Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 August 2002 Male coercion and convenience polyandry in a calopterygid damselfly
A. Cordero, J. A. Andrés
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Copulation in odonates requires female cooperation because females must raise their abdomen to allow intromission. Nevertheless in Calopteryx haemorrhoidalis haemorrhoidalis (Odonata) males commonly grasp ovipositing females and apparently force copulations. This has been interpreted as a consequence of extreme population density and male-male competition. We studied this behavior at two sites on a river that had different densities over three years. As predicted, at high densities most matings were forced (i.e. not preceded by courtship), but at low density most were preceded by courtship. Courtship matings were shorter at high density, but density did not affect the duration of forced matings. Females cooperated in forced matings even if they had very few mature eggs. Furthermore, females mated more times if they experienced higher male harassment during oviposition, and at low density second and subsequent matings were more likely to be forced. We interpret these results to mean that females engage in “convenience polyandry”, because they gain more by accepting copulation than by resisting males. The results also suggest that females might trade copulations for male protection, because under extreme population density harassment by males is so intense that they can impede oviposition.

Introduction

Since Bateman's (1948) classical study, the difference in mating rates between males and females is explained by the strong contribution of additional matings to male reproductive success. In polyandrous insect species male fitness increases with mating rate. In contrast, female fitness increases with moderate mating rates but decreases at extreme mating frequencies (reviewed by Arnqvist & Nilsson, 2000). Due to this asymmetry between the sexes, males are expected to attempt to mate indiscriminately (i.e., persistent) while females are expected to avoid superfluous matings (i.e., resistant). Hence, sexual conflict over mating rate might be one of the strongest forces driving the evolution of male and female reproductive strategies (Parker, 1979; Bradbury & Andersson, 1987; West-Eberhard et al., 1987; Rice, 1996; Alexander et al., 1997; Hosken et al., 2001).

In water striders and several crustaceans sexual resistance by females fluctuates depending on different physiological and/or ecological conditions (e.g. Arnqvist, 1992; Rowe, 1992; Jormalainen & Merilaita, 1995; Choe & Crespi, 1997). These data have led to the suggestion that optimal female mating rates should be seen as evolving reaction norms- rather than as fixed optima (Arnqvist & Nilsson, 2000). If so, female mating rate is expected to be plastic and to covary with local environmental conditions. This covariation arises from females modulating their resistance (and therefore their mating rate) in response to several factors that affect the relative costs and benefits of mating. One possibility is that females adjust their mating rate to balance the costs imposed by male persistence (i.e. male harassment). This situation, described as convenience polyandry (sensu Thornhill & Alcock, 1983), assumes that males are able to coerce females to accept superfluous matings.

Male coercion has been demonstrated in a wide range of animals (reviewed by Clutton-Brock & Parker, 1995). However, the extent of convenience polyandry remains unclear. The best evidence for females modulating their mating rate as an adaptive response to male harassment comes from studies of water striders (Arnqvist, 1988; Rowe et al., 1994). However, in some other insect orders it has been argued that male coercion and convenience polyandry is highly unlikely. For instance, in odonates, the existence of male coercion is still controversial. Some authors have assumed that it is common (Moore, 1989; Koenig, 1991; Conrad & Pritchard, 1992) while others have argued that this assumption is not well supported (Fincke, 1997). Evidence against the existence of male coercion in odonates has been based on several behavioral and morphological traits. First, it has been argued that the reproductive anatomy of odonates blocks males from forcing copulations-because females must cooperate by curving the abdomen to contact male genitalia (Waage, 1973; Fincke, 1982; Waage, 1984a; Fincke et al., 1997). Second, some authors have assumed that females carrying no mature eggs invariably resist matings (Fincke, 1997). Consequently, it has been proposed that cooperation in communicating sexual intentions is common in this group, since it would pay the male to heed female rejection signals. Third, in species with pre-copulatory courtship, the non-aggressive signals by the male (i.e., displays) have been used to support the idea that coercion does not occur (Fincke, 1997).

In spite of these arguments, it is known that in odonates male harassment has a negative impact on female fitness (Ubukata, 1984; Waage, 1987), that males have the potential to coerce females and that forced copulations indeed occur (Cordero, 1999). These indirect lines of evidence suggest that convenience polyandry may be found as a female reproductive tactic in some odonates. Our main goal is to test the existence of convenience polyandry in Calopteryx haemorrhoidalis haemorrhoidalis, a model species that shows the typical reproductive behavior of calopterygids (i.e. elaborate precopulatory courtship,  Video 1 (i1536-2442-002-14-video1.mpg)), but also “forced” copulations without courtship (Cordero, 1999) ( Video 2 (i1536-2442-002-14-video2.mpg)). These copulations are termed “forced” because females cannot choose between accepting or rejecting being taken in tandem, and because a clear rejection behavior is always observed when males try to take a female in tandem (Cordero, 1999). For this purpose, we studied the reproductive behavior of this species under low and high population density over three years. Furthermore, these observations were compared with previous records made on the same population when the population density was higher (Cordero, 1999). This design, allowed us to make three different predictions. First, if males are able to coerce females then undesired (i.e. forced, not preceded by courtship) copulations should be common at high-density spots, where male harassment rate is the highest. In contrast, at low population density forced copulations should be rare. Second, if females copulate to avoid the cost of male harassment, they should sometimes mate multiple times per egg clutch, and females should mate independently of the number of mature eggs they carry. Moreover, the second and following matings should be coerced (not preceded by courtship). If females were to mate mainly to receive sperm from males with better genes or territories, the subsequent copulations should be preceded by courtship. Third, female resistance to forced copulations should decrease as male harassment increases. That is, there should be a positive relationship between the frequency of male harassment and the number of matings by females (Rowe, 1992).

Materials and Methods

Study organism

Calopterygid damselflies are large insects common in rivers that show elaborate precopulatory courtship ( Video 1 (i1536-2442-002-14-video1.mpg)), site fidelity and territoriality. All matings start with a male grasping the females prothorax with their anal appendages which is termed taking them in tandem. Forming a tandem may or may not be preceded by courtship. In most species, males defend a patch of oviposition substrate, court females and guard them from intruders during oviposition (see Heymer, 1972; 1973; Córdoba-Aguilar, 2000, for a description of territorial and reproductive behavior of C. haemorrhoidalis). Alternative non-territorial tactics (Gross, 1996) are found in most species (satellite or sneaking males), but these males obtain very few matings (Plaistow & Siva-Jothy, 1996). Females also show two tactics. Some accept mating and guarded oviposition by the resident male, but others simply start ovipositing near a male that is guarding another female without mating with the male (Siva-Jothy & Hooper, 1996).

Study site

We studied a population of C. h. haemorrhoidalis at the Forma Quesa river, Frosinone province, in Central Italy (UTM: 33TUF 8787) in August 1999–2001. Results are compared with those reported by Cordero (1999) for the same population. Individuals were netted and marked with an indelible pen on their wings (females) or thorax (males).

Due to the large size of the studied population and the characteristics of the study site, we could not manipulate male density around our observation sites, nor were we able to replicate our treatments at various sites. Observations were therefore repeated over three years on two different river sections, one of high and one of low density. Consequently our experiment is “pseudoreplicated” in time. Pseudoreplication often makes impossible to infer causal relationships. However, we believe that the main conclusions of our experiments hold because they are based on strong predictions (see Oksanen, 2001) and because we were able to use a natural experiment that modified the density of one of the study sites (see below). The first section (site A) was situated at the start of a pool of the stream, where the depth was about 1 m, and a patch of Potamogeton (3×0.5 m) was used as oviposition substrate. Males concentrated on the shore and established territories on the Potamogeton. The second section (site B) was immediately below a dam (about 3 m high) that created a large sunny area and a very large patch of Potamogeton (more than 10×3 m) in a sunny area on one of the riverbanks. Maximum depth at this spot was about 30–40 cm. Marked animals rarely moved between the study sites (4 animals out of more than 700 marked in 1999).

Population density and copulation duration

Site A had a very high density of animals in 1997, probably because it was the first sunny place after 500 m of river shaded by riparian vegetation (Cordero, 1999). The riparian vegetation was cleared in 1998, and this provided many sunny areas for male territorial activity. In 1999–2001 male density was clearly lower at this site, while density remained very high in site B. To test if male population density at both study sites was indeed different, we calculated the index of abundance as the number of animals marked per hour during August 1999–2001.

Several studies have reported that copulation duration increases at high population densities or when the sex ratio is male biased (see Cordero, 1990; Cordero et al., 1995; Andrés & Cordero Rivera, 2000, for odonates and Alcock, 1994, for a review). This might be interpreted as a male response to increased intra-sexual competition. However, if females have control over copulation duration, longer copulations might be interpreted as a female strategy to avoid male harassment (Rowe, 1992). Copulation duration was studied at both sites by direct observation in 1999–2000. Given that temperature has a negative effect on copulation duration (Cordero, 1999), we recorded air temperature every 20–60 min in the shade of a tree in 1999. In 2000 we used a datalogger that measured temperature every 10 min. Air temperature was highly predictable by time of day because all observations were done on sunny days (y= −0.346x2 + 10.844× − 51.367; r2=0.90). This relationship was used to estimate air temperature on days when no data were available. The number of pumping movements was counted during the stage I of copulation because these are related to the removal of rivals' sperm (Miller & Miller, 1981; Siva-Jothy & Tsubaki, 1989), and therefore might be related to male paternity.

The reproductive value of females and male coercion

We investigated variation in female reproductive potential during the day to test if females can be forced to mate even if they have no mature eggs. In August 1999 we collected 71 females that accepted copulation at different times of day and counted the number of mature eggs in their ovaries.

If male coercion is common, forced matings should be more likely as second or subsequent matings. If females are not coerced into mating, copulations should be preceded by courtship irrespective of their order. These predictions were tested with an analysis of all matings whose start was observed during 1999–2001.

The number of matings that females accept per egg clutch is the result of the interaction between female resistance and male persistence. Coercion only occurs if females diminish their resistance due to male harassment. The probability that females mate in a particular oviposition event should increase with oviposition duration, because they remain exposed to male copulatory attempts for longer periods. We tested this hypothesis in 2000 by recording the duration of visits and the number of matings of females that arrived to oviposit. Focal females were randomly selected from females that either started oviposition without previous copulation or mated with a resident male. Females were observed until they disappeared from the focal territory. Females that remained in the area less than 10 min were excluded from this analysis.

If females incur costs for resisting males, the number of copulations should be positively related to male disturbance rather than oviposition duration. We tested this prediction with a second experiment done in 2001. We observed 60 females that arrived to oviposit and recorded the duration of their visits, number of harassments and number of matings. For ovipositing females, harassments were defined as any male approach that interrupted oviposition (courtships and attempts to obtain forced tandem). Once females interrupted oviposition, harassments were defined as male attacks and chases that prevented females from perching on the desired oviposition substrate. Females were observed until they disappeared from the focal territory.

Means are presented with their standard errors and sample size. Statistical analyses were done with GenStat 4.2, 5th edition (GenStat, 2000).

Results

Population density and copulation duration

At site A in 1997 population density was high (Cordero, 1999), but after riverine vegetation was cleared in 1998 males distributed themselves over a larger area and therefore this site showed lower density of males than site B over 1999–2001 (Fig. 1). For the period 1999–2001 density was significantly different between sites (ANOVA, F1,24=62.28, p<0.001), but not between years (F2,24=1.23, p=0.310), and there was no significant interaction site × year (F2,24=1.73, p=0.199). At both sites, males obtained copulations by courting females ( Video 1 (i1536-2442-002-14-video1.mpg)) or by forcibly taking ovipositing females in tandem ( Video 2 (i1536-2442-002-14-video2.mpg), Cordero, 1999). During 1999–2001 most copulations at site A were preceded by courtship (77% of 96 matings), while at site B most were forced (60% of 161 matings; χ21=31.1, p<0.001). In contrast, in 1997 most matings were forced at site A (55% of 65 copulations, Cordero, 1999).

Courtship-preceded copulations lasted an average of 1.85±0.09 (73) min and forced copulations 1.66±0.11 (40) min. We compared the duration of both types of mating using a generalized linear model (GLM), with copulation duration as the response variable (square-root transformed), and type of copulation, site, and air temperature as predictor variables. Year was not included due to insufficient data. Results indicate a significant effect of site (p=0.017) and type of copulation (p=0.008) and a negative effect of temperature (p<0.001). The interaction site × type of mating was also significant (p=0.044). Courtship-preceded copulations were longer at site A (Fig. 2), but both types of copulations had a similar duration at site B (high density).

The first part of copulation was characterized by pumping movements of the male genitalia (stage I of Miller & Miller, 1981). There was a positive relationship between copulation duration and the number of pumping movements during stage I (Fig. 3). The average number of stage I movements was 63±6.0 (21) and 46±3.7 (21) for courtship-preceded and forced matings respectively.

The reproductive value of females and male coercion

Mating activity was most intense during the mid morning, although copulations were observed between 9:23 and 17:54 h. Postcopula females had an average of 202.3±10.6 (71) mature eggs but some accepted mating with as few as 11 eggs (Fig. 4). The effect of time of day and body size on the number of mature eggs present in females captured after having accepted copulation was tested with a GLM with Poisson errors and log link (Crawley, 1993). The reproductive value of females did not change over the day (p=0.233), but body size had a negative effect on egg load (p=0.016). Females needed at least 30 min to lay most of their eggs (Fig. 5), and abandoned oviposition with an average of 54.4±11.5 eggs remaining unlaid (22).

During 1999 to 2001 at site A females were very rarely forced in their first mating (Fig. 6), but 38% were forced in their first mating in 1997 (when male density was higher). In contrast, most matings were forced at site B, irrespective of mating order. At both sites many females were able to lay eggs without males attempting to mate at all, and for this reason the mean number of matings was 0.95±0.223 (19) at site A, and 0.98±0.180 (41) at site B (data for focal females observed in 2001). Females that oviposited for at least 30 minutes mated 0.63±0.183 (8) times at site A and 1.30±0.300 (20) at site B. In 2000, females that oviposited over a longer period mated more times. A GLM with Poisson errors and log link, with number of matings as the response variable and visit duration as the predictor variable, was significant (p=0.011) (Fig. 7a). The effect of male disturbance, population density (site) and oviposition duration on female mating frequency was tested with a GLM with Poisson errors and log link in 2001. These results indicate that neither density (p=0.474) nor oviposition duration (p=0.147) had a significant effect. In contrast, male disturbance had a strong positive effect on female mating frequency (p<0.001, Fig. 7b).

Discussion

We have shown that C. h. haemorrhoidalis males have two different reproductive tactics: courting and coercive mating. On the other hand, females seem to show an adaptive response to this shift by modulating their mating rate. In fact, our results suggest that females are modulating their mating rate to balance the costs and benefits of multiple matings with the costs imposed by male harassment (i.e., convenience polyandry). Four predictions result if the convenience polyandry hypothesis is valid. First, that forced copulations should be common at high density whereas at low density most initial copulations should be preceded by courtship. This prediction is supported by the data. The fact that density is the crucial factor for this behavioral change is supported by the comparison of site A in 1997 (high density) and 1999–2001 (low density) and by the difference between sites A and B in 1999–2001. Second, that some females should mate more than once per egg clutch and that they should copulate even if they had very few mature eggs to lay. Some females indeed mated five times, but female mating rate was lower than expected (see below). Female mating rate was positively correlated with male disturbance, but not with the time that the females spent at the oviposition patches. The third prediction is that females are forced to mate multiply. The data show that most of the subsequent matings were not preceded by courtship as the female choice hypothesis predicts, and there was a consistent difference between sites and years (Fig. 6).

Finally, if males coerce females the female mating rate per egg clutch would be expected to be significantly greater than one. Our results do not support this prediction. However, it is likely that females moved between oviposition sites when disturbed, and therefore additional matings at different places might have been under-recorded. Under a very high density, Cordero (1999) observed a female that mated 11 times in 2 hours. It is also important that a significant proportion of the focal females were able to find a refuge at the oviposition places, thereby avoiding being detected by males. Many of them were observed to lay eggs without any mating at all. In fact, ovipositing without mating with resident males is a well-known reproductive tactic in calopterygids (Siva-Jothy & Hooper, 1996). This behavior could reduce the frequency of forced matings.

Females always resist forced attempts to take them in tandem ( Video 2 (i1536-2442-002-14-video2.mpg)), but nevertheless cooperate if successfully grasped (see also Thornhill & Alcock, 1983), even if they have very few mature eggs. This suggests that costs of resisting male coercion are greater than costs of accepting a new mating, and that as a result, females engage in convenience polyandry.. Furthermore, females probably benefit from new matings because males defend their mates with the same probability after both forced or courtship matings (Cordero, 1999). At high male density females were so often chased by males that they had great difficulty starting to oviposit. Males usually guard females by defending their territories but at very high density males sometimes switched to a contact guarding strategy ( Video 3 (i1536-2442-002-14-video3.mpg), Cordero, 1999). Therefore by accepting forced copulations, females might also benefit from some minutes of guarded oviposition. The costs of forced matings are probably minimal because copulation is short, especially at high density.

Our observations indicate that females might have three alternative tactics to avoid the costs of male harassment. First, they might try to “hide” from males thereby avoiding mating during oviposition. Second, if they are unsuccessful in hiding, they might accept superfluous copulations. A third possibility is to join a defended female and oviposit close to her (Waage, 1979). Males are apparently unable to distinguish between recent mates and non-mates (Cordero, 1999; but see Hooper, 1995), and will readily defend non-mates. For this reason groups of 4–5 females defended by one male were common ( Video 3 (i1536-2442-002-14-video3.mpg)), even though only 1–2 females had been previously mated by the resident male. The behavior of C. haemorrhoidalis is very similar to that of Calopteryx maculata (Waage, 1979; 1984b), and clearly suggests that calopterygid females have many behavioral tactics that modulate the mating rate, in spite of apparent male control of oviposition substrates.

Previous studies of water striders whose mating systems are characterized by convenience polyandry have shown that copulation duration covaries positively with population density or a male-biased operational sex ratio (see Arnqvist, 1988; Rowe et al., 1994). In these species, long copulations might not only be male adaptation to avoid sperm competition (Alcock, 1994), but also result from a female strategy to avoid additional male harassment (Arnqvist, 1988; Rowe, 1992; Rowe et al., 1994). In contrast, in our study, we found that copulation was shorter at high density. In these species in which longer copulations occur at high density, females may control copulation duration. In contrast, in odonates copulation duration seems to be controlled by males (Miller, 1987), and in calopterygids mating has no guarding function. Given these conditions, copulation duration is expected to be shorter at high population density because searching time decreases and males will be “selected” to increase their mating frequency at the expense of the time dedicated to each mating (Nuyts & Michiels, 1993). That is, males are constrained and cannot maximize both mating and fertilization success. As a result, males might optimize their reproductive success in two alternative ways. At low density, males could maximize their fertilization success (i.e., increase copulation duration to reduce the risk of sperm competition) while at high density males could maximize their mating success. In fact, at high density, the amount of rival sperm removed was reduced as a consequence of shortened copulations (Cordero et al. unpublished data), suggesting that this is the case in C. h. haemorrhoidalis. A second explanation is that males are investing less time in forced matings because females whose mating is forced have been taken while in oviposition, and might have fewer eggs to lay, and also are likely to mate again under conditions of high male density. Both factors reduce the number of eggs that a male could expect to fertilize, and might contribute to shorten copulations. Additional data are needed to test these hypotheses.

In conclusion, female coercion is common in C. h. haemorrhoidalis under high population density, and females seem to respond adaptively by increasing their mating rate. In species in which females have control over copulation duration females also respond by increasing copulation duration, to avoid further male harassment. However, in species like C. h. haemorrhoidalis in which males seem to have control over copulation duration, females might avoid further male harassment by finding secretive places for oviposition or if males defend their mates by joining a group of defended ovipositing females.

Acknowledgments

We thank Alex Córdoba-Aguilar for comments on a previous version of this paper, and the referees for their critical reading and useful suggestions. This research was founded by a grant from the Spanish Ministry of Science and Culture to ACR (PB97-0379). Grants from Xunta de Galicia (1999) and University of Vigo (2000) to ACR helped in travel costs.

References

1.

J. Alcock 1994. Postinsemination associations between males and females in insects: The mate guarding hypothesis. Annual Review of Entomology 39:1–21. Google Scholar

2.

R. D. Alexander, D. C. Marshall, and J. R. Cooley . 1997. Evolutionary perspectives on insect mating. In: Choe JC, Crespi BJ, editors. The Evolution of Mating Systems in Insects and Arachnids. 4–41. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Google Scholar

3.

J. A. Andrés and A. Cordero Rivera . 2000. Copulation duration and fertilization success in a damselfly: An example of cryptic female choice? Animal Behavior 59:695–703. Google Scholar

4.

G. Arnqvist and T. Nilsson . 2000. The evolution of polyandry: multiple mating and female fitness in insects. Animal Behavior 60:145–164. Google Scholar

5.

G. Arnqvist 1988. Mate guarding and sperm displacement in the water strider Gerris lateralis Schumm. (Heteroptera: Gerridae). Freshwater Biology 19:269–274. Google Scholar

6.

G. Arnqvist 1992. Pre-copulatory fighting in a water strider - Inter-sexual conflict or mate assessment. Animal Behavior 43:559–567. Google Scholar

7.

A. J. Bateman 1948. Intra-sexual selection in Drosophila. Heredity 2:349–368. Google Scholar

8.

J. W. Bradbury and M. B. Andersson . 1987. Sexual Selection: Testing the Alternatives. John Wiley and sons. Google Scholar

9.

J. C. Choe and B. J. Crespi . 1997. The Evolution of Mating Systems in Insects and Arachnids. Cambridge University Press. Google Scholar

10.

T. H. Clutton-Brock and G. A. Parker . 1995. Punishment in animal societies. Nature 373:209–216. Google Scholar

11.

K. F. Conrad and G. Pritchard . 1992. An ecological classification of odonate mating systems: the relative influence of natural, inter- and intra-sexual selection on males. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 45:255–269. Google Scholar

12.

A. Cordero 1990. The adaptive significance of the prolonged copulations of the damselfly, Ischnura graellsii (Odonata: Coenagrionidae). Animal Behavior 40:43–48. Google Scholar

13.

A. Cordero 1999. Forced copulations and female contact guarding at a high male density in a Calopterygid damselfly. Journal of Insect Behavior 12:27–37. Google Scholar

14.

A. Cordero, S. Santolamazza Carbone, and C. Utzeri . 1995. Male disturbance, repeated insemination and sperm competition in the damselfly Coenagrion scitulum (Zygoptera: Coenagrionidae). Animal Behavior 49:437–449. Google Scholar

15.

A. Córdoba-Aguilar 2000. Reproductive behavior of the territorial damselfly Calopteryx haemorrhoidalis asturica Ocharan (Zygoptera: Calopterygidae). Odonatologica 29:295–305. Google Scholar

16.

M. J. Crawley 1993. GLIM for Ecologists. Blackwell Science. Google Scholar

17.

W. G. Eberhard 1996. Female Control: Sexual Selection by Cryptic Female Choice. Princeton University Press. Google Scholar

18.

O. M. Fincke 1982. Lifetime mating success in a natural population of the damselfly, Enallagma hageni (Walsh) (Odonata: Coenagrionidae). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 10:293–302. Google Scholar

19.

O. M. Fincke 1997. Conflict resolution in the Odonata: implications for understanding female mating patterns and female choice. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 60:201–220. Google Scholar

20.

O. M. Fincke, J. K. Waage, and W. D. Koenig . 1997. Natural and sexual selection components of odonate mating patterns. In: Choe JC, Crespi BJ, editors. The Evolution of Mating Systems in Insects and Arachnids. 58–74. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Google Scholar

21.

GenStat 2000. GenStat for Windows. Release 4.2. Fifth Edition. VSN International Ltd. Google Scholar

22.

M. R. Gross 1996. Alternative reproductive strategies and tactics: diversity within the sexes. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 11:93–98. Google Scholar

23.

A. Heymer 1972. Comportement social et territorial des Calopterygidae (Odon. Zygoptera). Annales de la Societé entomologique de France (Nouvelle Série) 8:3–53. Google Scholar

24.

A. Heymer 1973. Étude du comportement reproducteur et analyse des méchanismes déclencheurs innés (MDI) optiques chez les Calopterygidae (Odon. Zygoptera). Annales de la Societé entomologique de France (Nouvelle Série) 9:219–255. Google Scholar

25.

R. E. Hooper 1995. Individual recognition of mates and non-mates by male Calopteryx splendens xanthostoma (Charpentier) (Zygoptera: Calopterygidae). Odonatologica 24:347–352. Google Scholar

26.

D. J. Hosken, T. W. J. Garner, and P. I. Ward . 2001. Sexual conflict selects for male and female reproductive characters. Current Biology 11:489–493. Google Scholar

27.

V. Jormalainen and S. Merilaita . 1995. Female resistance and duration of mate-guarding in three aquatic Peracarids (Crustacea). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 36:43–48. Google Scholar

28.

W. D. Koenig 1991. Levels of female choice in the white-tailed skimmer Plathemis lydia (Odonata: Libellulidae). Behavior 119:193–224. Google Scholar

29.

P. L. Miller 1987. An examination of the prolonged copulations of Ischnura elegans (Vander Linden) (Zygoptera: Coenagrionidae). Odonatologica 16:37–56. Google Scholar

30.

P. L. Miller and C. A. Miller . 1981. Field observations on copulatory behavior in Zygoptera, with an examination of the structure and activity of male genitalia. Odonatologica 10:201–218. Google Scholar

31.

A. J. Moore 1989. The behavioral ecology of Libellula luctuosa (Burmeister)(Odonata: Libellulidae): III. Male density, OSR, and male and female mating behavior. Ethology 80:120–136. Google Scholar

32.

E. Nuyts and N. K. Michiels . 1993. Integration of immediate and long term sperm precedence patterns and mating costs in an optimization model of insect copulation duration. Journal of Theoretical Biology 160:271–295. Google Scholar

33.

L. Oksanen 2001. Logic of experiments in ecology: is pseudoreplication a pseudoissue? Oikos 94:27–38. Google Scholar

34.

G. A. Parker 1979. Sexual selection and sexual conflict. In: Blum MS, Blum NA, editors. Sexual Selection and Reproductive Competition in Insects. 123–166. New York: Academic Press. Google Scholar

35.

S. Plaistow and M. T. Siva-Jothy . 1996. Energetic constraints and male mate-securing tactics in the damselfly Calopteryx splendens xanthostoma (Charpentier). Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological Sciences 263:1233–1238. Google Scholar

36.

W. R. Rice 1996. Sexually antagonistic male adaptation triggered by experimental arrest of female evolution. Nature 381:232–234. Google Scholar

37.

L. Rowe 1992. Convenience polyandry in a water strider: foraging conflicts and female control of copulation frequency and guarding duration. Animal Behavior 44:189–202. Google Scholar

38.

L. Rowe, G. Arnqvist, A. Sih, and J. Krupa . 1994. Sexual conflict and the evolutionary ecology of mating patterns: water striders as a model system. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 9:289–293. Google Scholar

39.

M. T. Siva-Jothy and R. E. Hooper . 1996. Differential use of stored sperm during oviposition in the damselfly Calopteryx splendens xanthostoma (Charpentier). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 39:389–393. Google Scholar

40.

M. T. Siva-Jothy and T. Tsubaki . 1989. Variation in copulation duration in Mnais pruinosa pruinosa Selys (Odonata: Calopterygidae) 1. Alternative mate-securing tactics and sperm precedence. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 24:39–45. Google Scholar

41.

R. Thornhill and J. Alcock . 1983. The Evolution of Insect Mating Systems. Harvard University Press. Google Scholar

42.

H. Ubukata 1984. Oviposition site selection and avoidance of additional mating by females of the dragonfly, Cordulia anenea amuriensis Selys (Corduliidae). Researches on Population Ecology 26:285–301. Google Scholar

43.

J. K. Waage 1973. Reproductive behavior and its relation to territoriality in Calopteryx maculata (Beauvois) (Odonata: Calopterygidae). Behavior 47:240–256. Google Scholar

44.

J. K. Waage 1979. Adaptive significance of postcopulatory guarding of mates and nonmates by male Calopteryx maculata (Odonata). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 6:147–154. Google Scholar

45.

J. K. Waage 1984a. Sperm competition and the evolution of odonate mating systems. In: Smith RL, editor. Sperm Competition and the Evolution of Animal Mating Systems. 251–290. Orlando: Academic Press. Google Scholar

46.

J. K. Waage 1984b. Female and male interactions during courtship in Calopteryx maculata and C. dimidiata (Odonata: Calopterygidae): influence of oviposition behavior. Animal Behavior 32:400–404. Google Scholar

47.

J. K. Waage 1987. Choice and utilization of oviposition sites by female Calopteryx maculata (Odonata: Calopterygidae). I. Influence of site size and the presence of other females. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 20:439–446. Google Scholar

48.

M. J. West-Eberhard, J. W. Bradbury, N. B. Davies, P-H. Gouyon, P. Hammerstein, B. König, G. A. Parker, D. C. Queller, N. Sachser, T. Slagsvold, F. Trillmich, and C. Vogel . 1987. Conflicts between and within the sexes in sexual selection. In: Bradbury JW, Andersson MB, editors. Sexual Selection: Testing the Alternatives. 180–195. Chichester: John Wiley and sons. Google Scholar

Figure 1.

An estimate of population density of Calopteryx haemorrhoidalis haemorrhoidalis (individuals marked/hour±SE) in both study sites. In 1997 only site A was studied (no SE is available for this estimate). In 1998 clearing of the riverine vegetation provided many sunny areas and males dispersed over greater shore distances. Consequently density was lower in site A over 1997–2001.

i1536-2442-2-14-1-f01.jpg

Figure 2.

The effect of site and type of copulation on mating duration. Values are mean duration after the removal of the negative effect of temperature (p<0.001). The interaction is significant (p=0.044).

i1536-2442-2-14-1-f02.jpg

Figure 3.

The relationship between copulation duration and the number of pumping movements during the stage I of copulation in courtship and forced matings.

i1536-2442-2-14-1-f03.jpg

Figure 4.

The number of mature eggs in females that accepted copulation. Note that some females mated even though they had very few eggs.

i1536-2442-2-14-1-f04.jpg

Figure 5.

The relationship between oviposition duration and the number of eggs remaining in the abdomen of females that spontaneously abandoned oviposition or females that were ovipositing and accepted a new mating (incomplete oviposition).

i1536-2442-2-14-1-f05.jpg

Figure 6.

The proportion of forced matings in first and subsequent matings of marked females. Numbers after years indicate sample size.

i1536-2442-2-14-1-f06.jpg

Figure 7a. The relationship between duration of oviposition and number of matings for females observed in 2000.

i1536-2442-2-14-1-f701.jpg

Figure 7b. The relationship between the number of male disturbances of ovipositing females (including chases when females tried to start oviposition) and the number of matings per clutch. Disturbances that ended in copulation are not included.

i1536-2442-2-14-1-f702.jpg
A. Cordero and J. A. Andrés "Male coercion and convenience polyandry in a calopterygid damselfly," Journal of Insect Science 2(14), 1-7, (1 August 2002). https://doi.org/10.1673/031.002.1401
Received: 13 May 2002; Accepted: 1 July 2002; Published: 1 August 2002
KEYWORDS
Calopterygidae
courtship
forced matings
sexual conflict
sexual harassment
Back to Top