Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 June 2010 Does the Mimetic Posture of Macaria aemulataria (Walker) (Geometridae) Larvae Enhance Survival Against Bird Predation?
Ignacio Castellanos
Author Affiliations +

Mimicry, in which one organism resembles another, is a widespread and important phenomenon in behavioral ecology (Wickler 1968; Ruxton et al. 2004). Mimesis, or the mimicry of inanimate objects such as twigs, leaves, bird droppings, the odor of plants, and so on, is also well known (Cott 1940; Edmunds 1974; Akino et al. 2004). Mimicry may include changes in morphological (e.g., hairs and spines) and behavioral traits, chemistry, and ecology, all aimed at enhancing escape and/or survival from predation (Edmunds 1974; Endler 1986). However, tests of the effectiveness of different forms of mimicry or mimesis, as anti-predator (sensu latu) defenses, are less common (but see Portugal & Trigo 2005). In this study we experimentally evaluated the survival advantage of the mimetic posture of larvae of Macaria aemulataria (Walker) (Geometridae) against a visually hunting predator, the tufted titmouse, Baeolophus bicolor (Paridae).

Macaria aemulataria has four instars and all are mimetic of leaf parts. Small instars tend to mimic the lateral ribs of leaves whereas larger instars tend to mimic the mid rib and petiole of leaves. Caterpillars occasionally feed during the day, but immediately stop feeding and return to their mimetic posture when birds land or move in their vicinity (I. Castellanos personal observation). They remain motionless in response to substrate-borne vibrations produced by foraging birds, but hang on a silk thread in response to substrate-borne vibrations generated by foraging invertebrate predators (Castellanos & Barbosa 2006). Macaria aemulataria is a specialist on species of Acer in riparian forests in Maryland, and has about three generations per year (Castellanos unpublished data). Macaria aemulataria larvae used in this study originated from adults collected at Patuxent Wildlife Refuge Research Center, Maryland (39° 03.639′N. 76° 44.244′W). Females from the field were allowed to oviposit in the laboratory and their larvae were reared individually in 237-ml plastic containers with Acer negundo L. (box elder) leaves before they were used in a trial.

The tufted titmouse is an omnivorous bird whose diet includes mainly arthropods, primarily caterpillars (Bent 1946). Its foraging behavior is characteristic of other passerine birds that also forage for arthropods among the foliage of forest trees (Robinson & Holmes 1982; Grubb & Pravosudov 1994). They typically attack prey items by employing a gleaning maneuver consisting of flights between branches and jumps along branches, taking stationary prey items after landing on a branch (Grubb & Pravosudov 1994). They tend to wait before either moving to another perch or before pursuing and capturing a detected prey (Robinson & Holmes 1982; I. Castellanos personal observations). Bird individuals used in this study were captured from a wooded area in Glenn Dale, Maryland (IACUC Permit No. R-02-38), and released in the same area after the trials ended.

Determination of mimesis as an anti-predator defense against birds. A branch with leaves from a box elder tree was placed inside a 1.25 × 1.25 × 0.65 m cage made of wooden frames and fish-netting, at an angle of approximately 75 degrees from the floor of the cage. We used these branches because A. negundo has symmetrical compound leaves and thus provides the ability to symmetrically present potential predators with equidistant prey options. To determine if the posture taken by fourth instars (and assumed to represent leafvein mimesis) enhances M. aemulataria survival against the tufted titmice, an individual fourth instar M. aemulataria was placed on each of nine branches (74.09 ± 3.14 cm (mean ± SEM) long) and allowed to settle for half an hour. This was sufficient time for a larva to assume a mimetic posture on a leaf, i.e., parallel and adjacent to the mid-vein. After the caterpillar chose a leaflet for posturing, another individual fourth instar was placed on the opposite leaflet at an equal distance from the branch. This caterpillar was placed perpendicularly to the mid-vein of the leaflet. In one of the nine trials, the released caterpillar selected a lateral leaf vein. In that trial the control larva was positioned perpendicular to the appropriate lateral vein on the opposing leaflet. In order to fix the position of the experimental caterpillar to a leaflet, two drops of glue were used, one to attach its thoracic legs and the other to attach its prolegs. To control for any effect of the glue, two drops of glue were placed on the leaflet that the normally posturing larva had selected. The distance between the mimetic and the control caterpillars was 16.18 ± 1.97 cm (mean ± SEM).

A single bird was introduced to the cage, where it could choose between the two caterpillars. Observations were conducted behind a black cloth through a 15 × 2 cm opening. Nine trials were conducted, each with a different bird, and for each trial the bird was exposed to different caterpillars on different branches. All trials were conducted in a naturally sunlit room at an ambient temperature of 25 ± 2°C. The choice of a caterpillar by the birds was analyzed with a binomial probability test (Sokal & Rohlf 1995).

In all nine trials, the birds took the non-mimetic (perpendicular to mid-vein) caterpillar. The probability that this result was due to chance is 0.002. Thus, we conclude that the birds did not readily perceive the mimetically positioned larvae. Given that in the field there are many other larvae that are not vein mimics (Barbosa & Caldas 2007), regardless of the proximate mechanisms, the mimetic coloration and perching behavior of larvae of M. aemulataria appears to enhance survival against bird predation.

Acknowledgements

I. Castellanos thanks the Department of Entomology and the Program in Behavior, Ecology, Evolution and Systematics, University of Maryland, and the Programa de Mejoramiento del Profesorado (PROMEP), Secretaría de Educación Pública, México, for their support.

Literature Cited

1.

T. Akino , K. Nakamura , & S. Wakamura . 2004. Diet-induced chemical phytomimesis by twig-like caterpillars of Biston robustum Butler (Lepidoptera: Geometridae). Chemoecology 14:165–174. Google Scholar

2.

P. Barbosa & A. Caldas . 2007. Do larvae of species in macrolepidopteran assemblages share traits that influence susceptibility to parasitism? Environ. Entomol. 32: 329–336. Google Scholar

3.

A. C. Bent 1946. Life histories of North American jays, crows and titmice. United States National Museum Bulletin No. 191. 495 pp. Google Scholar

4.

I. Castellanos & P. Barbosa . 2006. Evaluation of predation risk by a caterpillar using substrate-borne vibrations. Anim. Behav. 72: 461–469. Google Scholar

5.

H. B. Cott 1940. Adaptive coloration in animals. Methuen and Company, London. 508 pp. Google Scholar

6.

M. Edmunds 1974. Defence in animals: a survey of anti-predator defences. Longman Group, Ltd, New York. 357 pp. Google Scholar

7.

J.A. Endler 1986. Defense against predators, pp. 109–134. In M. E. Feder & G.V. Lauder (eds.), Predator-Prey relationships: perspectives and approaches from the study of Lower Vertebrates. Univ. Chicago Press, Chicago. Google Scholar

8.

T. C. Grubb & V. V. Pravosudov . 1994. Tufted titmouse, pp. 1–15. In A. Poole & F. Gill (eds.). The birds of North America, no. 86. The Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania and The American Ornithologists' Union, Washington D. C. Google Scholar

9.

A. H. A. Portugal & J. R. Trigo . 2005. Similarity of cuticular lipids between a caterpillar and its host plant: a way to make a prey undetectable for predatory ants? J. Chem. Ecol. 31: 2551–2561. Google Scholar

10.

S. K. Robinson & R. T. Holmes . 1982. Foraging behavior of forest birds: the relationships among search tactics, diet and habitat structure. Ecology 63:1918–1931. Google Scholar

11.

G. D. Ruxton , T. N. Sherratt & M. P. Speed . 2004. Avoiding attack: the evolutionary ecology of crypsis, warning signals and mimicry. Oxford University Press, Oxford. 249 pp. Google Scholar

12.

R. R. Sokal & F. J. Rohlf . 1995. Biometry. W. H. Freeman and Company, New York. 887 pp. Google Scholar

13.

W. Wickler 1968. Mimicry in plants and animals. McGraw-Hill, New York. 253 pp. Google Scholar
Ignacio Castellanos "Does the Mimetic Posture of Macaria aemulataria (Walker) (Geometridae) Larvae Enhance Survival Against Bird Predation?," The Journal of the Lepidopterists' Society 64(2), 113-115, (1 June 2010). https://doi.org/10.18473/lepi.v64i2.a9
Received: 31 March 2009; Accepted: 4 November 2009; Published: 1 June 2010
KEYWORDS
behavior
caterpillar
defense
predator-prey interaction
Back to Top