Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 July 2011 How Do Precise Adaptive Features Arise in Development? Examples with Evolution of Context-Specific Sex Ratios and Perfect Beaks
Alexander V. Badyaev
Author Affiliations +

The older writers on evolution were often staggered by the seeming necessity of accounting for the evolution of fine details, for example, the fine structure of all the bones. From the view that structure is never inherited as such, but merely as types of adaptive cell behavior which leads to particular types of structure under particular conditions, the difficulty to a considerable extent disappears. (Sewall Wright 1931:147).

ARE GENERATIVE PROCESSES CAUSES OR CONSEQUENCES OF EVOLUTION?

ORGANISMAL BIOLOGISTS ARE lucky to have in front of them, in the field or in the laboratory, an ever-present proving ground for theories that seek to explain the daunting diversity and complexity of the biological world. Foremost among these is the origin and evolution of precise adaptations that we observe around us. We are taught that evolution of these adaptations is accomplished by natural selection, and indeed there is good evidence that once an adaptive modification arises in a sufficient proportion of the population, natural selection acting on its heritable determinants becomes a powerful force for its evolutionary spread, establishment, and further modification. But where do actual adaptive solutions initially come from? How do selectively advantageous traits arise during development?

It is intuitively clear that many of the precise and complex behaviors, functions, and patterns that we see around us are largely emergent—that is, not “determined” in a conventional populationgenetics sense. It is naive to talk about genetic determination and adaptive evolution of every minute element of composite behaviors involved in the complex aerial display of a hummingbird, the coordinated dance sequence of manikins, the integration of neurological and biomechanical mechanisms that enable a swallow to skim the water surface in flight, or the ontogeny of complex camouflage in a sandpiper chick. Even the simplest of these phenomena involves precise coordination of millions of neurons and biochemical pathways, and there are simply not enough genes to specify every step and combination of these elements, much less their evolutionary retention. But even if there were billions of available genes for determination of these elements and of their context-dependent combinations, the population-genetics requirements for origination of such specification by natural selection (e.g., Crow and Kimura 1970, Gavrilets 2004, Lynch 2007, Lynch and Abegg 2010)—incremental fitness increases of intermediate (and not yet functional) stages, significant genetic penetrance of most elements, and very large population sizes—are unrealistic for most complex adaptations and organisms that we study and certainly impossible in any bird species. Yet these complex functions, patterns, and behaviors are precise; are repeatable within species, lineages, and families; and, thus, bear all the marks of evolutionary stability that we typically associate with genetic determination. Clearly, some other solutions are needed to explain their origins, and the search for these solutions could not be timelier.

Recent discoveries have established extraordinary conservation of basic molecular and cellular mechanisms over vast phylogenetic distances and organismal systems (e.g., Davidson 2006, Koonin and Wolf 2009, Shubin et al. 2009). For example, the evolution of the overwhelming majority of molecular and cellular mechanisms that generate contemporary adaptations in birds preceded not only the evolution of animals, but also the origin of multicellularity (Newman 2006, King et al. 2008). Furthermore, the speed with which complex adaptations appear (e.g., multiple origins of flight morphology within vertebrates) is difficult to reconcile with gradual accumulation of incremental steps, but is concordant with heritable changes in regulatory rearrangements of conserved developmental and functional modules (Müller and Newman 2003, Kirschner and Gerhart 2005, Reid 2007). Although current functional use and adaptive significance do not provide particularly deep insights into evolutionary origin and developmental causes (e.g., Gould and Lewontin 1979), much contemporary thinking in ornithology assumes that they do.

Our tendency to merge natural selection—the process arising from variation in the fit between members of a population and their environment—with the process of development is traceable to historical simplification of Darwin's (1859) original view of natural selection as the driver of both local adaptation and evolutionary diversification—the view that assumed direct inheritance of adaptive modifications and, thus, explicitly merged selection and development, such that selection shapes developmental variation that it subsequently sorts. Although Darwin famously considered developmental variation “slight, random, and abundant,” which was necessary for giving natural selection a creative role in evolution, less known is the fact that Darwin also excluded natural selection from processes that generate novelty, because natural selection, in his view, could act only on what is already common and established (Gould 2002, Reid 2007).

Regardless of the reasons, confounding natural selection and developmental variation necessarily forces us to neglect generative processes in evolution and the fact that behind every morphological, physiological, and behavioral change is a modification of the organisms's development, and exploratory and emergent processes play a defining role in this process (Baldwin 1902, Johnston and Gottlieb 1990, West-Eberhard 2003, Forgacs and Newman 2005, Kirschner and Gerhart 2005, Badyaev 2011). Indeed, remarkable conservation of generative processes over evolutionary time not only brings forth the question of how natural selection can accomplish tremendous phenotypic diversity with such a limited set of ingredients, but, most importantly, gives us a powerful insight into how evolutionary change actually proceeds. The central emerging theme is that autonomous developmental processes can create configurations that strongly reduce the number of evolutionary steps needed for the evolution of precise and novel adaptations and can also greatly facilitate their evolutionary retention. Consequently, novelties, diversification, and adaptation are best understood in terms of conserved developmental processes being stabilized and arranged by natural selection (Whyte 1965, Callebaut et al. 2007).

DISTINGUISHING ADAPTATION FROM ADAPTABILITY

Evolution proceeds by combining conserved and modular generative processes into hierarchical configurations at different times, places, and contexts by regulatory changes (King and Wilson 1975, Gould 1977, Alberch 1980, Stern 2000, Wilkins 2001, Carroll 2005, Davidson 2006, Gerhart and Kirschner 2007). These regulatory elements are subject to genetic changes and evolutionary fixation. However, because their core components are unchanging and do not deplete their phenotypic variability, evolution of such adaptive configurations does not preclude subsequent diversification and modification.

Explicit focus on the mechanisms that give rise to phenotypes brings forth the realization that the processes arising from fundamental biochemical and physical properties of biological tissues and their interaction with the environment provide the very basis for innovations, exploratory behaviors, and developmental plasticity (Forgacs and Newman 2005, Kicheva et al. 2007, Montell 2008, Newman 2010, Haigo and Bilder 2011) that can subsequently be exploited and stabilized by genetic drift and natural selection. Evolved plasticity and adaptability of these autonomous core processes greatly facilitate evolution because they account for initial environmental response, population spread, and epigenetic integration of novel adaptive solutions (Baldwin 1902, Schmalhausen 1938)—steps that are needed for eventual evolution of genetic integration. Whether prolonged periods of epigenetic integration of novel traits in a population are followed by their genetic integration depends on the consistency of natural selection, and, in many cases, epigenetic integration is a transient and reversible stage that, for example, enables populations to become established in novel environments. Regardless, this view calls for explicit separation of processes that produce adaptations from processes that maintain and modify them and, thus, lifts the burden of creativity from natural selection and places it on the functioning and developing phenotype (e.g., Piersma and van Gils 2010). In this framework, natural selection provides “boundary conditions” for developmental variability (West-Eberhard 2003, Müller 2007, Badyaev 2011) and can facilitate innovation by the mobilization and stabilization of core developmental components that are not themselves targets of selection.

Here, I suggest that decoupling natural selection and development gives powerful insights into the phenomenon of coexistence of precise adaptations—something that is assumed to be formed by natural selection by consistently eliminating developmental variability—and exceptional diversification in the same traits, implying an abundance of developmental variability that does not interfere with adaptive evolution. Such perspective removes two major stumbling blocks in understanding the evolution of complex adaptations. First is the necessity to account for incremental fitness increases of intermediate stages of complex (and yet not functional) features. Second is the necessity to account for coordinated evolution of every minute element of complex adaptation— something that commonly results in unrealistic expectations of evolutionary rates and population sizes.

The key to applying this perspective is to understand the nature of phenotypic variation that arises during development, and I illustrate it here with two particularly puzzling empirical phenomena in avian biology—the evolution of context-dependent adjustments of sex ratio in relation to laying order of eggs in a clutch and coexistence of precise adaptation and exceptional diversification in beaks. I show that the common resolution of both phenomena lies in the decoupling of natural selection and core developmental processes and the realization that both are guided by evolution of regulatory changes that determine the time, context, and congregation of conserved generative processes—organization that enables precise local adaptation as well as extensive diversification.

COMPLEX EGG-LAYING ORDERS: RECONCILING ADAPTIVE ADVANTAGE AND RARITY OF CONTEXT

The precision and speed with which sex bias in the laying order of eggs is expressed in appropriate environmental contexts is puzzling. For a trait as complex as sex-biased egg-laying order, this expression seems to require an improbable combination of coordinated evolutionary changes in follicle sequestration order, growth, ovulation, and sex-determination. Evolution of such a multistep adaptation would require unusually strong and persistent selection (i.e., a single predominant context) over many generations. Yet adaptive sex-biased egg-laying orders are commonly expressed in response to rare and variable contexts such as mate quality, seasonal changes, resource abundance, and population establishment (Ankney 1982, Cordera et al. 2001, Velando et al. 2002, Andersson et al. 2003, Duckworth 2009).

Resolution of this paradox lies in the realization that the selfregulatory and emergent processes that govern the dynamics of oogenesis in birds (Fig. 1) can produce nonrandom coordination of oocyte growth, ovulation order, and sex determination under routine perturbations of shared physiological mechanisms. In this case, the role of natural selection is limited to discrimination among the combinations of oocytes produced by developmental processes and, if context is persistent enough, to fine tuning regulation of a favored configuration so that it can be reliably produced in the future. Ubiquitous reuse of the same regulatory elements throughout oogenesis (Fig. 1) gives natural selection only a few targets, but these targets can accomplish an entire array of changes in sex determination and egg-laying order that can be produced essentially instantaneously and without major genetic changes in conserved core processes of oogenesis.

FIG. 1.

Main growth factors producing context-specific modulation of organism-wide signaling of follicle-stimulating hormone (FSH) and luteinizing hormone (LH). Growth factors are transforming growth factor-β (TFC), insulin-line growth factor (ICF), tumor necrosis factor-α (TNF-α), bone morphogenetic proteins (BMP), inhibin-α (Inh-α), inhibin-β (Inh-β), Inh-β/Inh-α ratio, and activin (Act). Time scale is calibrated for the House Finch (Carpodacus mexicanus). At the onset of breeding season, cohorts of small white follicles (SWF) are advanced to the small yellow follicle (SWF) stage; the order of recruitment is linked to initial acquisition of the ability to transport yolk across membranes. Differences in timing of consecutive SYF selections into preovulatory groups can result in groups that initiated growth at different times in relation to their hormonal synthesis and exposure to plasma hormones. Follicles in such necessarily heterochronic groups grow rapidly, partition (double-headed arrows) liver-produced yolk precursors with adjacent follicles in their groups, and eventually merge into a common ovulatory hierarchy guided by disproportional growth and associated spatial effects of inhibin-signaling by the largest follicle (F1). The LH surge that causes F1 to ovulate simultaneously stops growth of F2 and advances it to ovulation stage. Table shows developmental stage and growth factors (when known) that were shown to regulate adaptive changes in clutch size, ovulation order, sex determination, and egg-laying-substance gradients (based on Moreau et al. 1998, Hoffman et al. 2007, Badyaev et al. 2008a, Rutkowska and Badyaev 2008, Onagbesan et al. 2009).

f01_467.jpg

During the breeding season, the avian ovary contains three main classes of oocytes: small white follicles (SWF) that have been present in the ovary since embryonic stage, small yellow follicles (SYF) that are produced by periodic recruitment of cohorts of SWF with the onset of breeding season, and rapidly growing preovulatory oocytes that are selected from a few SYF, form a pronounced size hierarchy and ovulate sequentially during egg laying (Fig. 1). All types of follicles are present in the ovary at the same time, and all aspects of their recruitment, atresia, growth, and interactions are regulated at the organismal level by two key pituitary gonadotropins (FSH and LH; Fig. 1). However, although all oocytes, irrespective of stage, are exposed to the same hormonal surges, several highly conserved signaling elements (growth factors) convert these surges into context-specific effects on follicle development (Fig. 1; Johnson 1996, 2000; Johnson et al. 1996; Woods and Johnson 2005; Onagbesan et al. 2009). For example, in preovulatory follicles, the effect of FSH on activin produces oocyte membrane gaps that enable yolk uptake and facilitate follicular recruitment (Schuster et al. 2004), whereas in the older follicles activin modulates FSH receptor sensitivity and regulates growth (Chen and Johnson 1996, Johnson et al. 2008). Similarly, in prehierarchical follicles inhibin prevents follicular differentiation, whereas at the preovulatory stage it produces a regular ovulation rate (Lovell et al. 2001, 2003; Yang et al. 2001; Johnson et al. 2005). Depending on location within the ovary, TGF (Fig. 1) modulates the same LH signaling to (1) prevent SWF atresia, (2) increase growth and prevent over-recruitment of SYF (thus regulating clutch size), (3) keep preovulatory follicles alive, and (4) advance follicles in the ovulation sequence (Fig. 1; Johnson and Woods 2009, Onagbesan et al. 2009).

Such redundancy and conservation, along with pronounced emergent properties of oogenesis—such as the hierarchy produced by disproportionate growth, self-organization of hierarchical clusters, and activation—inhibition of within-ovary arrangements of preovulatory oocytes (Badyaev et al. 2006b, Lebedev et al. 2006, Tosca et al. 2006)—mean that only a few regulatory changes can accomplish strong and reversible coordination of ovulation order, sex determination, and allocation of substances in oocytes (Fig. 1), resulting in repeatable complex egg-laying orders in response to environmental or physiological conditions experienced by breeding females. Such effects can be maintained epigenetically for a prolonged time and be expressed in an entire population without acquiring genetic determination (Uller and Badyaev 2009)—and most probably they never do, because selection favoring such complex egg-laying patterns is unlikely to be recurrent over multiple generations. The role of natural selection is thus limited to eliminating “nonsurvivable” combinations of ovulation order, sex determination, and substance allocation (e.g., female sex bias in oocytes with greater testosterone allocation) and retaining favorable combinations under recurrent conditions (e.g., male bias in clutches produced by malnourished mothers). Importantly, selection does not “cause” these complex sex-biased egg-laying patterns, but only sorts and stabilizes emergent sex-biased oocyte configurations.

Our studies of House Finches (Carpodacus mexicanus) provide strong support for this scenario: we documented close integration of oogenesis, intra-ovary oocyte dynamics, and sex-determination under common endocrinological mechanisms that regulate female reproduction and homeostasis. House Finches show strong, complex, and reversible sex bias in egg-laying patterns in response to novel environments and stressors (Badyaev et al. 2003, 2006a), patterns that appear rapidly and spread widely in newly established populations, but that are not expressed (although they are inducible) in native populations (Badyaev and Oh 2008). Although these patterns were key in the establishment of House Finches across their recently expanded range, they do not appear to involve genetic change in regulatory elements, but instead capitalize on emergent integration of ovulation order, sex bias, and steroidogenesis under physiological mechanisms that regulate female homeostatic response to environmental change (Badyaev 2009). This scenario is likely more common than is currently realized and could plausibly account for frequently documented, but evolutionary elusive, context dependency in sex-biased ovulation order in birds.

AVIAN BEAKS: RECONCILING ADAPTATION AND DIVERSIFICATION

Avian beaks are some of the best-studied examples of both precise adaptation and extreme evolutionary diversification. Evolution of such adaptations requires both close genetic integration of beak components and high heritability of their development to produce the incremental fine tuning of beak morphology. However, such consistent elimination of developmental variability should, at the same time, prevent the evolutionary diversification of beaks that is commonly observed in birds (Grant 1986, Benkman 1993, Lovette et al. 2002).

Resolution of this paradox lies in the realization that many of the emergent and self-regulatory processes that constitute beak ontogeny (Fig. 2) are not themselves visible to natural selection. Instead, the role of selection is largely confined to eliminating and stabilizing postproduction configurations of conserved developmental modules and to fine tuning such configurations to the most recurrent context. Ubiquitous reuse of conserved regulatory elements throughout beak ontogeny (Fig. 2) gives selection an opportunity to rapidly accomplish adaptation, within-species polymorphism, and evolutionary diversification (Fig. 2) without depletion of developmental variability or excessive waiting time and population sizes needed for incremental evolution of complex beaks.

Beak morphogenesis starts with migration of neural crest cells into the embryo's facial region and mandibular arch (Fig. 2; Helms and Schneider 2003). Neural crest cells retain their stem cell characteristics during migration and do not have an affinity for a final placement until they arrive at their final destination and aggregate into five facial prominences that will ultimately merge into a beak (Fig. 2). The induction and transport of neural crest cells is regulated by a protein gradient (Fig. 2), and once cells form prominences they become enclosed by an ectoderm-derived layer of epithelium (Helms and Schneider 2003, Wu et al. 2006, Geetha-Loganathan et al. 2009). Activation-inhibition interactions and compensatory growth of rapidly dividing mesodermal cells in adjacent prominences and their ectodermal envelopes determine the juxtaposition of prominences and delineate the placement of a future beak (Fig. 2). Continuing cell proliferation induces reciprocal regulatory feedback between the mesenchyme cells and epithelium boundaries within each prominence, with the epithelium layer providing bounding effects and axial orientation directing cell outgrowth (Wu et al. 2006, Eames and Schneider 2008, Hu and Marcucio 2009). Rapidly growing frontonasal, two lateral nasal, and two maxillary prominences (Fig. 2) then merge and, bounded and directed by activation-inhibition feedback from their epithelium layer, form the upper beak. Merging of two mandible prominences produces the lower beak (Fig. 2). The prominences and their signaling networks are notoriously modular, autonomous, and conserved—they can be surgically divided and doubled-up within an embryo (e.g., producing shoveler-like beaks in chickens), or transferred between species (e.g., producing chimeric “qucks” [ducks with quail bills] and “duails” [quails with duck beaks]), or even exchanged between mouse and chicken (reviewed in Eames and Schneider 2005, Jheon and Schneider 2009).

In later embryonic stages, interactions between proliferating cartilage and bone areas of the upper beak activate local factors (Fig. 2) fine tune beak length and curvature, with many shapes emerging as a straightforward geometric consequence of cell proliferation and juxtraposition and the ratio of cartilage to bone cell mass (Wu et al. 2006, Abzhanov et al. 2007, Campàs et al. 2010, Mallarino et al. 2011). In later nestling and juvenile stages, mechanical stress associated with muscle attachment and force leads to local production of Bmp and further fine tuning of functional beaks (reviewed in Young and Badyaev 2007). The entire process is regulated by conserved regulatory factors that have distinct effects depending on the time and place of their expression. For example, Bmp 4 regulates distinct processes at different times (Fig. 2)—early commitment of neural crest cells to bone formation (Abzhanov et al. 2007), later cell proliferation within prominences (Wu et al. 2004), species-specific outgrowth of cartilage—bone areas in the upper beak (Abzhanov et al. 2004), mechanical stress-induced bone formation in both the upper and the lower beak, and adaptive plasticity in beak size between habitats (Badyaev et al. 2008b).

FIG. 2.

Main growth factors, organizational principles, and modular structure of beak morphogenesis. Groups of growth factors are wingless type (Wnt), fibroblast growth factor 8 (Fgf8), sonic hedgehog (Shh), bone morphogenetic proteins (BMP), transforming growth factor beta (TGFβ), calmodulin (CaM), Dickkopf (Dkk), and β-catenin. Time scale is based on the House Finch (hatching on days 12–14), but is broadly conserved across birds. Facial prominences (shown in red), formed by aggregation and proliferation of neural crest cells, are frontonasal (fn), lateral nasal (In), mandibular (md), and maxillary (mx). Cartilage and bone areas arising during late development in beak are prenasal cartilage (pnc), premaxillary bone (pmx), nasal bone (n), and dentary bone (dnt). Double-headed arrows show interactions between neighboring prominences during growth and expansion (left and middle figure), tissue partitioning between pnc and pmx, and effects of mechanical stress conductance in late developmental stages (right figure). Left figure: migrating neural crest cells (blue arrows) aggregate in prominences, and interaction between Shh and Fgf8 domains determines the beak placement (dashed triangle). Middle figure (frontal view of an embryo): ectoderm (green) and mesoderm (yellow) layers in fn show major regulatory effect of the frontonasal ectodermal zone (FEZ) on Bmp regulation of cell proliferation. Right figure: Bmp and CaM are expressed in different parts of pnc affecting mostly beak depth and length correspondingly. Tissue tradeoffs between pnc and pmx zones regulate Dkk effect on beak length. Table shows developmental stage and growth factors that were shown to regulate species divergence, polymorphism, and adaptive plasticity in beak size and shape (based on Abzhanov et al. 2004, 2006; Wu et al. 2004, 2006; Helms and Brugmann 2007; Badyaev et al. 2008b; Clabaut et al. 2009).

f02_467.jpg

The evolutionary significance of such modular organization and conserved signaling is that only a few genetic changes in regulatory elements can facilitate rapid evolution of local adaptation and extensive evolutionary diversification without depletion of genetic variance in beak morphology. Under this scenario, the role of natural selection is limited to eliminating developmental abnormalities and to stabilizing developmental configurations most adaptive under prevalent conditions. Genetic fixation of mutations in regulatory elements can enable evolutionary persistence of most favored configurations, but redundancy of the regulatory network, compensatory interactions among its elements, and overall highly modular organization assures short-term evolutionary retention of many functional configurations of beaks.

This scenario is corroborated by developmental and genetic changes in beak morphology that accompanied evolution of locally adapted beaks over 19 consecutive generations following colonization of a novel environment by the House Finch (Badyaev 2010). We found rapid reorganization and exceptional diversity in beak morphologies followed by reversible microevolutionary changes in the first few generations. Adaptive equivalence of distinct beak configurations enabled survival of House Finches in novel environments and wide exploration of beak morphospace before the most appropriate beak configuration was found and stabilized. The role of directional selection during this period was largely confined to elimination of developmental extremes and abnormalities produced by compensatory developmental interactions among beak components, while the beak's genetic architecture mirrored long-term stabilizing selection, assuring retention of adaptive evolutionary change (Badyaev 2010). Importantly, compensatory adjustments among beak components shielded genetic variance in individual traits, enabling production of locally adaptive morphology without compromising future modifications. That such rapid phenotypic evolution of local adaptation can be accomplished in only a few generations and in a small founder population of a few thousand birds (making natural selection too weak a force to directly modify developmental organization of beaks) is a testament to the evolutionary potential of such developmental organization (Fig. 2; Badyaev 2010).

TABLE 1.

General predictions of phenotypic changes during adaptive evolution under two scenarios.

t01_467.gif

Taken together, the examples with oocytes and beaks illustrate the general principle that redeployment and combinatorial changes in conserved developmental modules (that are not themselves a subject to natural selection) in a new place, time, and context in ontogeny enables evolution of adaptation by natural selection. This perspective provides a nonconventional, but perhaps more biologically realistic, set of evolutionary predictions (Table 1) that might be particularly relevant to the study of systems such as birds, in which an overwhelming majority of adaptations is produced by rearrangements of core processes that evolved billions of years ago in a completely different context. Most importantly, these examples are a powerful reminder that explicit conceptual separation of processes of origination and adaptation—that is, taking the concept of natural selection out of the process of development—along with detailed understanding of the developmental dynamics of adaptive traits that we study in the field invariably provides crucial insights into many outstanding problems in empirical evolutionary biology.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Empirical work reported here was supported by the National Science Foundation (DEB-0075388, DEB-0077804, and IBN-0218313), a Fulbright Fellowship, and the David and Lucille Packard Foundation Fellowship. Three anonymous reviewers provided helpful comments and suggestions.

LITERATURE CITED

1.

A. Abzhanov , W. P. Kuo , C. Hartmann , B. R. Grant , P. R. Grant , and C. J. Tabin . 2006. The calmodulin pathway and evolution of elongated beak morphology in Darwin's finches. Nature 442:563–567. Google Scholar

2.

A. Abzhanov , M. Protas , B. R. Grant , P. R. Grant , and C. J. Tabin . 2004. Bmp4 and morphological variation of beaks in Darwin's finches. Science 305:1462–1464. Google Scholar

3.

A. Abzhanov , S. J. Rodda , A. P. McMahon , and C. J. Tabin . 2007. Regulation of skeletogenic differentiation in cranial dermal bone. Development 134:3133–3144. Google Scholar

4.

P. Alberch 1980. Ontogenesis and morphological diversification. American Zoologist 20:653–667 Google Scholar

5.

M. Andersson , J. Wallander , L. Oring , E. Akst , J. M. Reed , and R. C. Fleischer . 2003. Adaptive seasonal trend in brood sex ratio: Test in two sister species with contrasting breeding systems. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 16:510–515. Google Scholar

6.

C. D. Ankney 1982. Sex ratio varies with egg sequence in Lesser Snow Geese. Auk 99:662–666. Google Scholar

7.

A. V. Badyaev 2009. Evolutionary significance of phenotypic accommodation in novel environments: An empirical test of the Baldwin effect. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B 364:1125–1141. Google Scholar

8.

A. V. Badyaev 2010. The beak of the other finch: Coevolution of genetic covariance structure and developmental modularity during adaptive evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B 365:1111–1126. Google Scholar

9.

A. V. Badyaev 2011. Origin of the fittest: Link between emergent variation and evolutionary change as a critical question in evolutionary biology. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 278:1921–1929. Google Scholar

10.

A. V. Badyaev , M. L. Beck , G. E. Hill , and L. A. Whittingham . 2003. The evolution of sexual size dimorphism in the House Finch: V. Maternal effects. Evolution 57:384–396. Google Scholar

11.

A. V. Badyaev , T. L. Hamstra , K. P. Oh , and D. Acevedo Seaman . 2006a. Sex-biased maternal effects reduce ectoparasite-induced mortality in a passerine bird. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 103:14406–14411. Google Scholar

12.

A. V. Badyaev , and K. P. Oh . 2008. Environmental induction and phenotypic retention of adaptive maternal effects. BMC Evolutionary Biology 8:e3. Google Scholar

13.

A. V. Badyaev , K. P. Oh , and R. Mui . 2006b. Evolution of sex-biased maternal effects in birds: II. Contrasting sex-specific oocyte competition in native and recently established populations. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 19:909–921. Google Scholar

14.

A. V. Badyaev , R. L. Young , G. E. Hill , and R. A. Duckworth . 2008a. Evolution of sex-biased maternal effects in birds: IV. Intraovarian growth dynamics can link sex-determination and sex-specific acquisition of resources. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 21:449–460. Google Scholar

15.

A. V. Badyaev , R. L. Young , K. P. Oh , and C. Addison . 2008b. Evolution on a local scale: Developmental, functional, and genetic bases of divergence in bill form and associated changes in song structure between adjacent habitats. Evolution 62:1951–1964. Google Scholar

16.

J. M. Baldwin 1902. Development and Evolution. Macmillan, New York. Google Scholar

17.

C. W. Benkman 1993. Adaptation to single resources and the evolution of crossbill (Loxia) diversity. Ecological Monographs 63:305– 325. Google Scholar

18.

W. Callebaut , G. B. Müller , and S. A. Newman . 2007. The organismic systems approach. Streamlining the naturalistic agenda. Pages 25–92 in Integrating Evolution and Development: From Theory to Practice ( R. Sansom and R. N. Brandon , Eds.). MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts. Google Scholar

19.

O. Campàs , R. Mallarino , A. Herrel , A. Abzhanov , and M. P. Brenner . 2010. Scaling and shear transformations capture beak shape variation in Darwin's finches. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 107:3356–3360. Google Scholar

20.

S. B. Carroll 2005. Evolution at two levels: On genes and form. PLoS Biology 3:e245. Google Scholar

21.

C. C. Chen , and P. A. Johnson . 1996. Expression of inhibin alpha and inhibin/activin beta A subunits in the granulosa layer of the large preovulatory follicles of the hen. Biology of Reproduction 55:450–454. Google Scholar

22.

C. Clabaut , A. Herrel , T. J. Sanger , T. B. Smith , and A. Abzhanov . 2009. Development of beak polymorphism in the African Seedcracker, Pyrenestes ostrinus. Evolution and Development 11:636–646. Google Scholar

23.

P. J. Cordero , J. Vinuela , J. M. Aparicio , and J. P. Veiga . 2001. Seasonal variation in sex ratio and sexual egg dimorphism favouring daughters in first clutches of the Spotless Starling. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 14:829–834. Google Scholar

24.

J. E Crow , and M. Kimura . 1970. An Introduction to Population Genetics Theory. Harper and Row, New York. Google Scholar

25.

C. Darwin 1859. On the Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, or the Preservation of Favoured Races in the Struggle for Life. John Murray, London. Google Scholar

26.

E. H. Davidson 2006. The Regulatory Genome: Gene Regulatory Networks in Development and Evolution. Academic Press, San Diego, California. Google Scholar

27.

R. A. Duckworth 2009. Maternal effects and range expansion: A key factor in a dynamic process? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B 364:1075–1086. Google Scholar

28.

B. F. Eames , and R. A. Schneider . 2005. Quail-duck chimeras reveal spatiotemporal plasticity in molecular and histogenic programs of cranial feather development. Development 132:1499–1509. Google Scholar

29.

B. F. Eames , and R. A. Schneider . 2008. The genesis of cartilage size and shape during development and evolution. Development 135:3947–3958. Google Scholar

30.

G. Forgacs , and S. A. Newman . 2005. Biological Physics of the Developing Embryo. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, United Kingdom. Google Scholar

31.

S. Gavrilets 2004. Fitness Landscapes and the Origin of Species. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey. Google Scholar

32.

P. Geetha-Loganathan , S. Nimmagadda , L. Antoni , K. Fu , C. J. Whiting , P. Francis-West , and J. M. Ruichman . 2009. Expression of WNT signaling pathway genes during chicken craniofacial delevelopment. Developmental Dynamics 238:1150–1165. Google Scholar

33.

J. Gerhart , and M. Kirschner . 2007. The theory of facilitated variation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 104:8582–8589. Google Scholar

34.

S. J. Gould 1977. Ontogeny and Phylogeny. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts. Google Scholar

35.

S. J. Gould 2002. The Structure of Evolutionary Theory. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts. Google Scholar

36.

S. J. Gould , and R. C. Lewontin . 1979. The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: A critique of the adaptationist programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 205:581–598. Google Scholar

37.

P.R. Grant 1986. Ecology and Evolution of Darwin's Finches. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey. Google Scholar

38.

S. L. Haigo , and D. Bilder . 2011. Global tissue revolutions in a morphogenetic movement controlling elongation. Science 331:1071– 1074. Google Scholar

39.

J. A. Helms , and S. A. Brugmann . 2007. The origins of species-specific facial morphology: The proof is in the pigeon. Integrative and Comparative Biology 47:338–342. Google Scholar

40.

J. A. Helms , and R. A. Schneider . 2003. Cranial skeletal biology. Nature 423:326–331. Google Scholar

41.

J. B. Hoffman , A. P. Benson , V. L. Christensen , B. D. Fairchild , and A. J. Davis . 2007. Follicular development and expression of the messenger ribonucleic acid for the inhibin/activin subunits in two genetic lines of turkey hens that differ in total egg production. Poultry Science Association 86:944–952. Google Scholar

42.

D. Hu , and R. S. Marcucio . 2009. Unique organization of the frontonasal ectodermal zone in birds and mammals. Developmental Biology 325:200–210. Google Scholar

43.

A. H. Jheon , and R. A. Schneider . 2009. The cells that fill the bill: Neural crest and the evolution of craniofacial development. Journal of Dental Research 88:12–21. Google Scholar

44.

A. L. Johnson 1996. The avian ovarian hierarchy: A balance between follicle differentiation and atresia. Poultry and Avian Biology Reviews 7:99–110. Google Scholar

45.

A. L. Johnson 2000. Granulosa cell apoptosis: Conservation of cell signaling in an avian ovarian model system. Biological Signals and Receptors 9:96–101. Google Scholar

46.

A. L. Johnson , J. T. Bridgham , J. P. Witty , and J. L. Tilly . 1996. Susceptibility of avian ovarian granulosa cells to apoptosis is dependent upon stage of follicle development and is related to endogenous levels of bcl-xlong gene expression. Endocrinology 137:2059–2066. Google Scholar

47.

A. L. Johnson , M. J. Haugen , and D. C. Woods . 2008. Role for inhibitor of differentiation/deoxyribonucleic acid-binding (Id) proteins in granulosa cell differentiation. Endocrinology 149:3187–3195. Google Scholar

48.

A. L. Johnson , and D. C. Woods . 2009. Dynamics of avian ovarian follicle development: Cellular mechanisms of granulosa cell differentiation. General and Comparative Endocrinology 163:12–17. Google Scholar

49.

P. A. Johnson , C. F. Brooks , and A. J. Davis . 2005. Patterns of secretion of immunoreactive inhibin/activin subunits by avian granulosa cells. General and Comparative Endocrinology 141:233–239. Google Scholar

50.

T. D. Johnston , and G. Gottlieb . 1990. Neophenogenesis: A developmental theory of phenotypic evolution. Journal of Theoretical Biology 147:471–495. Google Scholar

51.

A. Kicheva , P. Pantazis , T. Bollenbach , Y. Kalaidzidis , T. Bittig , F. Jülicher , and M. González-Gaitán . 2007. Kinetics of morphogen gradient formation. Science 315:521–525. Google Scholar

52.

M. C. King , and A. C. Wilson . 1975. Evolution at two levels in humans and chimpanzees. Science 188:107–116. Google Scholar

53.

N. King , M. J. Westbrook , S. L. Young , A. Kuo , M. Abedin , J. Chapman , S. Fairclough , U. Hellsten , Y. Isogai , I. Letunic , and Others. 2008. The genome of the choanoflagellate Monosiga brevicollis and the origin of metazoans. Nature 451:783–788. Google Scholar

54.

M. Kirschner , and J. C. Gerhart . 2005. The Plausibility of Life: Resolving Darwin's Dilemma. Yale University Press, New Haven, Connecticut. Google Scholar

55.

E. V. Koonin , and Y. I. Wolf . 2009. The fundamental units, processes and patterns in evolution, and the Tree of Life conundrum. Biology Direct 4:33. Google Scholar

56.

V. A. Lebedev , I. Y. Lebedeva , R. Grossmann , T. I. Kuzmina , and N. Parvizi . 2006. Ovulatory cycle-related alterations in the thecal growth and membrane protein content of thecal tissue of hen preovulatory follicles. Theriogenology 66:217–223. Google Scholar

57.

T. M. Lovell , R. T. Gladwell , N. P. Groome , and P. G. Knight . 2003. Ovarian follicle development in the laying hen is accompanied by divergent changes in inhibin A, inhibin B, activin A and follistatin production in granulosa and theca layers. Journal of Endocrinology 177:45–55. Google Scholar

58.

T. M. Lovell , P. G. Knight , N. P. Groome , and R. T. Gladwell . 2001. Changes in plasma inhibin A levels during sexual maturation in the female chicken and the effects of active immunization against inhibin alpha-subunit on reproductive hormone profiles and ovarian function. Biology of Reproduction 64:188–196. Google Scholar

59.

I. J. Lovette , E. Bermingham , and R. E. Ricklefs . 2002. Clade-specific morphological diversification and adaptive radiation in Hawaiian songbirds. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B 269:37–42. Google Scholar

60.

M. Lynch 2007. The Origins of Genome Architecture. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, Massachusetts. Google Scholar

61.

M. Lynch , and A. Abegg . 2010. The rate of establishment of complex adaptations. Molecular Biology and Evolution 27:1404–1414. Google Scholar

62.

R. Mallarino , P. R. Grant , B. R. Grant , A. Herrel , W. P. Kuo , and A. Abzhanov . 2011. Two developmental modules establish 3D beak-shape variation in Darwin's finches. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 108:4057–4062. Google Scholar

63.

D. J. Montell 2008. Morphogenetic cell movements: Diversity from modular mechanical properties. Science 322:1502–1505. Google Scholar

64.

J. D. Moreau , D. G. Satterlee , J. J. Rejman , G. G. Cadd , K. G. Kousoulas , and W. C. Fioretti . 1998. Active immunization of Japanese Quail hens with a recombinant chicken inhibin fusion protein enhances production performance. Poultry Science 77:894–901. Google Scholar

65.

G. B. Müller 2007 Evo-devo: Extending the evolutionary synthesis. Nature Reviews Genetics 8:943–949. Google Scholar

66.

G. B. Müller , and S. Newman , Eds. 2003. Origination of Organismal Form: Beyond the Gene in Developmental and Evolutionary Biology. MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts. Google Scholar

67.

S. A. Newman 2006. The developmental-genetic toolkit and the molecular homology-analogy paradox. Biological Theory 1:12–16. Google Scholar

68.

S. A. Newman 2010. Dynamic patterning modules. Pages 281–306 in Evolution: The Extended Synthesis ( M. Pigliucci and G. B. Müller , Eds.). MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts. Google Scholar

69.

O. Onagbesan , V. Bruggeman , and E. Decuypere . 2009. Intraovarian growth factors regulating ovarian function in avian species: A review. Animal Reproduction Science 111:121–140. Google Scholar

70.

T. Piersma , and J. A. Van Gils . 2010. The Flexible Phenotype: A Body-Centered Integration of Ecology, Physiology, and Behaviour. Oxford University Press, Oxford, United Kingdom. Google Scholar

71.

R. G. B. Reid 2007. Biological Emergences: Evolution by Natural Experiment. MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts. Google Scholar

72.

J. Rutkowska , and A. V. Badyaev . 2008. Meiotic drive and sex determination: Molecular mechanisms of sex ratio adjustment in birds. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B 363:1675–1686. Google Scholar

73.

I. I. Schmalhausen 1938. Organism as a Whole in Individual Development and History. Academy of Sciences, Leningrad, USSR. Google Scholar

74.

M. K. Schuster , B. Schmierer , A. Shkumatava , and K. Kuchler . 2004. Activin A and follicle-stimulating hormone control tight junctions in avian granulosa by regulating occludin expression. Biology of Reproduction 70:1493–1499. Google Scholar

75.

N. Shubin , C. J. Tabin , and S. Carroll . 2009. Deep homology and the origins of evolutionary novelty. Nature 457:818–823. Google Scholar

76.

D. L. Stern 2000. Evolutionary developmental biology and the problem of variation. Evolution 54:1079–1091. Google Scholar

77.

L. Tosca , S. Crochet , P. Ferré , F. Foufelle , S. Tesseraud , and J. Dupont . 2006. AMP-activated protein kinase activation modulates progesterone secretion in granulosa cells from hen preovulatory follicles. Journal of Endocrinology 190:85–97. Google Scholar

78.

T. Uller , and A. V. Badyaev . 2009. Evolution of “determinants” in sex-determination: A novel hypothesis for the origin of environmental contingencies in avian sex-bias. Seminars in Cell & Developmental Biology 20:304–312. Google Scholar

79.

A. Velando , J. Graves , and J. E. Ortega-Ruano . 2002. Sex ratio in relation to timing of breeding, and laying sequence in a dimorphic seabird. Ibis 144:9–16. Google Scholar

80.

M. J. West-Eberhard 2003. Developmental Plasticity and Evolution. Oxford University Press, New York. Google Scholar

81.

L. L. Whyte 1965. Internal Factors in Evolution. George Braziller, New York. Google Scholar

82.

A. S. Wilkins 2001. The Evolution of Developmental Pathways. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, Massachusetts. Google Scholar

83.

D. C. Woods , and A. L. Johnson . 2005. Regulation of follicle-stimulating hormone-receptor messenger RNA in hen granulosa cells relative to follicle selection. Biology of Reproduction 72:643–650. Google Scholar

84.

S. Wright 1931. Evolution in Mendelian populations. Genetics 16:97–159. Google Scholar

85.

P. Wu , T.-X. Jiang , J.-Y Shen , R. B. Widelitz , and C.-M. Chuong . 2006. Morphoregulation of avian beaks: Comparative mapping of growth zone activities and morphological evolution. Developmental Dynamics 235:1400–1412. Google Scholar

86.

P. Wu , T.-X. Jiang , S. Suksaweang , R. B. Widelitz , and C. Chuong . 2004. Molecular shaping of the beak. Science 305:1465–1466. Google Scholar

87.

P.-X. Yang , K. Y. Arail , W.-Z. Jin , G. Watanabe , N. P. Groome , and K. Taya . 2001. Preovulatory follicles in the ovary as the source of circulating inhibin in the duck. General and Comparative Endocrinology 121:156–162. Google Scholar

88.

R. L. Young , and A. V. Badyaev . 2007 Evolution of ontogeny: Linking epigenetic remodeling and genetic adaptation in skeletal structures. Integrative and Comparative Biology 47:234–244. Google Scholar
© 2011 by The American Ornithologists' Union. All rights reserved. Please direct all requests for permission to photocopy or reproduce article content through the University of California Press's Rights and Permissions website, http://www.ucpressjournals.com/reprintInfo.asp.
Alexander V. Badyaev "How Do Precise Adaptive Features Arise in Development? Examples with Evolution of Context-Specific Sex Ratios and Perfect Beaks," The Auk 128(3), 467-474, (1 July 2011). https://doi.org/10.1525/auk.2011.11103
Received: 12 May 2011; Accepted: 2 June 2011; Published: 1 July 2011
Back to Top