BioOne.org will be down briefly for maintenance on 17 December 2024 between 18:00-22:00 Pacific Time US. We apologize for any inconvenience.
Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 April 2005 PARENTAL NEST DEFENSE ON VIDEOTAPE: MORE REALITY THAN “MYTH“
Pamela J. Pietz, Diane A. Granfors
Author Affiliations +

Predation is recognized as the primary source of nest mortality in most passerine species (e.g. Ricklefs 1969, Martin 1992a); thus, it is no surprise that parental nest defense has received considerable scientific attention (see below). By nest defense, we refer to any parental behavior that decreases the probability that a predator (or brood parasite) will harm the nest contents and that simultaneously entails some cost to the bird engaged in the behavior—either by increasing the bird's risk of injury or death (Montgomerie and Weatherhead 1988) or by at least increasing its expenditure of time and energy (Buitron 1983).

Bradley and Marzluff (2003) used an innovative combination of approaches to investigate potential nest predation by three species of rodents; in the discussion section of that paper, they made the general assertion that “nest defense by parent birds seems to be more myth than reality.“ That statement is at odds with a vast scientific literature on the topic that spans several decades (e.g. Skutch 1955, reviews by Montgomerie and Weatherhead 1988, Martin 1992b, Sealy et al. 1998). A simplistic search for the phrase “nest defense“ in one online database (Wildlife and Ecology Studies Worldwide) produced a list of >90 papers published on nest defense by birds for 1994-2004 alone. Fifty of those papers deal with nest defense by passerines. Although parental defense is often unsuccessful, especially in passerines, one cannot dismiss its existence or its potential value as a predator deterrent (e.g. Montgomerie and Weatherhead 1988, Martin 1992b).

Given the wealth of observational and experimental evidence of nest defense, one may wonder whether further discussion of Bradley and Marzluff's statement is even warranted. However, the authors supported their dismissal of nest defense with data from a relatively new research tool—miniature video cameras used to continuously monitor active nests. In the last several years, such cameras have provided unprecedented information on nest predators of passerines (e.g. Thompson et al. 1999, Pietz and Granfors 2000a, Granfors et al. 2001, Williams and Wood 2002, Liebezeit and George 2003, Renfrew and Ribic 2003, Stake and Cimprich 2003, Thompson and Burhans 2003, Schaefer 2004). We therefore thought it appropriate to discuss the suitability of this tool for studies of nest defense, and to present some additional data related to nest defense that we acquired with video cameras.

Form and frequency of nest defense

Bradley and Marzluff (2003) noted that “during 61 video-monitored predation events of passerine nests, F. R. Thompson III (pers. comm.) has rarely observed nest defense, and never successful defense, against bird, mammal, or snake predators at night or day.“ The authors cited one of our papers (Pietz and Granfors 2000a) to support their contention that “adult passerines will immediately flush without defending their nest when disturbed by predators during nighttime.“ In fact, although we did not explicitly say that in our paper, we have noticed that adult birds usually flush off the nest (during both day and night) shortly before a predator comes into view on videotape. However, in a number of cases, the adult birds fly back into the field of view, apparently trying to drive off the predator. We detected such nest defense (Table 1) on videotapes of 24 of 85 predation events documented during grassland studies in North Dakota and Minnesota (data collected with several collaborators in 1996–2001 and 2003). We documented defense by five species of grassland-nesting passerines against eight species of mammalian, avian, and reptilian predators. Defense occurred during both daytime and nighttime and was directed at mice, thirteen-lined ground squirrels, a Franklin's ground squirrel, a raccoon, a long-tailed weasel, Brown-headed Cowbirds, and plains garter snakes (scientific names in Table 1).

The instances of parental nest defense that we have detected on videotape can be qualitatively informative, but they do not necessarily provide a basis for estimating the frequency of parental nest defense. Several factors make it difficult to accurately quantify the proportion of cases in which nest defense occurred at our video-monitored nests. In our grassland habitats, we placed cameras so close to nests (10–30 cm to prevent vegetation from obscuring nest contents) that the field of view was usually restricted to the nest and a small area immediately around it. Only a small part of larger predators, like badgers (Taxidea taxus) and white-tailed deer (Odocoileus virginianus), were within the cameras' fields of view (Pietz and Granfors 2000a, b). Thus, we do not know whether the rarity of documented attacks on larger predators (only one case of defense during 24 nest predations by hawks, skunks, badgers, raccoon, canids, and deer) indicates an unwillingness to defend against animals of that size or the high probability that those attacks would be beyond camera view. Nest defense against smaller predators also may occur out of view, but >43% of 46 nest predations by smaller predators (mice or voles, ground squirrels, weasels, snakes, cowbirds) elicited nest defense that was captured on videotape.

By using wide-angle lenses or setting cameras farther from nests, researchers can obtain video images with larger fields of view (e.g. Brown et al. 1998, Thompson and Burhans 2003, Schaefer 2004). However, those images generally show less detail and thus make it more difficult to identify small predators. At night, the benefit of an increased field of view also depends on the proportion of the view that is illuminated by an infrared light source.

Even with a relatively large field of view, most camera systems used for monitoring nests are unlikely to document parental activities that prevent a predator from discovering the nest. Distraction displays, injury-feigning, alarmcalling, and mobbing generally occur away from the nest and can redirect a predator (e.g. Buitron 1983, Montgomerie and Weatherhead 1988) long before it is close enough to the nest to be detected on camera.

Aside from those difficulties in documenting nest defense, there is a more pedestrian reason we hesitate to assess rates of nest defense from our videotapes. To date, we have reviewed only a small fraction of ∼1,800 tapes obtained at >160 passerine nests. By recording continuously at a tape speed of four images per second, we obtained one tape every 24 h from the time a camera was deployed at a nest until the nest either failed or fledged young. To meet our initial research objectives, we reviewed in detail any tape with evidence of egg or nestling losses. It is possible that tapes we have not reviewed completely contain cases of successful nest defense during which no losses occurred. Because detailed tape reviews are very time-consuming, many predation studies may lack the resources to check all tapes for nonpredation events.

Despite those caveats, it is probably reasonable to use camera data to estimate a minimum frequency of parental defense at our grassland nests. During our studies, parental defense was detected at 26% of 81 camera-monitored nests that were affected by predator visits. The apparent lack of defense noted in another study (i.e. F. R. Thompson III pers. comm. in Bradley and Marzluff (2003) may reflect behavioral differences associated with other nesting species in different habitats responding to different predators. It is also possible that methodological differences (e.g. in camera systems, tape reviews) produced the disparity in our assessments of nest defense. In either case, the frequency of parental defense noted in our grassland studies should not be extrapolated to other nesting species, predator communities, or habitat types. On the other hand, for grassland-nesting passerines of the north-central United States, our data suggest that parental nest defense is relatively common.

Rewards and risks of nest defense

Some instances of nest defense that we documented were at least partially successful (i.e. some nest contents survived the attack). Among the 21 nests at which we videotaped nest defense, at least 5 successfully fledged young (Table 1). In one of those cases, described in Granfors et al. (2001), a female Bobolink vigorously chased and pecked a female Brown-headed Cowbird that was removing young from the Bobolink's nest. The Brown-headed Cowbird managed to remove three of four nestlings, but one nestling survived to fledge (Table 1, row 2). In another case, a Clay-colored Sparrow apparently thwarted predation by a long-tailed weasel once at the egg stage and again at the nestling stage. Although nest defense was not documented at the egg stage, the weasel's abrupt retreat from the nest, without removing any of the three Clay-colored Sparrow eggs, suggested that parental defense may have occurred outside the camera's field of view. Five days later, parental defense was documented at that nest (Table 1, row 13) as a weasel removed one 2-day-old nestling and injured another (which subsequently died). There were no further attacks on the nest, however, and the third nestling survived and fledged. That case of defense was especially remarkable, given the ability of weasels to kill adult birds (e.g. Keith 1961). It may be difficult to measure the relative value of rewards and risks involved in parental nest defense; however, it is clear that nest defense sometimes pays off.

The risks of nest defense were also documented on videotape, in that some nest predators killed parents as well as their young. In one case, an adult Savannah Sparrow was killed by a short-tailed or least weasel (Mustela erminea or M. nivalis). More surprisingly, after unsuccessfully defending her nestlings from ground squirrel attacks over a 2-day period (Table 1, row 5), a female Chestnut-collared Longspur was caught, killed, and removed from her nest by a thirteen-lined ground squirrel (fig. 2B in Pietz and Granfors 2000a). It was not clear whether those birds were killed because they attempted to stand their ground or were simply caught off guard. However, the potential danger to a bird that attempts to defend its nest is evident. Given that such relatively small predators are a threat to adult passerines, it is not surprising that parent birds frequently flush from the nest at the first indication of something approaching. In many cases, they probably do not know what is approaching the nest, and are less vulnerable assessing the situation on the wing or from a perch. Most of the parental defense we detected on videotape consisted of attacks launched several seconds after the adult had flushed from the nest.

Conclusions

Most video nest-monitors are poorly suited to quantify the full range of behaviors that parent birds use to defend their nests. Nevertheless, those cameras can provide glimpses of behaviors that we are rarely able to observe directly, and may provide minimum estimates of the frequency of nest defense. Our videotaped data on grassland passerines suggest that nest defense can be quite common. Comparing data among video studies might help elucidate the factors that contribute to variability in the occurrence and detection of parental nest defense.

Acknowledgments

Thanks to the many people who helped collect camera data during our grassland nest studies, especially G. B. Berkey, R. J. Fletcher, C. M. Graue, L. A. Joyal, S. Mkheidze, A. Samuelsen, and F. Y. Sargeant. Thanks also to those at Northern Prairie Wildlife Research Center and U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service, Regions 6 and 3, who supported the camera research, especially T. A. Grant, D. H. Johnson, S. L. Jones, E. M. Madden, and M. Winter. Helpful comments on the manuscript were provided by E. K. Bollinger, D. A. Buhl, L. D. Igl, D. H. Johnson, M. H. Sherfy, M. A. Sovada, P. J. Weatherhead, and an anonymous reviewer.

Literature Cited

1.

J. E. Bradley and J. M. Marzluff . 2003. Rodents as nest predators: Influences on predatory behavior and consequences to nesting birds. Auk 120:1180–1187. Google Scholar

2.

K. P. Brown, H. Moller, J. Innes, and P. Jansen . 1998. Identifying predators at nests of small birds in a New Zealand forest. Ibis 140:274–279. Google Scholar

3.

D. Buitron 1983. Variability in the responses of Black-billed Magpies to natural predators. Behaviour 87:209–236. Google Scholar

4.

D. A. Granfors, P. J. Pietz, and L. A. Joyal . 2001. Frequency of egg and nestling destruction by female Brown-headed Cowbirds at grassland nests. Auk 118:765–769. Google Scholar

5.

L. B. Keith 1961. A study of waterfowl ecology on small impoundments in southeastern Alberta. Wildlife Monographs, no. 6. Google Scholar

6.

J. R. Liebezeit and T. L. George . 2003. Comparison of mechanically egg-triggered cameras and time-lapse video cameras in identifying predators at Dusky Flycatcher nests. Journal of Field Ornithology 74:261–269. Google Scholar

7.

T. E. Martin 1992a. Breeding productivity considerations: What are the appropriate habitat features for management? Pages 455–473 in Ecology and Conservation of Neotropical Migrant Landbirds (J. M. Hagan III and D. W. Johnston, Eds.). Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, D.C.  Google Scholar

8.

T. E. Martin 1992b. Interaction of nest predation and food limitation in reproductive strategies. Pages 163–197 in Current Ornithology, vol. 9 (D. M. Power, Ed.). Plenum Press, New York. Google Scholar

9.

R. D. Montgomerie and P. J. Weatherhead . 1988. Risks and rewards of nest defence by parent birds. Quarterly Review of Biology 63:167–187. Google Scholar

10.

P. J. Pietz and D. A. Granfors . 2000a. Identifying predators and fates of grassland passerine nests using miniature video cameras. Journal of Wildlife Management 64:71–87. Google Scholar

11.

P. J. Pietz and D. A. Granfors . 2000b. White-tailed deer (Odocoileus viginianus) predation on grassland songbird nestlings. American Midland Naturalist 144:419–422. Google Scholar

12.

R. B. Renfrew and C. A. Ribic . 2003. Grassland passerine nest predators near pasture edges identified on videotape. Auk 120:371–383. Google Scholar

13.

R. E. Ricklefs 1969. An analysis of nesting mortality in birds. Smithsonian Contributions to Zoology, no. 9. Google Scholar

14.

T. Schaefer 2004. Video monitoring of shrubnests reveals nest predators. Bird Study 51:170–177. Google Scholar

15.

S. G. Sealy, D. L. Neudorf, K. A. Hobson, and S. A. Gill . 1998. Nest defense by potential hosts of the Brown-headed Cowbird: Methodological approaches, benefits of defense, and coevolution. Pages 194–211 in Parasitic Birds and Their Hosts: Studies in Coevolution (S. I. Rothstein and S. K. Robinson, Eds.). Oxford University Press, New York. Google Scholar

16.

A. F. Skutch 1955. The parental stratagems of birds. Ibis 97:118–142. Google Scholar

17.

M. M. Stake and D. A. Cimprich . 2003. Using video to monitor predation at Black-capped Vireo nests. Condor 105:348–357. Google Scholar

18.

F. R. Thompson III and D. E. Burhans . 2003. Predation of songbird nests differs by predator and between field and forest habitats. Journal of Wildlife Management 67:408–416. Google Scholar

19.

F. R. Thompson III, W. Dijak, and D. E. Burhans . 1999. Video identification of predators at songbird nests in old fields. Auk 116:259–264. Google Scholar

20.

G. E. Williams and P. Bohall Wood . 2002. Are traditional methods of determining nest predators and nest fates reliable? An experiment with Wood Thrushes (Hylocichla mustelina) using miniature video cameras. Auk 119:1126–1132. Google Scholar

Appendices

Table 1.

Parental defense documented at camera-monitored passerine nests in North Dakota and Minnesota

i0004-8038-122-2-701-t01.gif
Pamela J. Pietz and Diane A. Granfors "PARENTAL NEST DEFENSE ON VIDEOTAPE: MORE REALITY THAN “MYTH“," The Auk 122(2), 701-705, (1 April 2005). https://doi.org/10.1642/0004-8038(2005)122[0701:PNDOVM]2.0.CO;2
Received: 29 April 2004; Accepted: 15 November 2004; Published: 1 April 2005
Back to Top