Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 July 2007 Philopatry: A return to origins
John M. Pearce
Author Affiliations +

The word “philopatry” is a combination of the prefix philo (from the Greek philos, “beloved”) and the Latin patria, which means “fatherland” or “homeland.” Since the first English-language use of “philopatry” in an ornithological context by Huntington (1951), the term has been applied to two types of site-faithful behavior in birds. Closest to the etymological meaning is the first, “natal philopatry,” which means not dispersing far from, or returning to, a birthplace for reproduction. The second is “breeding philopatry,” which means returning to the same breeding area each year, though that area may not be an individual’s birth place (Shields 1982, Anderson et al. 1992). Therefore, any assessment of breeding philopatry likely includes some immigrant individuals, whereas assessments of natal philopatry include only locally hatched or born individuals.

In the past several years, the use of philopatry in the ornithological literature has widened further, to include site fidelity to nonbreeding areas, such as sites used for molting (Iverson et al. 2004), wintering (Robertson and Cooke 1999, Mehl et al. 2004), or stopover during migration (Merom et al. 2000). Use of the term “philopatry” to describe not only natal homing, but general site fidelity to both breeding and nonbreeding sites of individuals whose natal areas are unknown is, I believe, problematic and warrants reconsideration. This is because there are substantial genetic and demographic implications of philopatry in its purest and historical sense (i.e., natal philopatry), such as increased relatedness and population differentiation (Greenwood 1980, Quinn and White 1987, Avise et al. 1992). Indeed, the historical and theoretical discussions of natal philopatry focus on the behavior of limited dispersal from a birth place, how this promotes inbreeding, why inbreeding might be adaptive, and how a lack of gene flow might promote speciation (Mayr 1963, Shields 1982, Anderson et al. 1992). I believe that these population-genetic and demographic implications are potentially misapplied when “philopatry” is used to describe site-faithful behavior in general (see Pearce and Talbot 2006).

The potential danger of applying philopatry to non-natal and nonbreeding conditions is that it creates the expectation of certain outcomes, such as low dispersal rates, population genetic differentiation, and unique population segments, when such conditions may not exist. Given that most avian species do not molt, winter, or have migratory stopovers where they breed, I propose that the term “philopatry” and its genetic expectations be used only in relation to natal philopatry and not extended to (1) breeding- site fidelity of individuals whose natal areas are unknown and (2) nonbreeding areas where site- faithful behavior is observed. I believe this correctly distinguishes natal philopatry as a specific type of site fidelity with its own implications for population genetics and dynamics. Thus, philopatry should be viewed as synonymous and interchangeable with the terms “natal-site fidelity” and “natal philopatry,” and the term “breeding-site fidelity” should replace “breeding philopatry,” because it reflects the unknown natal origins of birds captured as adults. Although the broader condition of site fidelity may have implications for fitness, mate pairing, and population delineation—as examined in several studies (Robertson and Cooke 1999, Merom et al. 2000, Iverson et al. 2004, Mehl et al. 2004)—future investigations of site fidelity should be pursued without automatically invoking the term “philopatry” and assuming that the genetic and demographic connotations of natal philopatry also apply. In contrast to philopatry, the probability of fidelity (F) and dispersal (1 − F) are estimable parameters (Burnham 1993, Kendall and Nichols 2004), and the demographic and genetic consequences of site fidelity, regardless of where it occurs, can serve as hypotheses for testing with multiple data types (e.g., Arsenault et al. 2005). Such data mergers should enhance our understanding of the demographic, behavioral, and genetic implications of natal philopatry and site fidelity.

Acknowledgments

I thank N. Tileston of the Alaska Resources Library and Information Services for assisting with literature. P. Flint, A. Harding, D. Derksen, M. Lindberg, H. Wilson, F. Cooke, and one anonymous reviewer offered constructive commentary on the manuscript.

Literature Cited

1.

M. G. Anderson, J. M. Rhymer, and F. C. Rohwer . 1992. Philopatry, dispersal, and the genetic structure of waterfowl populations. Pages 365–395 in Ecology and Management of Breeding Waterfowl (B. D. J. Batt, A. D. Afton, M. G. Anderson, C. D. Ankney, D. H. Johnson, J. A. Kadlec, and G. L. Krapu, Eds.). University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis.  Google Scholar

2.

D. P. Arsenault, P. B. Stacey, and G. A. Hoelzer . 2005. Mark-recapture and DNA fingerprinting data reveal high breeding-site fidelity, low natal philopatry, and low levels of genetic population differentiation in Flammulated Owls (Otus flammeolus). Auk 122:329–337. Google Scholar

3.

J. C. Avise, R. T. Alisauskas, W. S. Nelson, and C. D. Ankney . 1992. Matriarchal population genetic structure in an avian species with female natal philopatry. Evolution 46:1084–1096. Google Scholar

4.

K. P. Burnham 1993. A theory for combined analysis of ring recovery and recapture data. Pages 199– 213 in Marked Individuals in the Study of Bird Population (J.-D. Lebreton and P. M. North, Eds.). Birkhauser Verlag, Basel, Switzerland.  Google Scholar

5.

P. J. Greenwood 1980. Mating systems, philopatry and dispersal in birds and mammals. Animal Behaviour 28:1140–1162. Google Scholar

6.

C. E. Huntington 1951. “Ortstreue” and subspecies formation in the Pied Flycatcher. Ecology 32:352–355. Google Scholar

7.

S. A. Iverson, D. Esler, and D. J. Rizzolo . 2004. Winter philopatry of Harlequin Ducks in Prince William Sound, Alaska. Condor 106:711–715. Google Scholar

8.

W. L. Kendall and J. D. Nichols . 2004. On the estimation of dispersal and movement of birds. Condor 106:720–731. Google Scholar

9.

E. Mayr 1963. Animal Species and Evolution. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts.  Google Scholar

10.

K. R. Mehl, R. T. Alisauskas, K. A. Hobson, and D. K. Kellett . 2004. To winter east or west? Heterogeneity in winter philopatry in a central- Arctic population of King Eiders. Condor 106:241–251. Google Scholar

11.

K. Merom, Y. Yom-Tov, and R. McClery . 2000. Philopatry to stopover site and body condition of transient Reed Warblers during autumn migration through Israel. Condor 102:441–444. Google Scholar

12.

J. M. Pearce and S. L. Talbot . 2006. Demography, genetics, and the value of mixed messages. Condor 108:474–479. Google Scholar

13.

T. W. Quinn and B. N. White . 1987. Analysis of DNA sequence variation. Pages 163–198 in Avian Genetics (F. Cooke and P. A. Buckley, Eds.). Academic Press, London.  Google Scholar

14.

G. J. Robertson and F. Cooke . 1999. Winter philopatry in migratory waterfowl. Auk 116:20–34. Google Scholar

15.

W. M. Shields 1982. Philopatry, Inbreeding, and the Evolution of Sex. State University of New York Press, Albany.  Google Scholar

Appendices

John M. Pearce "Philopatry: A return to origins," The Auk 124(3), 1085-1087, (1 July 2007). https://doi.org/10.1642/0004-8038(2007)124[1085:PARTO]2.0.CO;2
Received: 14 November 2006; Accepted: 1 March 2007; Published: 1 July 2007
Back to Top