Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 July 2015 Acoustic Communication in Phaneropterinae (Tettigonioidea) - A Global Review with Some New Data
Klaus-Gerhard Heller, Claudia Hemp, Sigfrid Ingrisch, Chunxiang Liu
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Phaneropterinae is the largest subfamily within the bush-crickets/ katydids (Tettigonioidea), with about 2451 species, and with a world-wide distribution. Its acoustic communication differs from all other tettigonioid groups in that females primarily and typically respond to the male calling song with their own acoustic reply, a behaviour referred to as duetting. This type of response seems to have been lost only in a few species with wingless females.

According to our literature review, information about the song patterns of about 330 species of Phaneropterinae have been published world-wide. Included in this number are ca 170 species of Barbitistini, a flightless West Palearctic tribe, which are treated separately. In the present study we summarize information from the above 330 species. We examine the morphology of stridulatory and hearing organs, and analyze the acoustic signals for frequency, number of syllables and number of interval types. We also have examined if and how responding by sound may have influenced other aspects of the acoustic communication system, especially the structure of the male calling song.

Overall, the songs of male Phaneropterinae are similar to those of other tettigonioids. However, some Phaneropterinae species with very long and complex songs are found on all continents, exceeding in these characters nearly all other Ensifera species. These songs contain several different types of syllables and intervals of various duration. Because of this high interspecific variability (reaching from very simple to extremely complex), male phaneropterine songs are by far more variable than those of other tettigonioid families.

However, since there are so few data on the behaviour of most Phaneropterinae species, and especially for females, we still are limited in our understanding of the reasons behind the song variability. Sexual selection by females choosing to respond preferentially to certain song types could be an important evolutionary force, but probably only in combination with some unknown ecological and behavioural factors.

Introduction

The subfamily Phaneropterinae is a large (2451 valid species; OSFO 2014/11/05), diverse, but also distinct group of bush-crickets (katydids; Tettigonioidea). Due to the large number of species, it has attracted the attention of taxonomists, resulting in the first world-wide revision of any tettigonioid group (Brunner von Wattenwyl, 1878). Later, however, a rapid increase in the number of new described species prevented further general revisions, and subsequent comprehensive treatments of the phaneropterine fauna were restricted to selected areas or countries, such as the USSR by Bey-Bienko (1954) or China by Kang et al. (2014). Recent molecular studies support the monophyly of the group (Mugleston et al. 2013) as defined by older authorities. By some authors, Phaneropterinae s.str. is now considered as one of four subfamilies of Phaneropteridae (Heller et al. 2014).

Morphologically, many phaneropterines can easily be recognised by the length of the hind wings, which surpass the forewings. While this character is not found in all species and is also shared by some species from other groups, the shape of the eggs seems to be diagnostic. As already described and figured by Bey-Bienko (1954), the eggs of all species studied so far are flat [for one remarkable modification see Massa (2013)], probably adapted for oviposition into plant tissues. Corresponding to egg shape and oviposition site, the ovipositor is usually strongly laterally compressed. Together with the closely related Pseudophyllinae and Mecopodinae (Gorochov 1995, Mugleston et al. 2013), all species feed nearly exclusively on plants (Rentz 2010), contrasting with the more omnivorous or even purely carnivorous habit of most other tettigonioids.

Phaneropterinae are characterized not only in hind wing length, egg shape, oviposition and feeding behaviour, but also in communication. Like most tettigonioids, they communicate by sound. However, unlike most other tettigonid groups, female phaneropterines respond to the male calling song with their own acoustic reply, thus initiating a duet. Only in very few genera has this female behaviour been secondarily lost, together with the sound producing organs (some species in Poecilimon Fischer, 1853, Odontura Rambur, 1839, Odonturoides Ragge, 1980, Arachnitus Hebard, 1932; see e.g., Helversen et al. 2012, Strauß et al. 2014). This duetting behaviour has long been known. The female sounds seem to have been first heard and described by Riley (1874 fide Spooner 1968), while the female sound producing organs were discovered by Pungúr (1886). At the end of the last century several reviews about Phaneropterinae communication were produced (Zhantiev & Korsunovskaya 1986, Spooner 1995), but based mainly on geographically restricted data and focusing on selected aspects.

Acoustical answering is not uncommon among insects communicating by sound, but among tettigonioids out side Phaneropterinae it is known only in some species of Bradyporidae (see Bailey 2003, Drosopoulos and Claridge 2006). At this time, we do not know the behavioral, ecological, and evolutionary consequences of such duetting. Nor do we know if duetting behaviour has influenced morphology, sound production, hearing and behaviour in phaneropterines.

Some North American phaneropterines possess extremely complex songs (Walker & Dew 1972, Walker et al. 2003, Walker 2004). Walker (2004) assumed such complexity evolved from complexity of context; i.e., duetting, including complex exchange of information about the mating partner, and possible eavesdropping selected for song complexity. But is this true for most or only a few species? For any comparison with other groups we are severely restricted because compiled data about tettigonid acoustics do not exist. An additional complication is that we know little about mate-location behavior in Phaneropterinae: Does the male approach the stationary female or vice-versa? At what point do individuals switch from acoustic orientation to visual, chemical, tactile, or vibrational orientation? Do individuals alter song characteristics with distance from mate, intra-specific competition, or current predator threat? Until we know more about current and ancestoral contexts, it is difficult to answer questions about why.

Fig. 1.

Unit of male calling song of Barbitites yersini as example of a song with one syllable type, four pause types, 17 syllables and a duration of 750 ms. Oscillograms of stridulatory movement and song [synchronous registration of left tegmen movement and sound (upper line: upward deflection represents opening, downward closing; lower line: sound)]. See Stumpner & Meyer 2001 for details of the function of the pause types.

f01_07.jpg

The first step in understanding acoustic communication in phaneropterines is analysis of their acoustics. However, comparing complex songs is not easy, and parameters of interest to the researchers may change with time. In this review we will therefore give tables with some basic information about the song of each species so that researchers can compare their data. We wish to present an overview of what is known about the acoustics of the different species of Phaneropterinae, treat the morphology of the organs for sound production and hearing, and also discuss data from hearing physiology and duetting behaviour. The main aspect, however, will be a comparative study of the male songs. Female replies will also be covered, but they seem to be much simpler in structure than male songs, and, unfortunately, much less known.

Methods

For this review, we evaluated all data about phaneropterine songs that we could find from the literature, CDs, and the internet site SINA; see detailed data in  Supplementary Material SM1 (SM1 song data typical Phaneropterinae.xls) and  SM2 (SM2 song data Barbitistini.xls). However, typically only one or a few sources per species are given (for older data see Heller 1988, Ragge & Reynolds 1998, SINA). In addition, we present new detailed data for the following species: Ectadia fulva Brunner von Wattenwyl, 1893 (THAILAND: Chiang Mai Prov, Doi Suthep (-Pui) (18°48′N, 98°55′E), 1100–1150 m, 13 iv 1995, leg. S. Ingrisch), Monticolaria kilimanjarica Sjöstedt, 1909 (TANZANIA: Mt. Kilimanjaro, 6 xii 2012, leg. Claudia Hemp; recording methods see Heller et al. 2010) and Ectadia sp. (CHINA: Yunnan, Honghe, Pingbian, Daweishan Forest Reserve (22°54′N, 103 °41′E), 1700–2100 m, 15–17 viii 2013, leg. Liu Chunxiang). For the tables ( SM1 (SM1 song data typical Phaneropterinae.xls) and  SM2 (SM2 song data Barbitistini.xls)) and calculations, we have also used some unpublished sound information from the specimens listed in the  Supplementary Material SM3 (SM3 Specimen data.xls). For the authors of the scientific names see OSFO.

For definitions of terms used in insect acoustic analysis, consult Ewing (1989), Huber et al. (1989), Bailey (1991) or Ragge and Reynolds (1998).

In this review we compared the carrier frequencies (peak) and several parameters of the temporal pattern, including:

  • (1) Number of different syllable types. During one cycle of movement (opening and closing of the tegmina) an individual produces one syllable (see Ragge & Reynolds 1998). Using different movement patterns an animal can produce different syllable types. The wing stridulatory movements of many species have been studied (see Heller 1988). Where it is unknown, we have tried to get as correct an estimate as obtainable from the sound pattern. There are, however, a few species where the stridulatory movement is so complicated that the term syllable would be misleading compared to ‘normal’ species (e.g., species of Acrometopa Fieber, 1853, of Isophya Brunner von Wattenwyl, 1878, and of Ectadia fulva). In these cases we have described clearly separated acoustical units as syllables.

  • (2) Number of pause types (see Fig. 1). By using several types of pauses between the syllables an animal is able to produce complicated, species-specific songs.

  • (3) Number of syllables per unit. In all species, after some time the same pattern of syllables and pauses is repeated. As a measure of song complexity, we counted how many syllables were contained in one unit of the song (before the next repetition starts). Here, problems can arise if identical subunits are repeated several times and it is unclear how many constitute a basic unit, and if one or several subunits are necessary for recognition by the receiving insect.

  • (4) Duration of song. We measured the duration of one unit from beginning of the first to end of the last syllable. All data refer to the period of activity only. Long intervals when the animal does not sing are excluded.

The male song pattern is known in 325 phaneropterine species, among them 164 members of the West Palearctic tribe Barbitistini. This group (269 recognized species; OSFO) differs from most other phaneropterines as they are micropterous and unable to fly. Since this character may influence the acoustic communication and is obviously over-represented in our sample, both groups (Barbitistini and ‘typical’ Phaneropterinae) were analysed separately. ‘Typical’ Phaneropterinae include some species with different degrees of wing reduction outside Barbitistini.

Results

Morphology

Male stridulatory organs.—In phaneropterines, as in all tettigonioids, the male stridulatory file is situated on the underside of the left tegmen (Fig. 7B), and a scraper at the inner (anal) edge of the right tegmen (see e.g., fig. 3 in Heller & Helversen 1986). Stridulatory files are now often figured in revisions or descriptions of new species since they are often species-specific and useful for determination. However, normally it is impossible to make any predictions about the song from knowledge of the file. Therefore we will not give a list of species for which file data or figures are available. Concerning teeth number, phaneropterines are quite variable. The lowest number seems to be found in Elimaea rosea Brunner von Wattenwyl, 1878 which has only 12 (range = 10–14) teeth (Ingrisch 2011), while the highest numbers (up to 380) are found in some species of the genus Hemielimaea Brunner von Wattenwyl, 1878 (Ingrisch & Gorochov 2007). Closely related species with the same teeth number can produce quite different songs (e.g., Walker et al. 2003).

Unlike in most other tettigonioids, phaneropterine files often show abrupt changes in tooth size or tooth density (e.g., Leroy 1970). If the song — or better, the stridulatory movement — is known, it is often easy to understand the function of the irregularities, but not in advance. In Acrometopa, for example, the large teeth are used for the production of the louder part of the song only (see Figs 7, 8 and Heller 1988 for other examples).

The inner dorsal edge of the right tegmen serves as as scraper or plectrum (see fig. 1 in Heller 1988 for a scanning electron microscope photo), and scrapes along the file to produce the wing vibrations that generate the acoustic signal. The right tegmen also contains the mirror, veins and/or other resonating structures that help determine the frequency spectrum of the song (see e.g., fig. 2 in Heller & Hemp 2014; Fig. 2A). These structures are typically confined to the dorsal field of the tegmen, an area close to the articulation. Only in males of some species of the genus Oxyecous are large parts of the right tegmen swollen, transparent, and mirror-like (Ragge 1956; Fig. 2C) and may function in sound radiation. Precursors of such swollen tegmina seem to exist in Melidia species (Fig. 2A; see photos in OSFO) and Ducetia punctipennis (Gerstaecker, 1869) (Fig. 2B; see note in Ragge 1961 and photos in OSFO). Also in species without a clearly defined mirror (e.g., Horatosphaga leggei; see Heller et al. 2014) other parts of the tegmina may resonate.

Fig. 2.

Left and right male tegmina in species that have both a distinct mirror on the right tegmen (near base of wing), and also a transparent mirror-like area, outside the mirror, on the right tegmen. A) Melidia laminata B) Ducetia punctipennis; C) Oxyecous apertus. Scale 5 mm.

f02_07.jpg

Female stridulatory organs.—Unlike in most other Tettigonioidea, female phaneropterines also usually possess tegminal stridulatory organs. Their morphology differs from that of males. The female stridulatory organs were discovered and figured by Pungúr (1886) in a species of Poecilimon. In all phaneropterines studied thus far, the teeth are spine-like and found on the upper side of the right tegmen, and the scraper is often formed by the inner (anal) edge of the left tegmen. The number of teeth and also the number of stridulatory veins varies greatly in females, and is mostly not species-specific. In Holochlora nigrotympana Ingrisch, 1990, for example, high numbers of teeth and veins are easily visible (fig. 1 in Heller et al. 1997b), whereas the low numbers (10–15 small teeth on one vein) in Nanoleptopoda Braun, 2011 are even difficult to detect (Braun 2011). A second scraper system is known only in some Barbitistini. Here an elevated vein acting as scraper crosses the lower side of the right tegmen (see figures in Heller & Helversen 1986, Zhantiev & Korsunovskaya 1986).

Hearing organs. —In males and females the auditory sensory cells are combined in complex organs (Montealegre-Z et al. 2012, Palghat et al. 2012), one in each fore tibia, situated just below the femur-tibia articulation. Each organ has two tympanic membranes, one on each side of the leg. These membranes are exposed both to sound coming directly from the outside, and to acoustic input coming from the prothoracic spiracle reaching the tympana from the inner side via the auditory trachea. In this general aspect the Phaneropterinae do not differ from other bush-crickets. However, in most Phaneropterinae the thoracic auditory spiracles and bullae (entrance of the auditory trachea) are extraordinarily large, larger than in any other tettigoniid group (Bailey 1990). Although large in both sexes, they are often larger in males than in females (Bailey 1990). The sound coming through the thorax is thought to be dominant over that of sound impinging directly on the external surface of the tympanic membrane (eg., Michelsen et al. 1994). Comparative physiological measurements in Barbitistini support the hypothesis that large bullae increase the sensitivity, at least for high frequencies above 10 to 15 kHz (Stumpner & Heller 1992, Strauß et al. 2014), even in this short-winged tribe, where the bullae are relatively small compared to most long-winged species.

There is a large variety in size and shape of cuticular folds around the tympana. In many phaneropterines the tympanic membranes are completely open or ‘naked’, meaning that there are no cuticular ridges shielding or covering part of the membrane. In others species, one side (the anterior) is covered by a cuticular fold of different size, and yet in other species the membranes of both sides are behind cuticular folds or walls (Bailey 1990). The possession of these folds is used to define some tribes; asymmetric ears are characteristic for the mainly Asian Holochlorini and the African Poreuomenini. At present, there is no evidence of an acoustic function of the folds, especially not in the low-frequency range where the input through the tympana may be most important (see below). Here the wavelengths are much larger than the folds. Possibly the folds offer some mechanical protection of the tympanic membranes. In Oxyecoaus lesnei Chopard, 1936, the asymmetric fold is much larger in the male than in the female, but the reason is unknown (Ragge 1956, Bailey 1990).

Sounds and hearing

Although both male and female phaneropterines can usually produce sounds via wing stridulation, only the male's sound is referred to as a calling song. When characterizing and comparing various calling songs, two different aspects of the sound should be considered separately: 1) A song may have different carrier frequencies, which may activate different sensory cells (spectral properties; frequency domain analysis), and/or 2) a song may have different amplitude modulations, evaluated by the central nervous system (time domain analysis). Whether or not the songs of males and females and of different species differ in intensity is not considered in this review, because too few data are available (see Helversen et al. 2015 for data and a discussion).

Spectral properties of male songs.—The songs of most male phaneropterines consist of heavily damped impulses, which result from the contact of one tooth of the stridulatory file with the plectrum (see Figs 4, 7). (The few phaneropterines with resonant songs are listed in Hemp et al. 2013.) So there is typically not a single dominant frequency like in crickets, but a broad band. In Tables 1 ( SM1 (SM1 song data typical Phaneropterinae.xls)) and 2 ( SM2 (SM2 song data Barbitistini.xls)) the frequency with the highest amplitude is given. If there was no specific information in the respective paper, we report the maximum of the spectrum if there was a clear maximum, or otherwise the mean of the band some dB below an irregular peak.

The compilation of the data shows that most phaneropterines have relatively low-frequency songs with maxima in the audio (< 20 kHz) or low ultrasound range (Fig. 3A). However, these results may be biased for several reasons. Some studies lacked equipment capable of recording ultrasound. In addition, large species, which generally have lower songs (Del Castillo & Gwynne 2009, Heller et al. 2010) might have been preferred for study, or species using only ultrasound were overlooked. On the other hand, even the tiny species Himertula marmorata (Brunner von Wattenwyl, 1891), one of the smallest long-winged phaneropterines (tegmen length 12 mm), has a relatively low (33 kHz) frequency song (Fig. 4).

The lowest-frequency song for male phaneropterines (4 kHz) is found in Zeuneria biramosa Sjöstedt, 1929 (Heller et al. 2014) and the highest frequency song, at 90 kHz (an extreme outlier), is produced by Ectomoptera nepicauda Ragge, 1980 (Heller & Hemp 2014). The frequency of the songs of Barbitistini is clearly higher than in typical phaneropterines (Fig. 3B). This is in accordance with the generally smaller tegmina in this flightless group, but also with the requirements for the range of the signals. Given equal intensity, low-frequency signals can generally be heard over a greater distance (see e.g., Heller et al. 2010), which may be of more importance for flying-adept, agile species than for flightless ones.

Fig. 3.

Distribution of peak frequencies of male songs in typical phaneropterines (A) and Barbitistini (B) and correlation between male and female peak frequencies (C).

f03_07.jpg

Typically, the song of tettigonioids shows more or less the same spectrum in different parts of the song, i.e., there is no frequency modulation. There are, however, some exceptions. For example, the hemisyllables produced during opening and closing of the tegmina differ sometimes in spectral composition (e.g., Heller & Korsunovskaya 2009). Such cases, however, are rarely documented in Phaneropterinae. One notable exception is Poecilimon tschorochensis Adelung, 1907, which produces narrow-banded low frequency and broad-banded ultrasound dominated song components (Heller et al. 2006). An even more remarkable case is found in Ectadia sp. (Heller & Liu, unpubl.), which has a complicated, long lasting (ca 60 s), multi-syllabic song. It starts with short, broad-banded clicks with a maximum at about 19 kHz, followed by a distinctive group of impulses paired in time with another impulse group (closing and opening hemisyllables?), and continuing in a long series. The spectra differ between the impulse groups: the first one with a peak frequency at 9 kHz, and the second at 19 kHz. During the course of the song these pairs become increasingly louder (by about 15 dB) and in the first group of impulses an ultrasound component (peak at 60 kHz) reaches in amplitude the low frequency peak (Fig. 5). A similar increase of an ultrasound component is also observed in the song of Ectadia fulva (unpubl.). Frequency changes have also been observed in the song of Horatosphaga leggei (Kirby, 1909) (Heller et al. 2014). Nothing is known about the function of these different frequencies nor how they are produced. Since in male-female communication, frequency differences can be evaluated by the female (see below), they may be important for males.

Fig. 4.

Sonogram and power spectrum of male song of Himertula marmorata.

f04_07.jpg

Fig. 5.

Sonogram of 1-s part of male song of Ectadia sp. (field recording; distance microphone-animal ca. 50 cm). Details see text.

f05_07.jpg

Spectral properties of female replies.—The spectral composition of female replies is much less known than that of males. Surprisingly, in most species, male and female spectra are quite similar, although the morphology of the stridulatory organs is distinctly different. In Barbitistini, male and female frequencies are correlated (r2 = 0.78; 33 species; Fig. 3C; unpubl. data). In the few known ‘typical’ phaneropterines, the same tendency can be observed, but there are outliers in both directions, female replies being higher or lower than the male song (Fig. 3C).

Temporal pattern of male songs.—In male phaneropterines, the time-amplitude patterns of the calling songs show an extremely high diversity (see e.g., Figs 7, 8) so that comparisons and classifications are difficult. For a quantitative approach we have used the following three basic measurements: number of syllables within one unit of the song, number of different syllable types, and number of different pause types. While these data are largely independent of temperature, we give also the duration of one unit of the song. This is an easily recognisable character, which is, however, affected by the temperature of the singing animal.

The number of syllables per unit of the song shows a distribution strongly skewed towards the left (Fig. 6) with a mode at one in Barbitistini (more than 90 species) and in ‘typical’ phaneropterines (about 30 species). However, in both groups there is a long tail to the right (Fig. 6) with extremes of more than 1000 syllables per unit in Barbitistini (genus Polysarcus) and around 500 in ‘typical’ phaneropterines (genus Tinzeda). Concerning the duration of the song, the range in both groups is also comparable and covers four orders of magnitude, extending from less than 10 ms in Andreiniimon nuptialis to 100 s in some song forms of Ducetia japonica (Thunberg, 1815). Barbitistini songs tend to be shorter (median = 0.3 s) than that of ‘typical’ phaneropterines (median =1 s; Fig. 6). Mode and median of the number of syllable types is one in both groups (Fig. 6), however, the mean is higher in typical phaneropterines (1.59) than in Barbitistini (1.23). Interestingly, species of the North American genus Amblycorypha Stål, 1873 tend to have the highest number of syllable types of all phaneropterines. These species also have the highest numbers of pause types. In general, typical phaneropterines have more different pause types (x = 2.47; median = 2) than Barbitistini (x = 1.71; median = 1; Fig. 6). The values make it understandable that often syllable repetition rates are not sufficient to characterize songs or song differences. In species in which the same syllable is repeated several times, the rates vary between 1 and 80 Hz.

Fig. 6.

Data on time pattern. Distribution of numbers of syllable types (above), numbers of pause types (second from above), duration of song (second from below) and numbers of syllables per song unit (below).

f06_07.jpg

Some long-winged phaneropterine species can produce two different song types: a slow-fast song, and a long-short song ( see Table SM1 (SM1 song data typical Phaneropterinae.xls)), but there are no data or hypotheses about their functions except for some Phaneroptera Serville, 1831 species, where Korsunovskaya (2008) assumes a rivalry or territorial function for the long song.

Fig. 7.

Ectadia fulva. A) Male calling song. Oscillograms of stridulatory movement and song [synchronous registration of left tegmen movement and sound (upper line: upward deflection represents opening, downward closing; lower line: sound)]. The deflections of the movement track before and after the song result from soundless vibratory body movements. B) Stridulatory file.

f07_07.jpg

After the calling song, the males of many phaneropterine species produce — rarely, occasionally or quite often - short sound elements which resemble female replies. These sounds may even occur within the male song if the female is expected to respond at this time (e.g., Stumpner & Meyer 2001). The first hypothesis about their function as female mimicry was proposed by Stumpner & Meyer (2001). They assumed that the males might stimulate female competition. Later Bailey et al. (2006) and Heller et al (2011) suggested that by producing these signals the males may disturb the orientation of other males towards a responding female. This idea is supported by experiments of Helversen et al. (2001) who showed that male Poecilimon affinis cannot spatially separate multiple, simultaneous female replies, and instead walk in an intermediate direction.

Production of male songs.—To produce different types of syllables, different stridulatory movements must be used. The first documented example was with Amblycorypha uhleri (Stål, 1876), where Walker & Dew (1972) recorded different tegminal movement patterns using high speed photography. Since then, other techniques have become available and the stridulatory movements of species in all large groups of singing Orthoptera have been recorded, including in tettigonioids (Montealegre-Z 2012), acridids(Helversen & Elsner 1977), and crickets (Robillard et al. 2013). However, our understanding of the production of complex songs is still very limited, including the use of irregular stridulatory files (for new movement data of phaneropterines see Iorgu 2012; Heller et al. 2014). Therefore we present here two remarkable examples:

  • 1) Ectadia fulva Brunner von Wattenwyl, 1893. The song of this species (see also Fig. 169 in Ingrisch 1998) is produced by one complete opening and closing movement of the tegmina, but since during this time the singing male opens and closes its fore wings partly several times, the term syllable should be applied with caution. From Fig. 7A it is evident that the male opens his wings rapidly at the beginning of the song. In the figure, before the song, two short soundless vibrations (tremulations) of the body can be recognised. With open tegmina, the male starts a series of about 20 movement cycles, during which the wings are closed only to a very limited degree. After a nearly mute beginning, during each of the small closing movements here a short, click-like sound is produced. About one second after the initial opening of the wings, the male switches to a slow closing movement which increases in speed until the end of the song. During this time many separate impulses are produced with decreasing intervals. Comparing the movement pattern with the structure of the file (Fig. 7), the male obviously stridulates first with the few large teeth at the distal part of the file and uses the long dense part of the file only during the second half of the song. The movement pattern has some similarities to that observed in species of the genus Acrometopa (Heller 1988), but the structure of the stridulatory file and also the song pattern are quite different.

  • 2) Monticolaria kilimandjarica. The time-amplitude pattern of the complicated song of this species was described in Hemp et al. (2009). Similar to E. fulva, the teeth at the distal end the stridulatory file are much larger than those at the proximal end of the file (see figs 8 & 9 in Hemp et al. 2009). However, in contrast to most phaneropterines (and other tettigonioids), the steep side of each tooth is directed towards the wing base, indicating sound production during the opening and not the closing of the wing. This prediction was confirmed by recent movement recordings (Fig. 8). M. kilimandjarica seems to be the first known insect species which produces a complicated song exclusively by opening wing movements with different speed and amplitude. All other species with this type of stridulation have simple songs containing one syllable type and mostly single, isolated syllables (Phaneroptera spp., Yersinella spp., Steropleurus (formerly Uromenus) andalusius (Rambur, 1838) and brunneri (Bolivar, 1876), Sabaterpia (formerly Ephippigerida) taeniata (Saussure, 1898), all Heller 1988; Ischnomela gracilis Stål, 1873, Montealegre-Z 2012).

Fig. 8.

Male calling song of Monticolaria kilimandjarica. Oscillograms of stridulatory movement and song [synchronous registration of left tegmen movement and sound (upper line: upward deflection represents opening, downward closing; lower line: sound)].

f08_07.jpg

Temporal pattern and production of female songs.—Female acoustic signals are known from 67 phaneropterine species, including 36 Barbitistini and 31 typical phaneropterines. As recognised from the published data, they always consist of short, click-like sounds, which are typically produced by a closing movement of the wings. An example is given in Fig. 9 for Poecilimon gracilis (Fieber, 1853). The number of clicks is not fixed. For example, Poecilimon laevissimus (Fischer, 1853) females often produce additional clicks immediately after the the initial sound producing movement (Heller & Helversen 1986). In other species females makes several stridulatory movements, each one producing clicks (Villarreal & Gilbert 2013).

Hearing in males and females.—The hearing sensitivity of phaneropterines has been studied in only a few species ( see SM1 (SM1 song data typical Phaneropterinae.xls) and  SM2 (SM2 song data Barbitistini.xls)). Most studies examined both sexes, but some looked at only one sex. Most results are based on neurophysiological measurements supplemented by few behavioural studies. As in many Tettigonioidea, frequency threshold curves for phaneropterines seem to be often quite broad (but see e.g., Nocke 1975, Korsunovskaya 2005) with the range of highest sensitivity typically between 10 and 20 kHz. The highest sensitivity of the receiver, and the intensity peak of the carrier frequency of the opposite sex may (e.g., Stictophaula ocellata Ingrisch, 1994), or may not (e.g., Holochlora nigrotympana Ingrisch, 1990) exactly overlap (see Heller et al. 1997b). However, in two Barbitistini species [Leptophyes punctatissima (Bose, 1792) and Poecilimon elegans (Brunner von Wattenwyl, 1878)], maximum intensity of the male calling song and maximum sensitivity of female hearing are both above 30 kHz. In fact, in these two species, the most sensitive hearing range lies at the highest frequency of all phaneropterines studied so far. Interestingly, both of these small species are notvery sensitive in general. InL. punctatissima, frequency threshold curves remain nearly flat from 10 to 60 kHz (Rössler et al. 1994), whereas P. elegans become gradually more sensitive from 10 to at least 40 kHz (Strauß et al. 2014). When comparing species in the genus Poecilimon, there is a correlation between the sensitivity of the ears (especially for ultrasound) and the size of the auditory thoracic spiracles (Stumpner & Heller 1992, Strauß et al. 2014).

Without going deeply into neurobiology we have to mention a hypothesis proposed by ter Hofstede and Fullard (2008) and ter Hofstede et al. (2010). They assume that the T-cells, which are considered to be important in many tettigonioids for sound detection of predators, are also used in male-female acoustic communication in phaneropterines.

Duetting behaviour

If two partners communicate by duetting it is useful for both if they follow certain rules, such as not to talk simultaneously - many insects are deaf during the production of their own song. Hence, the male who starts the duet must include silent gaps in his song so that he can hear a responding female. The situation can be additionally improved if the female does not answer at random, hoping to find a pause in the song, but if she can use acoustical characters (trigger; see below) in the male song predicting silent periods. On the other side, the male should not wait too long to decide if a click-like sound is a female answer to his song or an accidental noise in the environment.

The intervals between male and female acoustic signals can be amazingly short. This is seen in Barbitistini, where the silent interval between the end of the male song and the female response is mostly less than 100 ms. Such short intervals are often too fast to be detected without special equipment. For example, Faber (1953: p.180) described the reactions of a female of Polysarcus denticauda (Charpentier, 1825) to certain parts of the male song as strictly synchronous (“streng synchron”), based on observations in the years 1929–1952. In many long-winged phaneropterine species the intervals are longer (ca 50 to 1500 ms, with the single outlier Elimaea thaii Ingrisch, 1998 with 3500 ms; Ingrisch 1998). Such delay times were hypothesized by Alexander (1960) as species-specific and as potentially isolating characters. This idea was supported as soon as song recordings became available (Spooner 1968, Nickle 1976). Theoretically, a female should recognize the song of a conspecific male, judge the quality of the potential mating partner, then start a timer, preferentially after a trigger, and, finally, respond at the appropriate time interval. But which songs are answered by the females, how do they time their response and what do the males do after having heard it? Two different approaches are used to answer these questions: A) computer-generated song models test and determine the essential song parameters and B) two-choice experiments using live males that differ in certain characteristics, determine which male traits, including both acoustic and non-acoustic characteristics, elicit female responses.

Concerning the first approach (A), in 1986 almost simultaneously three papers were published with similar content but on different phaneropterine species. Zhantiev & Korsunovskaya (1986) demonstrated that the duration and internal structure of the syllable was decisive for a female response, and that, depending on the species, either the beginning or the end of the syllable was used as trigger. Heller & Helversen (1986) showed that females of different species had different response delays and that the males had quite narrow response windows, outside of which they did not accept the female's answer. Lastly, Robinson et al. (1986), followed by Zimmermann et al. (1989), analysed the duetting behaviour of one species in detail and documented therewith the existence of a male response window in another species. All the above studied species had simple male songs with one type of syllable and pause. Similar results were obtained in additional phaneropterine species with some species-specific modifications (e.g., Tauber & Pener 2000, Orci 2007). Some years later, Dobler et al. (1994a, b) analysed a species with a complicated song and showed that the durations of syllables and pauses, including a pause before a trigger syllable, was essential to elicit the female response. In this species [Ancistrura nigrovittata (Brunner von Wattenwyl, 1878)] males and females even rely on the use of different carrier frequencies, a situation unique among all phaneropterine species studied so far.

Concerning the second experimental approach (B), Galliart and Shaw (1991) used two-choice tests to show that some sound parameters (e.g., intensity) were important for a male's mating success. These results were later confirmed in female phonotaxis tests (Galliart & Shaw 1992) and elaborated in two-choice preference tests with computer-generated song models on the same species (Galliart & Shaw 1996). Similar results were obtained by Tauber et al. (2001) using Phaneroptera nana. Tuckerman et al. (1993) tested females of Scudderia curvicauda (de Geer, 1773) in sequential two-choice tests. Again, females preferred those male signals that contained more sound energy (higher number of syllables). Bailey and his co-workers studied duets of some Australian species with complicated songs. In Elephantodeta Brunner von Wattenwyl, 1878, they discovered a male acoustic satellite behaviour whereby a second, competing satellite-male produces a very short song during an interval of the long song of the first alpha-male. Satellite-males can sometimes phonotactically attract the female, thereby stealing her from the alpha-male (Bailey & Field 2000). In a species of Caedicia Stal, 1874 the female answers with a variable number of clicks which may indicate her motivation (Bailey & Hammond 2004). Similar results (whereby females answered with a variable number of clicks) were also obtained by Villarreal & Gilbert (2013) in Scudderia pistillata Brunner von Wattenwyl, 1878, another species with a relatively complicated song.

Fig. 9.

Stridulatory movement of female Poecilimon gracilis together with male-female duet (sound). Oscillograms of stridulatory movement and song [synchronous registration of left tegmen movement and sound (upper line: upward deflection represents opening, downward closing; lower line: sound)].

f09_07.jpg

According to experimental studies in Barbitistini, the triggers for releasing a female response can be placed into three groups:

  • 1) Complete male calling song: in all of these species, the male produces a mono-syllabic calling song, and the complete song must be received for the females to respond. However, females of different species use either the beginning (Leptophyes albovittata (Kollar, 1833) Zhantiev & Korsunovskaya 1986; Poecilimon ornatus (Schmidt, 1850) Heller et al. 1997a) or possibly a certain duration after beginning (not excluded for Leptophyes albovittata) or end (Isophya stepposa Bey-Bienko, 1954; Zhantiev & Korsunvskaya 1986) of this syllable as trigger to set the timing of her reply. In cases where females respond to the beginning of the male song, there can be problems with song recognition, because the female has no information about the structure of that song she responds to (see Heller et al. 1997a).

  • (2) In this case, the female seems to use the pronounced syllable endings as triggers [Poecilimon affinis (Frivaldsky, 1867) Helversen & Wendler 2000, Helversen et al. 2015].

  • (3) In these cases, there is a trigger syllable in the male song that is ‘announced’ by an unusually long pause (Ancistrura nigrovittata; Dobler et al. 1994a; see also Barbitistes species; Stumpner & Meyer 2001). Also, syllables with unusually high amplitude may act as a trigger (Polysarcus denticauda: Faber 1953, Heller 1990).

Conclusions

The results of this comparison show that male calling songs in Phaneropterinae are very diverse, ranging from extremely simple to extraordinarily complex. However, on a world-wide scale, the majority of phaneropterine species with known sounds do not produce complex songs, contrary to the comments of some authors (e.g., Walker 2004, Rentz 2010). Indeed, many species have quite simple songs. However, one should keep in mind that phaneropterine acoustic communication may actually be more complicated than is currently realized, simply because intraspecific variation of song patterns is insufficiently studied and understood. Many phaneropterine species can modify their songs, and/or have special sounds when interacting and duetting. The very inconspicuous imitation of female answers by males (see above) represents one example. Possibly some phaneropterine species with complicated male songs have integrated some female imitating sounds into the male calling song. For example, males of some species include female-like patterns in their calling songs, even when no females are present. The patterns of male phaneropterine songs appear often to be less fixed than in crickets or other tettigonioids. Another topic needing attention is the nearly unexplored possibility of change in song intensity (observed by e.g., Spooner 1995). This option would be valuable to duetting species, where the singers have some information about the distance to a partner. In addition, both sexes may even switch to vibrations or use mainly the vibrational component of the song, which may make them less conspicuous to competitors and predators. Judging from the as yet low number of detailed studies of duetting species, surprising acoustic strategies may be discovered in future. Complicated songs probably evolved several times independently in the Phaneropterinae. This hypothesis is supported by the scattered occurrence of such songs on all continents, often in presumably unrelated genera. Also, some Barbitistini species with complex songs are closely related to species with simple songs (see phylogenetic tree in Ullrich et al. 2010). Of course, sexual selection by females choosing to respond preferentially to special song types could be an important evolutionary force driving increasing song complexity, but probably only in combination with other unknown ecological and behavioural factors.

For other important questions the data are similarly limited. What are the advantages and disadvantages of duetting? Duetting may have low costs in terms of energy and risk. For example, signal duty cycle (percentage of time spent actually sound emitting against duration of all intervals) can be low if there is no response; see Heller & Helversen 1993). For flight-adept species, a bi-directional communication might be “very efficient, because a male can search large areas for responding females by a combination of singing and flying” (Helversen et al. 2012). However, this hypothesis awaits field confirmation. In fact, very little is known about the mate-location behaviour of long-winged phaneropterines, and the relative costs and benefits of different strategies.

For duetting to be lost in a species (i.e., evolutionary reversal to uni-directional communication), a high population density may be required, as indicated by the experiments of Helversen et al. (2012). Long ago, Hartley et al. (1974) speculated about population density as an important factor for different communication systems found in Ephippigerini (Bradyporinae). Population density may be also one factor (among others) determining which sex will move during the duet. There are obviously many different strategies among phaneropterines with moving or stationary males and/or females, and intermediate strategies (Zhantiev & Korsunovskaya 1986, Robinson 1990, Spooner 1995). Here not only the cost of calling in terms of energy and risk have to be considered (see Heller 1992), but also the costs and benefits obtained from multiple matings (number of offspring and increased/reduced survival by obtaining/producing a nutritious spermatophore). These questions demonstrate how deeply the acoustical communication is embedded into the mating biology of each species.

Acknowledgements

Our thanks go to Matthias Hennig, Berlin, for his help with digitalisation and to Wang Hanqiang for his comments during the identification of Himertula. For helpful comments on the manuscript we are grateful to Glenn Morris and an anonymous referee.

References

1.

R.D. Alexander 1960. Sound communication in Orthoptera and Cicadidae, pp. 38–92. In: W.E. Lanyon , W.N. Tavolga (Eds). Animal Sounds and Communication. Publications American Institute of Biological Sciences. Vol. 7. Google Scholar

2.

W.J. Bailey 1990. The ear of the bushcricket, pp. 217–247. In: W.J. Bailey , D.C.F. Rentz (Eds). The Tettigoniidae: Biology Systematics and Evolution. Crawford House Press, Bathurst. Google Scholar

3.

W.J. Bailey 1991. Acoustic Behaviour of Insects. Chapman and Hall, London. Google Scholar

4.

W.J. Bailey 2003. Insect duets: underlying mechanisms and their evolution. Physiological Entomology 28: 157–174. Google Scholar

5.

W.J. Bailey , G. Field 2000. Acoustic satellite behaviour in the Australian bushcricket Elephantodeta nobilis (Phaneropterinae, Tettigoniidae, Orthoptera). Animal Behaviour 59: 361–369. Google Scholar

6.

W.J. Bailey , T.J. Hammond 2004. Female reply strategies in a duetting Australian bushcricket, Caedicia sp. (Phaneropterinae: Tettigoniidae: Orthoptera). Journal of Experimental Biology 207: 803–811. Google Scholar

7.

W.J. Bailey , C. MacLeay , T. Gordon 2006. Acoustic mimicry and disruptive alternative calling tactics in an Australian bushcricket (Caedicia; Phaneropterinae; Tettigoniidae; Orthoptera): does mating influence male calling tactic? Physiological Entomology 31: 201–210. Google Scholar

8.

G. Ya. Bey-Bienko 1954. Orthoptera Vol. II No. 2 Tettigonioidea Phaneropterinae, Fauna of the U.S.S.R. Zoological Institute Akademii Nauk SSSR, 381pp; [English translation 1965 Jerusalem (Israel Program for Scientific Translations)]. Google Scholar

9.

H. Braun 2011. A brief revision of brachypterous Phaneropterinae of the tropical Andes (Orthoptera, Tettigoniidae, Odonturini). Zootaxa 2991: 35–43. Google Scholar

10.

C. Brunner von Wattenwyl 1878. Monographie der Phaneropteriden. Brockhaus, Wien. 401pp. Google Scholar

11.

R.C. Del Castillo , D.T. Gwynne 2009. Corrigendum. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 22: 1151–1152. Google Scholar

12.

S. Dobler , K.-G. Heller , O.v. Helversen 1994a. Song pattern recognition and an auditory time window in the female bushcricket Ancistrura nigrovittata (Orthoptera: Phaneropteridae). Journal of Comparative Physiology A Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology 175: 67–74. Google Scholar

13.

S. Dobler , A. Stumpner , K.-G. Heller 1994b. Sex-specific spectral tuning for the partner's song in the duetting bushcricket Ancistrura nigrovittata (Orthoptera: Phaneropteridae). Journal of Comparative Physiology A Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology 175: 303–310. Google Scholar

14.

S Drosopoulos , M.F. Claridge (Eds) 2006. Insect Sounds and Communication: Physiology, Behaviour, Ecology And Evolution. [Contemporary Topics In Entomology.]. Taylor & Francis, Boca Raton. Google Scholar

15.

D.C. Lades , D. Otte , M.M. Cigliano , H. Braun 2014. Orthoptera Species File Online. Version 2.0/5.0. available on  http://Orthoptera.SpeciesFile.org (May 2013-November 2014). Google Scholar

16.

A.W. Ewing 1989. Arthropod Bioacoustics. Edinburgh University Press, Edinburgh. Google Scholar

17.

A. Faber 1953. Laut-und Gebärdensprache bei Insekten. Orthoptera (Geradflügler). Teil I. Mitteilung aus dem Staatlichen Museum für Naturkunde in Stuttgart 287: 1–198. Google Scholar

18.

P.L. Galliart , K.C. Shaw 1991. Role of weight and acoustic parameters, including nature of chorusing, in the mating success of males of the katydid, Amblycotypha parvipennis (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae). Florida Entomologist 74: 453–464. Google Scholar

19.

P.L. Galliart , K.C. Shaw 1992. The relation of male and female acoustic parameters to female phonotaxis in the katydid, Amblycotypha parvipennis. Journal of Orthoptera Research 1: 109–115. Google Scholar

20.

P.L. Galliart , K.C. Shaw 1996. The effect of variation in parameters of the male calling song of the katydid, Amblycotypha parvipennis (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae), on female phonotaxis and phonoresponse. Journal of Insect Behavior 9: 841–855. Google Scholar

21.

A.V. Gorochov 1995. System and evolution of the suborder Ensifera (Orthoptera). Part II. Trudy Zoologitscheskogo Instituta Akad. Nauk SSSR [= Proc. Zool. Inst. Acad. Sci. USSR] 260: 1–213. Google Scholar

22.

J.C. Hartley , D.J. Robinson , A.C. Warne 1974. Female response song in the ephippigerines Steropleurus stali and Platystolus obvius (Orthoptera, Tettigoniidae). Animal Behaviour 22: 382–389. Google Scholar

23.

K.-G. Heller 1988. Bioakustik der europäischen Laubheuschrecken. J. Margraf, Weikersheim. Google Scholar

24.

K.-G. Heller 1990. Evolution of song pattern in east Mediterranean Phaneropterinae: constraints by the communication system, pp. 130–151. In: W.J. Bailey , D.C.F. Rentz (Eds). The Tettigoniidae: Biology, Systematics and Evolution. Crawford House Press, Bathurst. Google Scholar

25.

K.-G. Heller 1992. Risk shift between males and females in the pair-forming behavior of bushcrickets. Naturwissenschaften 79: 89–91. Google Scholar

26.

K.-G. Heller , D.v. Helversen 1986. Acoustic communication in phaneropterid bushcrickets: species-specific delay of female stridulatory response and matching male sensory time window. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology 18: 189–198. Google Scholar

27.

K.-G. Heller , D.v. Helversen 1993. Calling behavior in bushcrickets of the genus Poecilimon with differing communication systems (Orthoptera: Tettigonioidea: Phaneropteridae). Journal of Insect Behavior 6: 361–377. Google Scholar

28.

K.-G. Heller , C. Hemp 2014. Fiddler on the tree - a bush-cricket species with unusual stridulatory organs and song. PLOS one 9: e92366. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0092366. Google Scholar

29.

K.-G. Heller , O. Korsunovskaya 2009. Systematics and bioacoustics of the genus Lithodusa (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae) including the description of a new species from Turkey and comments on the classification of the Drymadusini. Journal of Orthoptera Research 18: 5–13. Google Scholar

30.

K.-G. Heller , O.v. Helversen , M. Sergejeva 1997a. Indiscriminate response behaviour in a female bushcricket: sex role reversal in selectivity of acoustic mate recognition? Naturwissenschaften 84: 252–255. Google Scholar

31.

K.-G. Heller , J. Schul , S. Ingrisch 1997b. Sex-specific differences in song frequency and tuning of the ears in some duetting bushcrickets (Orthoptera: Tettigonioidea: Phaneropteridae). Zoology (Jena) 100: 110–118. Google Scholar

32.

K.-G. Heller , O.S. Korsunovskaya , H. Sevgili , R.D. Zhantiev 2006. Bioacoustics and systematics of the Poecilimon heroicus-group (Orthoptera: Phaneropteridae: Barbitistinae). European Journal of Entomology 103: 853–865. Google Scholar

33.

K.-G. Heller , T.D. Ostrowski , C. Hemp 2010. Singing and hearing in Aerotegmina hilimandjarica (Tettigoniidae: Hexacentrinae), a species with unusual low carrier frequency of the calling song. Bioacoustics 19: 195–210. Google Scholar

34.

K.-G. Heller , L. Willemse , B. Ode , M. Volleth , R. Feist , K. Reinhold 2011. Bioacoustics and systematics of the Poecilimon hamatus group (Tettigonioidea: Phaneropteridae: Poecilimon: Hamatopoecilimon n. subg. Journal of Orthoptera Research 20: 81–95. Google Scholar

35.

K.-G. Heller , C. Hemp , C.-x Liu , M. Volleth 2014. Taxonomic, bioacoustic and faunistic data on a collection of Tettigonioidea from Eastern Congo (Insecta: Orthoptera). Zootaxa 3785: 343–376. Google Scholar

36.

D.v. Helversen , G. Wendler 2000. Coupling of visual to auditory cues during phonotactic approach in the phaneropterine bushcricket Poecilimon affinis. Journal of Comparative Physiology A Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology 186: 729–736. Google Scholar

37.

D.v. Helversen , J. Schul , H.U. Kleindienst 2001. Male recognition mechanism for female responses implies a dilemma for their localisation in a phaneropterine bushcricket. Journal of Comparative Physiology A Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology 186: 1153–1158. Google Scholar

38.

D.v. Helversen , O.v. Helversen , K.-G. Heller 2012. When to give up responding acoustically in Poecilimon bush-crickets: a clue to population density. Articulata 27: 57–66. Google Scholar

39.

O.v. Helversen , N. Elsner 1977. The stridulatory movements of acridid grasshoppers recorded with an opto-electronic device. Journal of Comparative Physiology 122: 53–64. Google Scholar

40.

O.v. Helversen , D.v. Helversen , K. Rohrseitz , I. Koopmann , A. Stumpner , K.-G. Heller 2015. Signals and sex-specific active space in a duetting insect (Orthoptera: Phaneropteridae: Poecilimon affinis). Journal of Orthoptera Research 24: 19–28. Google Scholar

41.

C. Hemp , K.L. Voje , K.-G. Heller , A. Hemp 2009. Biogeography, phylogeny and acoustics of the flightless bush-crickets of the East African genus Monticolaria Sjostedt, 1909, with the description of a new species (Orthoptera: Phaneropterinae). Zoological Journal of the Linnean Society 156: 494–506. Google Scholar

42.

C. Hemp , K.-G. Heller , E. Warchalowska-Sliwa , B. Grzywacz , A. Hemp 2013. Biogeography, ecology, acoustics and chromosomes of East African Eurycorypha Stål species (Orthoptera, Phaneropterinae) with the description of new species. Organisms Diversity & Evolution 13: 373–395. Google Scholar

43.

F. Huber , T.E Moore , W. Loher 1989. Cricket Behavior and Neurobiology. Cornell University Press, NewYork. Google Scholar

44.

S. Ingrisch 1998. A review of the Elimaeini of Western Indonesia, Malay Peninsula and Thailand (Ensifera, Tettigoniidae, Phaneropterinae). Tijdschrift voor Entomologie 141: 65–108. Google Scholar

45.

S. Ingrisch 2011. New taxa of Elimaeini (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae: Phaneropterinae) from South East Asia. Zootaxa 2935: 1–25. Google Scholar

46.

S. Ingrisch , A.V. Gorochov 2007. Review of the genus Hemielimaea Brunner von Wattenwyl, 1878 (Orthoptera, Tettigoniidae). Tijdschrift voor Entomologie 150: 87–100. Google Scholar

47.

I.S. Iorgu 2012. Acoustic analysis reveals a new cryptic bush-cricket in the Carpathian Mountains (Orthoptera, Phaneropteridae). ZooKeys 254: 1–22. Google Scholar

48.

L. Kang , C.-x. Liu , X.-w. Liu 2014. Fauna Sinica Insecta Vol. 57 Orthoptera Tettigoniidae Phaneropterinae. 574 pp. Google Scholar

49.

O.S. Korsunovskaya 2005. Coding of species-specific information in sound signals of the tettigoniids (Orthoptera, Tettigoniidae). Proceedings of the Russian Entomological Society = Trudy russkogo èntomologicheskogo obshchestva 76: 141–153. Google Scholar

50.

O.S. Korsunovskaya 2008. Sound signalling in katydids and bushcrickets (Orthoptera, Tettigonioidea). Report I. Zoologicheskii Zhurnal 87: 1453–1471. (translated Entomological Review 88: 1032–1050). Google Scholar

51.

L. Leroy 1970. Diversités d'aspects et la dissymetrie des rapes de stridulation des Insectes Orthopteres Phaneropterinae. Compte Rendu Hebdomadaire des Séances de l'Académie des Sciences. D 270: 96–99. Google Scholar

52.

B. Massa 2013. Diversity of leaf katydids (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae: Phaneropterinae) of Dzanga-Ndoki National Park, Central African Republic, with selected records from other African countries. Journal of Orthoptera Research 22: 125–152. Google Scholar

53.

A. Michelsen , K.-G. Heller , A. Stumpner , K. Rohrseitz 1994. A new biophysical method to determine the gain of the acoustic trachea in bushcrickets. Journal of Comparative Physiology A Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology 175: 145–151. Google Scholar

54.

F. Montealegre-Z. 2012 Reverse stridulatory wing motion produces highly resonant calls in a neotropical katydid (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae: Pseudophyllinae). Journal of Insect Physiology 58: 116–124. Google Scholar

55.

F. Montealegre-Z. , T. Jonsson , K.A. Robson-Brown , M. Postles , D. Robert 2012. Convergent evolution between insect and mammalian audition. Science 338: 968–971. Google Scholar

56.

J.D. Mugleston , H. Song , M.F. Whiting 2013. A century of paraphyly: a molecular phylogeny of katydids (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae) supports multiple origins of leaf-wings. Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution 69: 1120–1134. Google Scholar

57.

D.A. Nickle 1976. Interspecific differences in frequency and other physical parameters of pair-forming sounds of bush katydids (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae: Phaneropterinae). Annals of the Entomological Society of America 69: 1136–1144. Google Scholar

58.

H. Nocke 1975. Physical and physiological properties of the tettigoniid (‘grasshopper’) ear. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 100: 25–58. Google Scholar

59.

K.M. Orci 2007 Female preferences for male song characters in the bushcricket Isophya camptoxypha (Orthoptera, Tettigonioidea). Journal of Insect Behavior 20: 503–513. Google Scholar

60.

OSFO (Orthoptera Species File Online) = Eades et al. 2014. Google Scholar

61.

U. A Palghat ,. M. Kössl , M. Nowotny 2012. Tonotopically arranged traveling waves in the miniature hearing organ of bushcrickets. PLoS ONE 7(2): e31008. Google Scholar

62.

G. Pungúr 1886. Beiträge zur Naturgeschichte einer wenig bekannten Laubheuschreckenart. Rovart Lapok 3: 49–55. Google Scholar

63.

D.R. Ragge 1956. Arevision of the genus Oxyecous Chopard, 1935 (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae). Proceedings of the Royal Entomological Society of London (B) 25: 183–190. Google Scholar

64.

D.R. Ragge 1961. A revision of the genus Ducetia Stål (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae). Bulletin of the British Museum (Natural History), Entomology Series 10: 171–208. Google Scholar

65.

D.R. Ragge , W.J. Reynolds 1998. The Songs of the Grasshoppers and Crickets of Western Europe. Harley Books, Colchester, Essex. Google Scholar

66.

D.C.F. Rentz 2010. A Guide to the Katydids of Australia. CSIRO Publishing, Collingwood. Google Scholar

67.

C.V. Riley 1874. Sixth annual report on the noxious, beneficial, and other insects, of the State of Missouri, made to the State Board of Agriculture, pursuant to an appropriation for this purpose from the legislature of the State. 170 + xii pp.; Jefferson City. Google Scholar

68.

T. Robillard , F. Montealegre-Z. , L. Desutter-Grandcolas , P Grandcolas ,. D. Robert 2013. Mechanisms of high-frequency song generation in brachypterous crickets and the role of ghost frequencies. Journal of Experimental Biology 216: 2001–2011. Google Scholar

69.

D.J. Robinson 1990. Acoustic communication between the sexes in bushcrickets, pp. 112–129. In: W.J. Bailey , D.C.F. Rentz (Eds). The Tettigoniidae: Biology, Systematics and Evolution. Crawford House Press, Bathurst. Google Scholar

70.

D.J. Robinson , J. Rheinlaender , J.C. Hartley 1986. Temporal parameters of male female sound communication in Leptophyes punctatissima. Physiological Entomology 11: 317–324. Google Scholar

71.

W. Rössler , A. Hübschen , J. Schul , K. Kalmring . 1994. Functional morphology of bushcricket ears: comparison between two species belonging to the Phaneropterinae and Decticinae (Insecta, Ensifera). Zoomorphology 114: 39–46. Google Scholar

72.

SINA = Walker 2013 Google Scholar

73.

J.D. Spooner 1968. Pair-forming acoustic systems of phaneropterine katydids (Orthoptera, Tettigoniidae). Animal Behaviour 16: 197–212. Google Scholar

74.

J.D. Spooner 1995. Pair-forming phonotaxic strategies of phaneropterine katydids (Tettigoniidae: Phaneropterinae). Journal of Orthoptera Research 4: 127–129. Google Scholar

75.

J. Strauß , A.W. Lehmann , G.U.C. Lehmann 2014. Sensory evolution of hearing in tettigoniids with differing communication systems. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 27: 200–213. Google Scholar

76.

A. Stumpner , S. Meyer 2001. Songs and the function of song elements in four duetting bushcricket species (Ensifera, Phaneropteridae, Barbitistes). Journal of Insect Behavior 14: 511–534. Google Scholar

77.

A. Stumpner , K.-G. Heller 1992. Morphological and physiological differences of the auditory system in three related bushcrickets (Orthoptera, Phaneropteridae, Poecilimon). Physiological Entomology 17: 73–80. Google Scholar

78.

E. Tauber , M.P. Pener 2000. Song recognition in female bushcrickets Phaneroptera nana. Journal of Experimental Biology 203: 597–603. Google Scholar

79.

E. Tauber , D. Cohen , M.D. Greenfield , M.P. Pener 2001. Duet singing and female choice in the bushcricket Phaneroptera nana. Behaviour 138: 411–430. Google Scholar

80.

H.M. ter Hofstede , J.H. Fullard 2008. The neuroethology of song cessation in response to gleaning bat calls in two species of katydids, Neoconocephalus ensiger and Amblycorypha oblongifolia. Journal of Experimental Biology 211: 2431–2441. Google Scholar

81.

H.M. ter Hofstede , E. Kalko . J.H. Fullard 2010. Auditory-based defence against gleaning bats in neotropical katydids (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae). Journal of Comparative Physiology A Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology 196: 349–358. Google Scholar

82.

J.F. Tuckerman , D.T. Gwynne , G.K. Morris 1993. Reliable acoustic cues for female mate preference in a katydid (Scudderia curvicauda, Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae). Behavioral Ecology 4: 106–113. Google Scholar

83.

B. Ullrich , K. Reinhold , O. Niehuis , B. Misof 2010. Secondary structure and phylogenetic analysis of the internal transcribed spacers 1 and 2 of bush crickets (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae: Barbitistini). Journal of Zoological Systematic and Evolutionary Research 48: 219–228. Google Scholar

84.

S.M. Villarreal , C. Gilbert 2013. Female acoustic reply to variation in the male call in a duetting katydid, Scudderia pistillata. Behaviour 150: 525–546. Google Scholar

85.

T.J. Walker 2004. The uhleri group of the genus Amblycorypha (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae): extraordinarily complex songs and new species. Journal of Orthoptera Research 13: 169–183. Google Scholar

86.

T.J. Walker 2013. Crickets. In: Singing Insects of North America,  http://entnemdept.ifas.ufl.edu/walker/Buzz/katydids.htm  Google Scholar

87.

T.J. Walker , D. Dew 1972. Wing movement of calling katydids: Fiddling finesse. Science 178: 174–176. Google Scholar

88.

T.J. Walker , T.G. Forrest , J.D. Spooner 2003. The rotundifolia complex of the genus Amblycorypha (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae): songs reveal new species. Annals of the Entomological Society of America 96: 433–447. Google Scholar

89.

R.D. Zhantiev , O.S. Korsunovskaya 1986. Sound communication in bush crickets (Tettigoniidae: Phaneropterinae) of the European part of the USSR. Zoologicheskii Zhurnal 65: 1151–1163 (in Russian). Google Scholar

90.

U. Zimmermann , J. Rheinlaender , D. Robinson 1989. Cues for male phonotaxis in the duetting bushcricket Leptophyes punctatissima. Journal of Comparative Physiology A Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology 164: 621–628. Google Scholar
Klaus-Gerhard Heller, Claudia Hemp, Sigfrid Ingrisch, and Chunxiang Liu "Acoustic Communication in Phaneropterinae (Tettigonioidea) - A Global Review with Some New Data," Journal of Orthoptera Research 24(1), 7-18, (1 July 2015). https://doi.org/10.1665/034.024.0103
Published: 1 July 2015
KEYWORDS
amplitude modulation
carrier frequency
courtship
duetting
female response
katydid
mating
Back to Top