NATURALISTS AND EVOLUTIONARY biologists have long been intrigued by traits that are seemingly maladaptive (Gould and Lewontin 1979, Zahavi and Zahavi 1997, Krüger et al. 2001). The brightly colored eggs laid by many species of birds provide a particularly striking example of such a trait (Wallace 1889, Lack 1958, Lancaster 1964, Underwood and Sealy 2002, Moreno and Osorno 2003, Kilner 2006).
Conspicuous eggs are thought to be costly because they can attract the attention of nest predators and brood parasites (Lancaster 1964, Montevecchi 1976, Götmark 1992, Yahner and Mahan 1996, Castilla et al. 2007, Magige et al. 2008, Westmoreland 2008). As with all visual signals, conspicuousness is not an absolute trait of the egg but depends on the reflectance characteristics of the egg and nesting background, the ambient light in which the egg is perceived, and the visual sensitivity of the signal receiver (Endler 1990). Thus, when referring to conspicuous eggs, we mean eggs that do not match their nesting background in either coloration or pattern. Such eggs could range in color from immaculate blue-green to white, to pink, as opposed to brown or heavily speckled eggs that should be relatively cryptic to visually orienting enemies (Bertram and Burger 1981, Castilla et al. 2007).
Although avian eggs exhibit a great diversity of colors and patterns (Fig. 1), conspicuous eggs are taxonomically widespread, ranging from tinamous (Tinamiformes) to ibises (Ciconiiformes), poorwills (Caprimulgiformes), doves (Columbiformes), and thrushes (Passeriformes) (Cabot 1992, Baicich and Harrison 1997). Here, we develop an alternative to previous hypotheses for the evolution of conspicuous eggs: the blackmail hypothesis proposes that conspicuous egg coloration coerces males into providing additional parental care to offset the increased risk of nest predation or brood parasitism.
PREVIOUS HYPOTHESES FOR CONSPICUOUS EGG COLOR
Early hypotheses for the taxonomically broad occurrence of conspicuous eggs included aposematism (Swynnerton 1916, Cott 1948), thermal regulation (Mcaldowie 1886, Bakken et al. 1978), increased visibility in cavities (von Haartman 1957, Holyoak 1969), and camouflage in the greenish light transmitted and scattered by vegetation (Lack 1958). Despite much work on the subject, the evolutionary explanation for conspicuous eggs remains unclear (Underwood and Sealy 2002, Kilner 2006).
A more recent hypothesis concentrates on blue-green eggshell coloration specifically, rather than conspicuousness in a broader sense. This sexual-signaling hypothesis has garnered the most research attention in recent years (e.g., Moreno et al. 2006a, b; Hanley et al. 2008; Soler et al. 2008; Hanley and Doucet 2009). It proposes (1) that blue-green egg pigmentation acts as a signal of female quality (Moreno and Osorno 2003) and (2) that males base investment decisions on this trait. Several studies have found that within species that lay blue-green eggs, males provide more parental care at nests with more chromatic eggs (Moreno et al. 2004, 2006b; Hanley et al. 2008; Soler et al. 2008), and there is some indication that higher-quality females lay eggs that are more intensely blue-green (Moreno et al. 2005, 2006a; Siefferman et al. 2006; Hanley et al. 2008). However, support for the sexual-signaling hypothesis is not universal (Krist and Grim 2007, López-Rull et al. 2007, Hanley and Doucet 2009), and the plausibility of a signaling function, especially in cavity-nesting species, has been questioned (Reynolds et al. 2009). In fact, any universal explanation seems unlikely, given that many of the proposed hypotheses are nonexclusive, and their relevance will probably depend on a species' natural history.
CONSPICUOUS EGGS AS BLACKMAIL
As an additional hypothesis, we propose that conspicuous egg coloration may force males to provide additional parental care to offset the increased risk of nest predation or brood parasitism. This hypothesis can explain multiple forms of conspicuous egg coloration and might even explain unusual nesting strategies (see below). In the sections below, we describe the blackmail hypothesis, generate a set of testable predictions, and provide several examples that appear to be consistent with this hypothesis.
We propose that sexual conflict load (Houston et al. 2005) may be imposed on males if females produce brightly colored, immaculate eggs in open nests. Highly conspicuous eggs create an increased risk of detection by nest predators or brood parasites (Montevecchi 1976, Castilla et al. 2007, Muñoz et al. 2007) that potentially forces males into providing additional parental care to keep the conspicuous eggs hidden from view. This form of black-mail is similar to the predation risk imposed by nestlings that beg loudly (Zahavi and Zahavi 1997); however, in this situation, conflict arises between the sexes rather than between parents and offspring. Conflict between the sexes occurs because the evolutionarily optimal level of parental investment for either parent is always lower than that of its partner (Houston et al. 2005, Lessells 2006). This sexual conflict results from the fact that the cost of parental investment (either gametic or through parental care) is paid by one parent, whereas the benefits of that investment are accrued by both parents.
When confronted with conspicuous eggs, males could be selected to either share in incubation or feed incubating females at the nest, thereby minimizing the amount of time that the nest is left unattended with its visually conspicuous contents. A third option is also available to males: resistance, by partial or complete reduction in parental investment. For many species, however, the relative benefits of resistance would be lower than the cost of increasing the level of investment in the current clutch because other mating opportunities in the current breeding season are unlikely (in species with a single clutch per year or with a low breeding density), or because investing in current reproduction is more beneficial than saving energy for future reproductive efforts (in species with low survivorship or in which mate-searching effort is high). It is important to note that although resistance to manipulation is a reasonable alternative in certain breeding strategies, the initial evolution of manipulative behavior depends on the costs and benefits associated with the manipulation itself and not on the resistance of manipulation (Lessells 2006). Moreover, although manipulation of parental investment is believed to be rare because of large opportunity costs associated with manipulating a mating partner (e.g., time lost from other activities that would increase fitness) and because manipulation itself may be difficult (Lessells 2006), female birds have the ability to alter egg and clutch characteristics, thereby providing a mechanism for the direct manipulation of males into providing additional care (Schwabl 1996, Smith and Härdling 2000, Komdeur et al. 2002, Groothuis et al. 2005).
The presence of conspicuous eggs has been bewildering because it is difficult to imagine why a female would lay conspicuous eggs if this behavior is damaging to its partner and increases predation risk to its own clutch. However, theoretical models have shown that damaging mating tactics can be advantageous in general (Johnstone and Keller 2000), especially if direct manipulation can create a situation in which the individual's partner will invest more (Lessells 2006). In some situations, it can prove advantageous for an individual to handicap itself if that behavior results in compensatory effort from its partner (Barta et al. 2002, Houston et al. 2005). For example, a female may elicit more care from its partner if it produces a clutch size beyond its optimum, leaving its own energetic resources too low to sufficiently care for the offspring (Smith and Härdling 2000). Females can also impose conflict load on their partners by increasing the steroid content of eggs, which results in increased begging behavior (Schwabl 1996, Kitaysky et al. 2001, Groothuis et al. 2005) and a potentially greater demand for paternal care (Dickens and Hartley 2007, Dickens et al. 2008). Similarly, males can manipulate females by molting during the nestling period, lowering their foraging ability and forcing females to compensate with higher levels of investment than would otherwise be optimal for them (Hemborg 1999). In some species, sexual conflict is so intense that one or both partners may desert their clutch, and thereby cause the death of all offspring because they skirted their parental duties (Pogány et al. 2008, Eldegard and Sonerud 2009). A recent comparative analysis showed that sexual conflict may even explain the evolution of mating systems and parental care in shorebirds (Thomas and Székely 2005). Considering the magnitude of existing empirical and theoretical work on sexual conflict, a blackmail mechanism for conspicuous egg color seems plausible.
If males respond to conspicuous eggs by sharing in incubation or by feeding females during incubation, this could reduce the risk of nest predation in two ways. First and most obviously, the conspicuous eggs would be concealed from the view of potential predators. Second, the increased nest attendance can lead to higher and more consistent egg temperatures (Martin et al. 2007), a shorter incubation period (Lifjeld and Slagsvold 1986, Nilsson and Smith 1988, Martin et al. 2007) and lower total predation risk (Bosque and Bosque 1995). More optimal incubation temperatures should also increase the quality or condition of offspring (Vleck et al. 1980, Booth 1987, Hepp et al. 2006). Consequently, both parents could experience direct fitness gains from increased male effort during incubation. Furthermore, this increase in offspring quality could result in high levels of investment during the nestling period because males should invest more in higher-quality offspring (Leonard and Horn 1996).
PREDICTIONS AND CONSEQUENCES OF THE BLACKMAIL HYPOTHESIS
The first prediction of the blackmail hypothesis is that in species with conspicuous eggs, parents should spend more time concealing their eggs. In species with uniparental male care, this would be accomplished by an increase in the amount of time that the male spends incubating. In species with biparental care, the mode of concealment should depend on the relative conspicuousness of male and female plumage: where males are drab, they could help either by incubating or by feeding females during incubation; where males are more colorful than females, they should help mainly via incubation feeding. That is, we do not expect conspicuous males to sit on conspicuous eggs, because this would be just as costly from a visual-detection standpoint. Previous work has shown that incubation feeding is beneficial to the female (Smith et al. 1989) and energetically expensive for the male (Lifjeld and Slagsvold 1986), which makes it a likely currency for manipulation.
A second prediction is that species with conspicuous eggs should have shorter incubation periods (controlling for biologically relevant factors) because of the higher, more consistent incubation temperature associated with keeping eggs covered. Interestingly, dramatically low predation risk, compared with that in species with cryptic eggs, might not be expected to result from blackmail driven nest attendance: visual conspicuousness of nest contents is offset by high rates of nest attendance in blackmail species and offset by egg crypsis in non-blackmail species.
A third prediction is that conspicuous egg coloration should be more common in species that are more susceptible to visually oriented predators or parasites. There is already support at the family level for the idea that conspicuous eggs are laid in conspicuous nests (Götmark 1993), and a recent review that controlled for the influence of shared ancestry showed that blue egg coloration is more prevalent in open-cup nests, which are at higher risk of nest predation than cavity nests (Kilner 2006). In nest-building species, the nest itself may also influence conspicuousness to predators (Solis and de Lope 1995). It seems reasonable to assume, however, that the active or passive detection of a nest with conspicuous contents would be more likely than the detection of a nest with cryptic contents (Yahner and Mahan 1996, Svagelj et al. 2003).
A fourth prediction concerns brood parasitism. Brown-headed Cowbirds (Molothrus ater; hereafter “cowbirds”) are generalist brood parasites with hundreds of host species, most of which are smaller than cowbirds in adult body size (Friedmann and Kiff 1985). This size difference makes it difficult for hosts to physically prevent cowbirds from laying eggs in their nests (Ellison and Sealy 2007). In a detailed study of a common small-bodied host, the Yellow Warbler (Dendroica petechia), high levels of nest attendance by host females did not prevent a cowbird from laying its parasitic egg, but it usually prevented the cowbird's normal behavior of removing a host egg in association with parasitic laying (Tewksbury et al. 2002). In other words, nest attendance had no effect on parasitism, but it prevented a reduction in host clutch size. If cowbirds respond similarly to nest attendance in other host species, we can predict a consequence of the increased nest attendance that should result from conspicuous-egg blackmail: across species of cowbird hosts, the difference in the number of host eggs in parasitized and unparasitized nests should be smaller in species with conspicuous eggs.
Our final prediction is that as a consequence of the risk associated with conspicuous eggs that are left exposed, species with conspicuous eggs should begin the incubation period earlier to keep their nest contents concealed. Thus, incubation or egg shading in these species are more likely to begin when the antepenultimate or penultimate egg is laid.
CONSISTENT EXAMPLES
Our goal in this section is to generate enough curiosity that readers might be encouraged to pursue robust tests of the blackmail hypothesis, through modeling, experiments, or comparative analyses. To that end, we offer several examples of studies that appear to be consistent with the blackmail hypothesis.
One intriguing example comes from an experimental study of nest attendance by Mourning Doves (Zenaida macroura), a sexually monochromatic species in which males provide most of the daytime incubation (Baicich and Harrison 1997). Westmoreland and Best (1986) compared the fates of continually incubated nests and intermittently incubated nests (achieved by deliberately flushing the incubating bird), crossed with a second factor of eggs being either naturally white or artificially colored to be more cryptic. They found that naturally colored white eggs were depredated significantly more than cryptic eggs in nests with intermittent incubation; however, there was no difference between naturally colored and cryptic eggs when both were continually incubated (Westmoreland and Best 1986). Nest concealment, nest height, relative light intensity, and age of eggs at discovery did not differ among the treatments. Interestingly, 85% of clutch failures resulted from visually oriented avian nest predators. The authors suggested that columbids may have evolved constant incubation in response to their conspicuous nest contents (Westmoreland and Best 1986). Interestingly, subsequent work has shown that Columbiformes have a shorter incubation period than would be predicted by body weight (Westmoreland et al. 1986).
A second form of consistent evidence comes from studies of nest attentiveness, mate-feeding behavior, and risk of nest predation. In a comparative analysis, incubation feeding was negatively related to observed nest predation rates in open-cup-nesting species (Martin and Ghalambor 1999). Although egg conspicuousness was not considered in the analysis, the underlying phenomenon may be that incubation feeding allows a cryptic female to spend more time covering conspicuous eggs. In general, there is strong evidence that female nest attentiveness is positively related to male incubation feeding in several species (von Haartman 1958, Lyon and Montgomerie 1985, Moreno and Carlson 1989, Smith et al. 1989, Hałupka 1994). These findings may explain how male help via incubation or incubation feeding in open-cup nests can mitigate the cost of conspicuous nest contents by increasing nest attendance when the risk of predation is high.
A third point of interest comes from tests of the sexual-signaling hypothesis. Several studies have found correlations between egg color and male feeding of nestlings (Moreno et al. 2006b, Hanley et al. 2008, Soler et al. 2008). Such data are consistent with the sexual-signaling hypothesis but also with the blackmail hypothesis. As described earlier, increased incubation attendance (caused by a sexual-signaling mechanism or by black-mail) can lead to better conditions for embryonic development and thus to higher-quality nestlings. Consequently, males that were blackmailed into helping keep conspicuous eggs covered during incubation might then be selected to increase their effort to feed the resulting high-quality brood (Leonard and Horn 1996). This relationship between egg color and paternal care of nestlings should be thought of not as a direct effect of blackmail but as an optimal male strategy if incubation feeding leads to an increase in chick quality.
BROADER IMPLICATIONS
Although we explored the blackmail hypothesis as a way to explain conspicuous egg coloration, and we recognize that blackmail could also operate in non-avian species with conspicuous eggs, the hypothesis itself need not be this limited in scope. There may also be implications for egg coloration in some species of brood parasites. If male songbirds provide more incubation care for nests with conspicuous eggs, a blackmail mechanism might provide a selective advantage to conspicuous eggs of brood parasites. For example, despite extensive variation in the coloration and pattern of host eggs, the eggs of the Bronzed Cowbird (Tangavius aeneus) are immaculate; likewise, Shiny Cowbirds (M. bonariensis) often lay immaculate eggs (Lowther 1995, Baicich and Harrison 1997, Lowther and Post 1999). This absence of egg mimicry has often been attributed to a limited history of host—parasite coevolution: hosts that have not evolved egg rejection and parasites that have not experienced selection for egg mimicry (Davies 2000). Yet even if hosts are under pressure to evolve egg rejection, it is possible that a blackmail response could cause an increase in incubation attendance at cryptic-egg nests that have been parasitized by cowbirds with conspicuous eggs. In other words, there may be fewer obstacles to the evolution of increased nest attendance (via blackmail) than to the evolution of egg recognition and rejection behavior, particularly if host species also benefit through decreased egg ejection by the parasite as outlined in our fourth prediction. In the long run, however, the evolution of rejection would still be advantageous in that it would eliminate the high costs associated with rearing the brood-parasitic nestling (Dearborn and Lichtenstein 2002).
Another extension of the blackmail hypothesis is that threats of egg destruction need not be based solely on conspicuousness to enemies: a similar threat may be incurred through the use of precarious nest sites, as documented in White Terns (Gygis alba), which usually lay their egg on a small branch without building a nest (Niethammer and Patrick 1998). In this situation, increased nest attendance reduces the chance that wind or contact will cause the egg to fall and break. Similarly, blackmail may be instigated through egg placement in potoos, which lay a single conspicuous white egg precariously in a slight depression of a branch or stump (Stiles and Skutch 1989).
SYNTHESIS
Despite decades of interest and inquiry, the evolution of conspicuous eggs remains something of a mystery. Our hypothesis proposes that conspicuous egg coloration may evolve as a form of sexual conflict. Thus, conspicuous eggs, which require higher parental investment to remain concealed, may be at a selective advantage in some species. The blackmail hypothesis may help explain interspecific variation in male incubation feeding in birds (Ricklefs 1974, Jawor and Breitwisch 2006). Moreover, a blackmail outlook on parental care might be expanded to other seemingly maladaptive traits such as precarious egg placement and nonmimetic brood-parasitic eggs.
It is important to note that blackmail and other hypotheses for the evolution of conspicuous egg coloration are not mutually exclusive. Across species, a particular trait can vary in the adaptive function it serves or in the constraints that limit its evolution, and conspicuous eggs certainly could be present in different species for different reasons (Kilner 2006). Indeed, multiple hypotheses may even reinforce each other in certain situations. For example, if paternal investment and egg conspicuousness have increased through blackmail, this mechanism may have produced enough intraspecific variation for other selection pressures to operate in some species (e.g., evaluation of relative female quality).
We are not suggesting that risk associated with egg detectability explains all occurrences of conspicuous eggs, but simply that it is a plausible hypothesis in certain situations. This idea presents a testable scenario for the presence of conspicuous egg coloration. In addition to perhaps explaining intriguing nesting behaviors in birds, this idea creates a new avenue for egg color research and may explain, at least in part, seemingly maladaptive egg coloration, which has perplexed naturalists for more than 100 years.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank M. Abdellah, I. Bobeica, D. Bradley, J. Cuthbert, A. Mistakidis, E. Schneider, and A. Zahavi for insightful discussions on this topic and comments on the manuscript. We appreciate the comments and feedback we received from K. McGraw, S. Sealy, and anonymous reviewers. We also thank R. Payne, R. Storer, and J. Hinshaw for providing access to the egg collection at the University of Michigan Museum of Zoology.