linked single sensillum recordings. The Journal of Experimental Biology 211(18): 3020–3027. Google Scholar

77.

LA Giguère , LM. Dill 1979. The predatory response of Chaoborus larvae to acoustic stimuli, and the acoustic characteristics of their prey. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 50(2): 113–123. Google Scholar

78.

DA Grimaldi , MS. Engel 2005. Evolution of the insects , 1st edition. Cambridge University Press. Google Scholar

79.

S. Gupta 1998. External morphology of the antennal sensilla of the imago of Cloeon sp. (Ephemeroptera: Baetidae) by scanning electron microscopy. Journal of animal morphology and physiology 45(1–2): 142–144. Google Scholar

80.

FGM Gyssels , R. Stoks 2005. Threat-sensitive responses to predator attacks in a damselfly. Ethology 111(4): 411–423. Google Scholar

81.

FGM Gyssels , R. Stoks 2006. Behavioral responses to fish kairomones and autotomy in a damselfly. Journal of Ethology 24(1): 79–83. Google Scholar

82.

PP Harper , KW. Stewart 1984. Plecoptera. In: RW Merritt , KW Cummins , Editors. An Introduction to the Aquatic Insects of North America , pp. 182–230. Kendall/Hunt Publishing Company. Google Scholar

83.

PA. Heads 1985. The effect of invertebrate and vertebrate predators on the foraging movements of Ischnura elegans larvae (Odonata: Zygoptera). Freshwater Biology 15(5): 559–571. Google Scholar

84.

PA. Heads 1986. The costs of reduced feeding due to predator avoidance: potential effects on growth and fitness in Ischnura elegans larvae (Odonata: Zygoptera). Ecological Entomology 11(4): 369–377. Google Scholar

85.

JG Hildebrand , GM. Shepherd 1997. Mechanisms of olfactory discrimination: converging evidence for common principles across phyla. Annual Review of Neuroscience 20: 595–631. Google Scholar

86.

CA Hill , AN Fox , RJ Pitts , LB Kent , PL Tan , MA Chrystal , et al. 2002. G protein-coupled receptors in Anopheles gambiae. Science 298(5591): 176–178. Google Scholar

87.

SH Hill , BS Hansson , R. Ignell 2009. Characterization of antennal trichoid sensilla from female southern house mosquito, Culex quinquefasciatus Say. Chemical Senses 34(3): 231–252. Google Scholar

88.

ES. Hodgson 1951. Reaction thresholds of an aquatic beetle, Laccophilus maculosus Germ., to salts and alcohols. Physiological Zoology 24(2): 131–140. Google Scholar

89.

ES. Hodgson 1953. A study of chemoreception in aqueous and gas phases. Biology Bulletin 105(1): 115–127. Google Scholar

90.

F Hölker , P. Stief 2005. Adaptive behaviour of chironomid larvae (Chironomus riparius) in response to chemical stimuli from predators and resource density. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 58(3): 256–263. Google Scholar

91.

RA Holt , GM Subramanian , A Halpern , GG Sutton , R Charlab , DR Nusskern , et al. 2002. The genome sequence of the malaria mosquito Anopheles gambiae. Science 298(5591): 129–149. Google Scholar

92.

KR. Hopper 2001. Flexible antipredator behavior in a dragonfly species that coexists with different predator types. Oikos 93(3): 470–476. Google Scholar

93.

A Huhta , T Muotka , A Juntunen , M. Yrjönen 1999. Behavioural interactions in stream food webs: the case of drift-feeding fish, predatory invertebrates and grazing mayflies. Journal of Animal Ecology 68(5): 917–927. Google Scholar

94.

AD Huryn , DP. Chivers 1999. Contrasting behavioral responses by detritivorous and predatory mayflies to chemicals released by injured conspecifics and their predators. Journal of chemical Ecology 25(12): 2729–2740. Google Scholar

95.

HBN. Hynes 1941. The taxonomy and ecology of the nymphs of British Plecoptera with notes on the adults and eggs. Transactions of the Royal Entomological Society of London 91(10): 459–557. Google Scholar

96.

HBN. Hynes 1976. Biology of Plecoptera. Annual Review of Entomology 21: 135–153. Google Scholar

97.

JB Irusta , A. Araújo 2007. Reproductive tactics of sexes and fitness in the dragonfly, Diastatops obscura. Journal of Insect Science 7:24, Available online,  http://www.insectscience.org/7.24Google Scholar

98.

Y Ishida , AJ Cornel , WS. Leal 2002. Identification and cloning of a female antenna-specific odorant-binding protein in the mosquito Culex quinquefasciatus. Journal of Chemical Ecology 28(4): 867–871. Google Scholar

99.

VD. Ivanov 1993. Principles of sexual communication in caddisflies (Insecta, Trichoptera). In: K Wiese , Editor. Sensory Systems of Arthropods , pp. 609–626. Google Scholar

100.

JK Jackson , VH. Resh 1991. Periodicity in mate attraction and flight activity of three species of caddisflies (Trichoptera). Journal of the North American Benthological Society 10(2): 198–209. Google Scholar

101.

GS Jefferis , RM Vyas , D Berdnik , A Ramaekers , RF Stocker , NK Tanaka , K Ito , L. Luo 2004. Developmental origin of wiring specificity in the olfactory system of Drosophila. Development 131(1): 117–130. Google Scholar

102.

M. Jeffries 1990. Interspecific differences in movement and hunting success in damselfly larvae (Zygoptera: Insecta): responses to prey availability and predation threat. Freshwater Biology 23(2): 191–196. Google Scholar

103.

JC Jensen , RY. Zacharuk 1991. The fine structure of the multiporous sensilla on the antenna of the diving beetle Graphoderus occidentalis horn (Coleoptera: Dytiscidae). Canadian Journal of Zoology 70(4): 825–832. Google Scholar

104.

DK Jewett , DL Brigham , LB. Bjostad 1996. Hesperophylax occidentalis (Trichoptera: Limnephilidae): electroantennogram structure-activity study of sex pheromone component 6-methylnonan-3-one. Journal of chemical Ecology 22(1): 123–137. Google Scholar

105.

SGJr. Jewett 1963. A stonefly aquatic in the adult stage. Science 139(3554): 484–485. Google Scholar

106.

DH Jez , SB. McIver 1980. Fine structure of antennal sensilla of larval Toxorhynchites brevipalpis theobald (Diptera: Culicidae). International Journal of Insect Morphology and Embryology 9(2): 147–159. Google Scholar

107.

F. Johansson 1993. Effects of prey type, prey density and predator presence on behaviour and predation risk in a larval damselfly. Oikos 68(3): 481–489. Google Scholar

108.

F. Johansson 2002. Reaction norms and production costs of predator-induced morphological defenses in a larval dragonfly (Leucorrhinia dubia: Odonata). Canadian Journal of Zoology 80(5): 944–950. Google Scholar

109.

F Johansson , L. Samuelsson 1994. Fish-induced variation in abdominal spine length of Leucorrhinia dubia (Odonata) larvae? Oecologia 100(1–2): 74–79. Google Scholar

110.

F Johansson , E. Wahlström 2002. Induced morphological defence: evidence from wholelake manipulation experiments. Canadian Journal of Zoology 80(2): 199–206. Google Scholar

111.

M Kanou , T. Shimozawa 1983. The elicitation of the predatory labial strike of dragonfly larvae in response to a purely mechanical stimulus. Journal of Experimental Biology 107(1): 391–404. Google Scholar

112.

NN. Kapoor 1985. External morphology and distribution of the antennal sensilla of the stonefly, Paragnetina media (Walker) (Plecoptera: Perlidae). International Journal of Insect Morphology and Embryology 14(5): 273–280. Google Scholar

113.

DR Kashian , RE Zuellig , KA Mitchell , WH. Clements 2007. The cost of tolerance: sensitivity of stream benthic communities to UV-B and metals. Ecological Applications 17(2): 365–375. Google Scholar

114.

PS. Katz 2007. Evolution and development of neural circuits in invertebrates. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 17(1): 59–64. Google Scholar

115.

PS Katz , RM. Harris-Warrick 1999. The evolution of neuronal circuits underlying species-specific behavior. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 9(5): 628–633. Google Scholar

116.

TA. Keil 1997. Comparative morphogenesis of sensilla: a review. International Journal of Insect Morphology and Embryology 26(3–4): 151–160. Google Scholar

117.

JS. Kennedy 1938. The visual responses of flying mosquitoes. Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London 109(4): 221–242. Google Scholar

118.

SL Kohler , MA. McPeek 1989. Predation risk and the foraging behavior of competing stream insects. Ecology 70(6): 1811–1825. Google Scholar

119.

P. Koperski 1997. Changes in feeding behaviour of the larvae of the damselfly Enallagma cyathigerum in response to stimuli from predators. Ecological Entomology 22(2): 167–175. Google Scholar

120.

WP. Kovalak 1978. On the feeding habits of Phasganophora capitata (Plecoptera: Perlidae). The Great Lakes Entomologist 11(1): 45–49. Google Scholar

121.

G Kriska , B Balázs , G. Horváth 2007. Positive polarotaxis in a mayfly that never leaves the water surface: polarotactic water detection in Palingenia longicauda (Ephemeroptera). Naturwissenschaften 94(2): 148–154. Google Scholar

122.

G Kriska , G Horváth , S. Andrikovics 1998. Why do mayflies lay their eggs en masse on dry asphalt roads? Water-imitating polarized light reflected from asphalt attracts Ephemeroptera. The Journal of Experimental Biology 201(15): 2273–2286. Google Scholar

123.

L Kristoffersen , BS Hansson , O Anderbrant , MC. Larsson 2008. Aglomerular hemipteran antennal lobes-basic neuroanatomy of a small nose. Chemical Senses 33(9): 771–778. Google Scholar

124.

J. Lancaster 1990. Predation and drift of lotic macroinvertebrates during colonization. Oecologia 85(1): 48–56. Google Scholar

125.

P Landolt , M Sartori , D. Studemann 1997. Palingenia longicauda (Ephemeroptera, Palingeniidae): from mating to the larvulae stage. In: P Landolt , M Sartori , Editors. Ephemeroptera and Plecoptera: Biology-Ecology-Systematics , pp. 15–20. Mauron + Tinguely & Lachat, SA. Google Scholar

126.

MC Larsson , BS. Hansson 1998. Receptor neuron responses to potential sex pheromone components in the caddisfly Rhyacophila nubila (Trichoptera: Rhyacophilidae). Journal of Insect Physiology 44(2): 189–196. Google Scholar

127.

WS Leal , RMR Barbosa , W Xu , Y Ishida , Z Syed , N Latte , et al. 2008. Reverse and conventional chemical ecology approaches for the development of oviposition attractants for Culex mosquitoes. PLoS ONE 3(8):e3045. Google Scholar

128.

RB Levine , DB Morton , LL. Restifo 1995. Remodeling the insect nervous system. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 5(1): 28–35. Google Scholar

129.

JM Lindh , A Kännaste , BGJ Knols , I Faye , A-K. Borg-Karlson 2008. Oviposition responses of Anopheles gambiae s.s. (Diptera: Culicidae) and identification of volatiles from bacteria-containing solutions. Journal of Medical Entomology 45(6): 1039–1049. Google Scholar

130.

C Löfstedt , J Bergmann , W. Francke 2008. Identification of a sex pheromone produced by sternal glands in females of the caddisfly Molanna angustata curtis. Journal of chemical Ecology 34(2): 220–228. Google Scholar

131.

C Löfstedt , BS Hansson , E Petersson , P Valeur , A. Richards 1994. Pheromonal secretions from glands on the 5th abdominal sternite of hydropsychid and rhyacophilid caddisflies (Trichoptera). Journal of chemical Ecology 20(1): 153–171. Google Scholar

132.

C. Luecke 1986. A change in the pattern of vertical migration of Chaoborus flavicans after the introduction of trout. Journal of Plankton Research 8(4): 649–657. Google Scholar

133.

LJ Magnuson , RJ. Baerwald 1987. Water Current Trichobothria on the larvae of Toxorhynchites rutilus (Diptera: Culicidae). Annals of the Entomological Society of America 80(5): 637–641. Google Scholar

134.

B. Malmqvist 1992. Stream grazer responses to predator odour- an experimental study. Nordic Journal of Freshwater Research 67(1): 27–34. Google Scholar

135.

B Malmqvist , P. Sjöström 1987. Stream drift as a consequence of disturbance by invertebrate predators. Field and laboratory experiments. Oecologia 74(3): 396–403. Google Scholar

136.

EC Marin , RJ Watts , NK Tanak , K Ito , L. Luo 2005. Developmentally programmed remodeling of the Drosophila olfactory circuit. Development 132(4): 725–737. Google Scholar

137.

LA. Martinez 1987. Sensory mechanisms underlying the predator-prey interaction between perlodid stonefly nymphs and their mayfly nymph prey , pp. 226. Cornell University. Google Scholar

138.

MC McBean , SA White , JA. McGregor 2005. Foraging behaviour of the damselfly larva Pyrrhosoma nymphula (Sulzer) in response to predator presence (Zygoptera: Coenagrionidae). Odonatologica 34(2): 155–164. Google Scholar

139.

WP McCafferty , DW. Bloodgood 1989. The female and male coupling apparatus in Tortopus mayflies. Aquatic Insects 11(3): 141–146. Google Scholar

140.

SJ McCauley , CJ Davis , EE. Werner 2008. Predator induction of spine length in larval Leucorrhinia intacta (Odonata). Evolutionary Ecology Research 10(3): 435–447. Google Scholar

141.

AR McIntosh , BL. Peckarsky 1996. Differential behavioural responses of mayflies from streams with and without fish to trout odour. Freshwater Biology 35(1): 141–148. Google Scholar

142.

AR McIntosh , BL Peckarsky , BW. Taylor 1999. Rapid size-specific changes in the drift of Baetis bicaudatus (Ephemeroptera) caused by alterations in fish odour concentration. Oecologia 118(2): 256–264. Google Scholar

143.

AR McIntosh , CR. Townsend 1995. Contrasting predation risks presented by introduced brown trout and native common river galaxias in New Zealand streams. Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 52(9): 1821–1833. Google Scholar

144.

SB. McIver 1982. Sensilla of mosquitos (Diptera: Culicidae). Journal of Medical Entomology 19(5): 489–535. Google Scholar

145.

SB McIver , M. Beech 1986. Prey finding behavior and mechanosensilla of larval Toxorhynchites brevipalpis theobald (Diptera: Culicidae). International Journal of Insect Morphology and Embryology 15(3): 213–225. Google Scholar

146.

MA. McPeek 1990. Behavioral differences between Enallagma species (Odonata) influencing differential vulnerability to predators. Ecology 71(5): 1714–1726. Google Scholar

147.

MA McPeek , BL. Peckarsky 1998. Life histories and the strengths of species interactions: combining mortality, growth, and fecundity effects. Ecology 79(3): 867–879. Google Scholar

148.

ACA Melo , M Rützler , RJ Pitts , LJ. Zwiebel 2004. Identification of a chemosensory receptor from the yellow fever mosquito, Aedes aegypti, that is highly conserved and expressed in olfactory and gustatory organs. Chemical senses 29(5): 403–410. Google Scholar

149.

EP Meyer , T. Labhart 1993. Morphological specializations of dorsal rim ommatidia in the compound eye of dragonflies and damselflies (Odonata). Cell & Tissue Research 272(1): 17–22. Google Scholar

150.

PJ. Mill 1982. A decade of dragonfly neurobiology. Advances in Odonatology 1: 151–173. Google Scholar

151.

JG Millar , JD Chaney , MS. Mulla 1992. Identification of oviposition attractants for Culex quinquefasciatus from fermented Bermuda grass infusions. Journal of the American Mosquito Control Association 8(1): 11–17. Google Scholar

152.

L Mortensen , JML. Richardson 2008. Effects of chemical cues on foraging in damselfly larvae, Enallagma antennatum. Journal of Insect Behavior 21(4): 285–295. Google Scholar

153.

GL Motyka , RW Merritt , MJ Klug , JR. Miller 1985. Food-finding behavior of selected aquatic detritivores: Direct or indirect behavioral mechanism? Canadian Journal of Zoology 63(6): 1388–1394. Google Scholar

154.

J Murlis , MA Willis , RT. Cardé 1990. Odour signals: patterns in time and space. In: KB Døving , Editor. Proceedings of the tenth international symposium on olfaction and taste , pp. 6–17. Graphic Communication Systems. Google Scholar

155.

DG Naik , AA Babrekar , BB. Nath 2006. “Pheromone-like” compounds in the cuticle of aquatic Chironomus larva. Chemistry and Ecology 22(6): 501–508. Google Scholar

156.

JG Needham , MJ. Westfall 1955. A manual of the dragonflies of North America (Anisoptera) , 1st edition. University of California Press. Google Scholar

157.

V Nene , JR Wortman , D Lawson , B Haas , C Kodira , Z Tu , et al. 2007. Genome sequence of Aedes aegypti, a major arbovirus vector. Science 316(5832): 1718–1723. Google Scholar

158.

D Nicastro , U Smola , RR. Melzer 1995. The antennal sensilla of the carnivorous “phantom” larva of Chaoborus crystallinus (De Geer) (Diptera, Nematocera). Canadian Journal of Zoology 73(1): 15–26. Google Scholar

159.

A. Nielsen 1980. A comparative study of the genital segments and the genital chamber in female Trichoptera. Kongelige Danske Videnskabernes Selskab Biologiske 23: 1–199. Google Scholar

160.

S Oda , T. Hanazato 2008. Diel vertical migration patterns in two populations of Chaoborus flavicans larvae (Diptera: Chaoboridae) in response to fish kairomones. Journal of Limnology 67(2): 93–99. Google Scholar

161.

PR Ode , SA. Wissinger 1993. Interaction between chemical and tactile cues in mayfly detection of stoneflies. Freshwater Biology 30(3): 351–357. Google Scholar

162.

TO Olagbemiro , MA Birkett , (Luntz) AJ Mordue , JA. Pickett 2004. Laboratory and field responses of the mosquito, Culex quinquefasciatus, to plant-derived Culex spp. oviposition pheromone and the oviposition cue skatole. Journal of Chemical Ecology 30(5): 965–976. Google Scholar

163.

CRI Page , JT. Barber 1975. Interactions between mosquito larvae and mucilaginous plant seeds. II. Chemical attraction of larvae to seeds. Mosquito News 35(1): 47–54. Google Scholar

164.

BL. Peckarsky 1980. Predator-prey interactions between stoneflies and mayflies: behavioral observations. Ecology 61(4): 932–943. Google Scholar

165.

BL. Peckarsky 1987. Mayfly cerci as defense against stonefly predation: deflection and detection. Oikos 48(2): 161–170. Google Scholar

166.

BL. Peckarsky 1996. Alternative predator avoidance syndromes of stream-dwelling mayfly larvae. Ecology 77(6): 1888–1905. Google Scholar

167.

BL Peckarsky , SI. Dodson 1980. Do stonefly predators influence benthic distributions in streams? Ecology 61(6): 1275–1282. Google Scholar

168.

BL Peckarsky , AR. Mcintosh 1998. Fitness and community consequences of avoiding multiple predators. Oecologia 113(4): 565–576. Google Scholar

169.

BL Peckarsky , AR McIntosh , BW Taylor , J. Dahl 2002. Predator chemicals induce changes in mayfly life history traits: a whole-stream manipulation. Ecology 83(3): 612–618. Google Scholar

170.

BL Peckarsky , MA. Penton 1989a. Early warning lowers risk of stonefly predation for a vulnerable mayfly. Oikos 54(3): 301–309. Google Scholar

171.

BL Peckarsky , MA. Penton 1989b. Mechanisms of prey selection by stream-dwelling stoneflies. Ecology 70(5): 1203–1218. Google Scholar

172.

BL Peckarsky , BW Taylor , AR Mcintosh , MA McPeek , DA. Lytle 2001. Variation in mayfly size at metamorphosis as a developmental response to risk of predation. Ecology 82(3): 740–757. Google Scholar

173.

BL Peckarsky , RS. Wilcox 1989. Stonefly nymphs use hydrodynamic cues to discriminate between prey. Oecologia 79(2): 265–270. Google Scholar

174.

J Pelletier , A Guidolin , Z Syed , AJ Cornel , WS. Leal 2010a. Knockdown of a mosquito odorant-binding protein involved in the sensitive detection of oviposition attractants. Journal of Chemical Ecology 36(3): 245–248. Google Scholar

175.

J Pelletier , DT Hughes , CW Luetje , WS. Leal 2010b. An odorant receptor from the southern house mosquito Culex pipiens quinquefasciatus sensitive to ovipostion attractants. PLoS ONE 5(4): e10090. Google Scholar

176.

J Pelletier , WS. Leal 2009. Genome analysis and expression patterns of odorant-binding proteins from the southern house mosquito Culex pipiens quinquefasciatus. PLoS ONE 4(7):e6237. Google Scholar

177.

WL Peters , JG. Peters 1977. Adult life and emergence of Dolania americana in northwestern Florida (Ephemeroptera: Behningiidae). Internationale Revue der gesamten Hydrobiologie 62(3): 409–438. Google Scholar

178.

JW Petranka , K. Fakhoury 1991. Evidence of a chemically-mediated avoidance response of ovipositing insects to bluegills and green frog tadpoles. Copeia 1991(1):234–239. Google Scholar

179.

CL. Pierce 1988. Predator avoidance, microhabitat shift, and risk-sensitive foraging in larval dragonflies. Oecologia 77(1): 81–90. Google Scholar

180.

RJ Pitts , AN Fox , LJ. Zwiebel 2004. A highly conserved candidate chemoreceptor expressed in both olfactory and gustatory tissues in the malaria vector Anopheles gambiae. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 101(14): 5058–5063. Google Scholar

181.

SI Plotnikova , IL. Isavnina 2006. Data in favor of possible olfactory function of the antennal nerve and lateral lobe of protocerebrum of larva of the dragonfly Aeschna grandis. Journal of Evolutionary Biochemistry and Physiology 42(3): 338–341. Google Scholar

182.

G. Pritchard 1965a. Prey capture by dragonfly larvae (Odonata; Anisoptera). Canadian Journal of Zoology 43(2): 271–289. Google Scholar

183.

G. Pritchard 1965b. Sense organs in the labrum of aeshna interrupta lineata walker (Odonata; Anisoptera). Canadian Journal of Zoology 43(2): 333–336. Google Scholar

184.

G Pritchard , MH McKee , EM Pike , GJ Scrimgeour , J. Zloty 1993. Did the first insects live in water or in air? Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 49(1): 31–44. Google Scholar

185.

YT Qiu , JJA van Loon , W Takken , J Meijerink , HM. Smid 2006. Olfactory coding in antennal neurons of the malaria mosquito, Anopheles gambiae. Chemical Senses 31(9): 845–863. Google Scholar

186.

M Rebora , E. Gaino 2008. The antennal sensilla of the nymph of Ephemera danica. In: FR Hauer , JA Stanford , RL Newell , Editors. International Advances in the Ecology, Zoogeography, and systematics of mayflies and stoneflies , 128: 307–312. University of California Publications. Google Scholar

187.

E Rejmánková , R Higashi , J Grieco , N Achee , D. Roberts 2005. Volatile substances from larval habitats mediate species-specific oviposition in Anopheles mosquitoes. Journal of Medical Entomology 42(2): 95–103. Google Scholar

188.

VH Resh , JK Jackson , JR. Wood 1987. Techniques for demonstrating sex pheromones in Trichoptera. In: MB Naud , Editor. Proceedings of the fifth International Symposia of Trichoptera , pp. 161–165. Junk Publishers. Google Scholar

189.

VH Resh , DM. Rosenberg 1984. The ecology of aquatic insects , 1st edition. Praeger Publishers. Google Scholar

190.

VH Resh , JO. Solem 1984. Phylogenetic relationships and evolutionary adaptations of aquatic insects. In: RW Merritt , KW Cummins , Editors. An Introduction to the Aquatic Insects of North America , pp. 66–75. Kendall/Hunt Publising Company. Google Scholar

191.

VH Resh , JR. Wood 1985. Site of sex pheromone production in 3 species of Trichoptera. Aquatic Insects 7(2): 65–71. Google Scholar

192.

SJ Richards , CM. Bull 1990. Non-visual detection of anuran tadpoles by odonate larvae. Journal of Herpetology 24(3): 311–313. Google Scholar

193.

S Richmond , DC. Lasenby 2006. The behavioural response of mayfly nymphs (Stenonema sp.) to chemical cues from crayfish (Orconectes rusticus). Hydrobiologia 560(1): 335–343. Google Scholar

194.

HM Robertson , HE. Paterson 1982. Mate recognition and mechanical isolation in Enallagma damselflies (Odonata: Coenagrionidae). Evolution 36(2): 243–250. Google Scholar

195.

G. Roemhild 1980. Pheromone glands of microcaddisflies, (Trichoptera: Hydroptilidae). Journal of Morphology 163(1): 9–12. Google Scholar

196.

HH. Ross 1967. The evolution and past dispersal of the Trichoptera. Annual Review of Entomology 12: 169–206. Google Scholar

197.

JB Sandberg , KW. Stewart 2006. Continued Studies of Vibrational Communication (Drumming) Of North American Plecoptera. Illiesia 2(1): 1–14. Google Scholar

198.

AK Schaffner , & BR. Anholt 1998. Influence of predator presence and prey density on behavior and growth of damselfly larvae (Ischnura elegans) (Odonata: Zygoptera). Journal of Insect Behavior 11(6): 793–809. Google Scholar

199.

GJ Scrimgeour , JM Culp , KJ. Cash 1994. Anti-predator responses of mayfly larvae to conspecific and predator stimuli. Journal of the North American Benthological Society 13(2): 299–309. Google Scholar

200.

MS Sengul , Z. Tu 2008. Characterization and expression of the odorant-binding protein 7 gene in Anopheles stephensi and comparative analysis among five mosquito species. Insect Molecular Biology 17(6): 631–645. Google Scholar

201.

MS Sengul , Z. Tu 2010. Identification and characterization of odorant-binding protein 1 gene from the Asian malaria mosquito, Anopheles stephensi. Insect Molecular Biology 19(1): 49–60. Google Scholar

202.

TE. Sherk 1977. Development of the compound eyes of dragonflies (Odonata). I. Larval compound eyes. Journal of Experimental Zoology 201(3): 391–416. Google Scholar

203.

TE. Sherk 1978. Development of the compound eyes of dragonflies (Odonata). III. Adult compound eyes. Journal of Experimental Zoology 203(1): 61–80. Google Scholar

204.

P. Sjöström 1985. Hunting behaviour of the perlid stonefly nymph Dinocras cephalotes (Plecoptera) under different light conditions. Animal Behavior 33(2): 534–540. Google Scholar

205.

EH. Slifer 1977. Sense organs on the antennal flagellum of mayflies (Ephemeroptera). Journal of Morphology 153(3): 355–362. Google Scholar

206.

EH. Slifer 1979. Sense organs on the antennal flagellum of a stonefly (Plecoptera, insecta). Journal of Morphology 160(1): 1–5. Google Scholar

207.

EH Slifer , SS. Sekhon 1971. Structures on the antennal flagellum of a caddisfly, Frenesia missa (Tricoptera, Limnephilidae). Journal of Morphology 135(3): 373–388. Google Scholar

208.

EH Slifer , SS. Sekhon 1972. Sense organs on the antennal flagella of damselflies and dragonflies (Odonata). International Journal of Insect Morphology and Embryology 1(4): 289–300. Google Scholar

209.

JO. Solem 1985. Female sex pheromones in Rhyacophila nubila (Zetterstedt) (Trichoptera, Rhyacophilidae) and arrival pattern to sticky traps. Fauna Norvegica, Series B 32(2): 80–82. Google Scholar

210.

JO Solem , E. Petersson 1987. Demonstration of female sex pheromones and adult behaviour in Molanna angustata (Trichoptera: Molannidae). Entomologia Generalis 12(2–3): 115–118. Google Scholar

211.

DA Soluk , NC. Collins 1988. Synergistic interactions between fish and stoneflies: facilitation and interference among stream predators. Oikos 52(1): 94–100. Google Scholar

212.

B Spänhoff , C Kock , A Meyer , EI. Meyer 2005. Do grazing caddisfly larvae of Melampophylax mucoreus (Limnephilidae) use their antennae for olfactory food detection? Physiological Entomology 30(2): 134–143. Google Scholar

213.

KW. Stewart 1997. Vibrational Communication in Insects: Epitome in the language of Stoneflies? American Entomologist 43(2): 81–91. Google Scholar

214.

R. Stoks 2001. Food stress and predator-induced stress shape developmental performance in a damselfly. Oecologia 127(2): 222–229. Google Scholar

215.

A Stollewerk , P. Simpson 2005. Evolution of early development of the nervous system: a comparison between arthropods. BioEssays 27(9): 874–883. Google Scholar

216.

NJ. Strausfeld 2009. Brain organization and the origin of insects: an assessment. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 276(1664): 1929–1937. Google Scholar

217.

NJ Strausfeld , L Hansen , Y Li , RS. Gomez 1998. Evolution, discovery, and interpretations of arthropod mushroom bodies. Learning & Memory 5(1–2): 11–37. Google Scholar

218.

NJ Strausfeld , I Sinakevitch , SM Brown , SM. Farris 2009. Ground plan of the insect mushroom body: functional and evolutionary implications. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 513(3): 265–291. Google Scholar

219.

JF. Sutcliffe 1994. Sensory bases of attractancy: morphology of the mosquito olfactory sensilla - A review. Journal of the American Mosquito control Association 10(2 Pt 2): 309–315. Google Scholar

220.

VL Svidersky , SI. Plotnikova 2004. On structural-functional organization of dragonfly mushroom bodies and some general considerations about purpose of these formations. Journal of Evolutionary Biochemestry and Physiology 40(6): 608–624. Google Scholar

221.

H. Tachet 1977. Vibrations and predatory behaviour of Plectrocnemia larvae (Trichoptera). Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 45(1): 61–74. Google Scholar

222.

Y. Takemon 1993. Water intake by the adult mayfly Epeorus ikanonis (Ephemeroptera: Heptageniidae) and its effect on their longevity. Ecological Research 8(2): 185–192. Google Scholar

223.

W Takken , BGJ. Knols 1999. Odour-mediated behavior of Afro tropical malaria mosquitoes. Annual Review of Entomology 44: 131–157. Google Scholar

224.

HJ. Teskey 1984. Aquatic Diptera. Part one: Larvae of aquatic Diptera. In: RW Merritt , KW Cummins , Editors. An Introduction to the Aquatic Insects of North America , pp. 448–466. Kendall/Hunt Publishing Company. Google Scholar

225.

P Tikkanen , T Muotka , A. Huhta 1994. Predator detection and avoidance by lotic mayfly nymphs of different size. Oecologia 99(3–4): 252–259. Google Scholar

226.

P Tikkanen , T Muotka , A. Huhta 1996. Fishless-stream mayflies express behavioral flexibility in response to predatory fish. Animal Behavior 51(6): 1391–1399. Google Scholar

227.

M Tissot , RF. Stocker 2000. Metamorphosis in Drosophila and other insects: the fate of neurons throughout the stages. Progress in Neurobiology 62(1): 89–111. Google Scholar

228.

W. Tozer 1982. External antennal morphology of the adult and larva of Nectopsyche albida (Walker) (Trichoptera: Leptoceridae). Freshwater Invertebrate Biology 1(1): 35–43. Google Scholar

229.

JW. Truman 1990. Metamorphosis of the central nervous system of Drosophila. Journal of Neurobiology 21(7): 1072–1084. Google Scholar

230.

JW. Truman 1996. Metamorphosis of the insect nervous system. In: LL Gilbert , JR Tata , BG Atkinson , Editors. Metamorphosis: postembryonic reprogramming of gene expression in amphibian and insect cells , pp. 283–320. Academic Press. Google Scholar

231.

M Virant-Doberlet , A. Cokl 2003. Communication with substrate-borne signals in small plant-dwelling insects. Annual reviews of Entomology 48: 29–50. Google Scholar

232.

S. Vogel 1994. Life in moving fluids , 2nd edition. Princeton University Press. Google Scholar

233.

RG Vogt , GD Prestwich , MR. Lerner 1991. Odorant-binding-protein subfamilies associate with distinct classes of olfactory receptor neurons in insects. Journal of Neurobiology 22(1): 74–84. Google Scholar

234.

RG Vogt , LM. Riddiford 1981. Pheromone binding and inactivation by moth antennae. Nature 293(5828): 161–163. Google Scholar

235.

OEJ. Walton 1980. Invertebrate drift from predator-prey associations. Ecology 61(6): 1486–1497. Google Scholar

236.

G Wang , AF Carey , JR Carlson , LJ. Zwiebel 2010. Molecular basis of odor coding in the malaria vector mosquito Anopheles gambiae. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107(9): 4418–4423. Google Scholar

237.

B Warren , G Gibson , IJ. Russell 2009. Sex recognition through midnight mating duets in Culex mosquitoes is mediated by acoustic distortion. Current Biology 19(6): 485–491. Google Scholar

238.

TF. Waters 1979. Influence of benthos life history upon the estimation of secondary production Journal of the Fisheries Research Board of Canada 36: 1425–1430. Google Scholar

239.

GB. Wiggins 1984. Trichoptera. In: RW Merritt , KW Cummins , Editors. An Introduction to the Aquatic Insects of North America , pp. 271–347. Kendall/Hunt Publishing Company. Google Scholar

240.

DD. Williams 1987. A laboratory study of predator-prey interactions of stoneflies and mayflies. Freshwater Biology 17(3): 471–490. Google Scholar

241.

DD Williams , BW. Feltmate 1992. Aquatic insects , 1st edition. Redwood Press Ltd. Google Scholar

242.

DW Williams , JW. Truman 2005. Remodeling dendrites during insect metamorphosis. Journal of Neurobiology 64(1): 24–33. Google Scholar

243.

RW Winner , JS. Greber 1980. Prey selection by chaoborus punctipennis under laboratory conditions. Hydrobiologia 68(3): 231–233. Google Scholar

244.

BD. Wisenden 2000. Olfactory assessment of predation risk in the aquatic environment. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 355(1401): 1205–1208. Google Scholar

245.

BD Wisenden , DP Chivers , RJF. Smith 1997. Learned recognition of predation risk by Enallagma damselfly larvae (Odonata, Zygoptera) on the basis of chemical cues. Journal of chemical Ecology 23(1): 137–151. Google Scholar

246.

JR Wood , VH. Resh 1984. Demonstration of sex pheromones in caddisflies (Trichoptera). Journal of chemical Ecology 10(1): 171–175. Google Scholar

247.

Y Xia , G Wang , D Buscariollo , RJ Pitts , H Wenger , LJ. Zwiebel 2008. The molecular and cellular basis of olfactory-driven behavior in Anopheles gambiae larvae. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105(17): 6433–6438. Google Scholar

248.

Y Xia , LJ. Zwiebel 2006. Identification and characterization of an odorant receptor from the West Nile Virus mosquito, Culex quinquefasciatus. Insect Biochemistry and Molecular Biology 36(3): 169–176. Google Scholar

249.

PX Xu , LJ Zwiebel , DP. Smith 2003. Identification of a distinct family of genes encoding atypical odorant-binding proteins in the malaria vector mosquito, Anopheles gambiae. Insect Molecular Biology 12(6): 549–560. Google Scholar

250.

RY. Zacharuk 1980. Ultrastructure and function of insect chemosensilla. Annual Review of Entomology 25: 27–47. Google Scholar

251.

RY Zacharuk , L Ru-Siu Yin , SG. Blue 1971. Fine structure of the antenna and its sensory cone in larvae of Aedes aegypti (L.). Journal of Morphology 135(3): 273–298. Google Scholar
This is an open access paper. We use the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 license that permits unrestricted use, provided that the paper is properly attributed.
José G. Crespo "A Review of Chemosensation and Related Behavior in Aquatic Insects," Journal of Insect Science 11(62), 1-39, (1 May 2011). https://doi.org/10.1673/031.011.6201
Received: 25 March 2010; Accepted: 1 September 2010; Published: 1 May 2011
KEYWORDS
antennal morphology
neurophysiology
odors
olfaction
sensilla
Back to Top