Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 January 2020 Ecological Covariates at Kill Sites Influence Tiger (Panthera tigris) Hunting Success in Huai Kha Khaeng Wildlife Sanctuary, Thailand
Somporn Pakpien, Achara Simcharoen, Somphot Duangchantrasiri, Vijak Chimchome, Nantachai Pongpattannurak, James L. D. Smith
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Despite significant knowledge of tiger ecology, information on hunting behavior is limited because tigers hunt in habitats where they are difficult to observe. From May 2013 to June 2015, we visited kill sites of eight female radio-collared tigers (Panthera tigris) to identify prey species of this species in Huai Kha Khaeng Wildlife Sanctuary, Thailand. At 150 kill sites, 11 mammalian species were identified from skeletal remains or hair samples. Sambar (Rusa unicolor), banteng (Bos javanicus), and gaur (Bos gaurus) composed 95.1% of tiger prey biomass. A subset of 87 kill sites was paired with 87 randomly selected sites within the home ranges of five of the eight radio-collared tigers to determine the influence of prey abundance and other ecological variables on hunting success. At each site, geomorphic and ecological covariates were sampled in 900 m2 square plots. A generalized linear model was used to investigate differences between kill sites and random sites. Mean relative prey abundance at kill sites was significantly lower than relative prey abundance at random sites (77.8 and 139.3 tracks/ha, respectively) indicating tigers did not kill in areas of higher relative prey abundance. Model selection was used to examine 12 landscape features that potentially influence kill site location. In the best model, low shrub cover and high crown cover were highly significant; tree density was included in this model but was not significant. This is the first study to demonstrate that kill location requires a combination of landscape features to first detect and then successfully stalk prey.

Animals should seek habitat with adequate food, cover, nest/den sites, or other resources critical for survival (Manly, McDonald, Thomas, McDonald, & Erickson, 2002). For female felids, sufficient food to raise young is often their primary resource need, and natural selection is expected to drive foraging decisions to optimize food intake and minimize energy expenditure (Krebs & Davies, 1993). Food demands of female tigers increase rapidly as cubs mature and mothers continue to be the primary provider until their young are approximately 1.5 years old; at this time, male offspring are often larger than their mothers (Smith, McDougal, & Miquelle, 1989). Johnson (1980) proposed a hierarchical model as a framework by which animals efficiently meet their resource needs. Simcharoen et al. (2014) studied second order habitat selection which focuses on where female tigers settle and the relationship of prey abundance to their territory size. These authors found an inverse relationship between female territory size and the abundance of large prey. Within a female’s territory, third and fourth order resource selection includes selection of denning, resting, and hunting sites. Here, we focus on kill site selection.

Many studies have shown that high prey abundance is the primary factor that predicts hunting success (Litvaitis, Sherburne, & Bissonette, 1986; Murray, Boutin, O’Donoghue, & Nams, 1995; Palomares, Delibes, Revilla, Calzada, & Fedriani, 2001; Spong, 2002). Alternatively, Hebblewhite, Merrill, and McDonald (2005) propose a landscape hypothesis that predators prefer habitat where it is easier to kill their primary prey. Following Hollings (1959), Hebblewhite et al. simplify predation into the instantaneous probability of encounter followed by the conditional probability of successfully killing prey. This landscape hypothesis suggests that landscape features such as slope, ruggedness, and various aspects of horizontal cover first favor prey detection and once prey is detected, catchability will be favored (Hebblewhite et al., 2005). Our study examines resource selection by hunting female tigers to evaluate the importance of prey abundance and landscape attributes that affect hunting success (Stephens & Krebs, 1986).

Once a prey animal is detected, felids typically approach it using available vegetative cover (Elliott, Cowan, & Hollings, 1977). Concealment allows them to hunt by ambushing prey by stalking and then chasing prey for a short distance (Kruuk, 1986; Caro & Fitzgibbon, 1992; Sunquist & Sunquist, 1989). Tigers (Panthera tigris) employ this strategy, stalking or ambushing prey from cover (Schaller, 1967; Sunquist & Sunquist, 1989). Cover, however, conceals predators so that hunting success is improved and also reduces prey detection by predators (Balme, Hunter, & Slotow, 2007). Thus, the landscape hypothesis must balance first detection of prey and then stalking success. Both of these factors are components of Holling’s (1959) theoretical framework; predators should select habitats to maximize both aspects of hunting success (Hayward & Kerley, 2005). The objectives of our study were as follows: (a) identify the main prey species consumed by tigers and (b) determine relative importance of prey abundance and the ecological variables that influence hunting success of tigers in Huai Kha Khaeng Wildlife Sanctuary, Thailand.

Method

Study Area

The study was conducted between May 2013 to June 2015 in Huai Kha Khaeng Wildlife Sanctuary, Thailand (∼15° 31′ N, ∼99° 16′ E) which is located in the eastern portion of the Western Forest Complex (WEFCOM). The study was concentrated in the northern part of the Sanctuary (Figure 1) which is characterized by mixed deciduous, dry dipterocarp, and dry evergreen forest. The average annual rainfall (1375 mm) is divided into a wet season (May–October), with a mean of 1088 mm of rain and a dry season (November to April) with a mean of 298 mm. The temperature reaches 40° C in April at the end of the dry season. The tiger is the largest carnivore in this ecosystem and its density ranges from 1.25 to 2.01 tigers/km2 (Duangchantrasiri et al., 2016). Phetdee (2000) identified 16 prey species consumed by tigers but the primary prey were animals ≥ 100 kg, represented by large ungulates including sambar (Rusa unicolor), banteng (Bos javanicus), gaur (B. gaurus), and water buffalo (Bubalus bubalis), which characterize 89.8% of tiger diet in this region.

Figure 1.

Map of Huai Kha Khaeng Wildlife Sanctuary showing the locations of 150 sites where tiger-killed prey were found and identified and 87 kill and 87 random sites where ecological correlate data were measured to investigate tiger kill site characteristics.

10.1177_1940082917719000-fig1.tif

Data Collection and Analysis

Kill site data

Potential kill sites were identified using cluster analysis of hourly location data from eight female satellite radio-collared tigers (Vectronic Aerospace GmbH, Germany; radio collaring was in accordance with the University of Minnesota IACUC protocol 0906A67489). If we obtained >2 locations with consecutive movement distances < 100 m within 48 h, sites were identified as locations where a tiger had potentially killed a prey animal (Figure 2; Miller et al., 2013). We investigated these sites on foot and if a kill was located, species, sex, and age class of prey were identified from skeletal material, hair, and hoofs (Lekagul & McNeely, 1977; Phetdee, 2000). We used Phetdee’s (2000) pelage and skeletal size criteria to classify juvenile versus adult. Gaur, banteng, and sambar were classified as adult when > 9 months old, and wild boar were classified as adult when 6 months old. Kills were found at 150 sites but only 87 of these, used by 5 female satellite radio-collared tigers, were investigated to study kill site characteristics. At kill sites, we identified the actual kill site, which could be identified from the drag marks or the presence of the rumen, which is usually removed before the animal is dragged. Kill sites were compared with 87 randomly sampled sites (excluding kill sites) from the home ranges of the five female collared tigers.

Figure 2.

(a) We found this sambar kill by visiting sites where a tiger was located three or more times in consecutive 1 hr GPS fixes. (b) We occasionally placed camera traps at kill sites to obtain additional information on a tiger’s condition and, for females, their reproductive status (photo: Thailand Tiger Project).

10.1177_1940082917719000-fig2.tif

Prey abundance data

To test the hypothesis that tigers kill prey in areas of high prey abundance, we assessed relative prey abundance at both kill and random sites. At each site, we searched for tracks and dung of sambar, banteng, gaur, water buffalo, wild boar (Sus scrofa), and muntjac (Muntiacus muntjac) within four 10 -m radius subplots which were oriented in cardinal directions 30 m from the site center. An independent sample t-test was used to compare the relative prey abundance at kill and random sites.

Ecological covariate data

To examine factors that influence kill site location, we chose 12 ecological covariates that we hypothesized might influence tiger hunting success. A 30 × 30 m plot was placed at each kill and random site to quantify eight of these ecological features. Vegetation structure at these plots was characterized as number of shrubs, percent of crown cover, number of barrier features (e.g., fallen logs, lianas), bamboo clump density, basal area of trees, tree density, visibility, and slope (Table 1). Visibility at both kill and random sites were measured as percentage visible of a 50 × 100-cm board placed at the center of each plot (Nudds, 1977). These variables were combined with four geographic variables generated using ArcMap 9.3 (Esri, Redlands, CA) that were also hypothesized to influence catchability; these were distances to permanent stream, distance to seasonal stream, distance to salt lick, and elevation. Collinearity between these 12 variables was assessed prior to analysis using Spearman’s rank correlation test. A set of generalized linear models with binomial error distributions and a logit link function was used to evaluate the ecological variables that best differentiate kills sites from random sites. The most likely model was selected using Akaike’s information criteria (Burnham & Anderson, 2002). Statistical analyses were performed in R software (R Core Team, 2015). The importance of ecological variables in the top performing models was assessed based on their respective z-values, the associated probability of each variable’s beta coefficient, and the 94.5% confidence interval of the beta estimates.

Table 1.

Description of the 12 Ecological Covariates That Were Used for Comparison of Kill and Random Sites.

10.1177_1940082917719000-table1.tif

Results

Prey Species and Prey Abundance

Eleven prey species were identified at 150 kill sites (Table 2). Sambar, banteng, and gaur composed 95.1% of biomass of these kills. Analysis of hair texture, teeth, and skeletal remains indicated that tigers killed adult prey 67.6% of the time or three times more frequently than juvenile prey (22.7%); we could not classify age of 9.7% of kills. Mean relative abundance of prey sign at 87 kill sites (77.9 sign/ha, n = 77, SD = 72.3) was significantly lower than at 87 randomly selected nonkill sites (139.2 sign/ha, n = 77, SD = 112.3 sign; t = − 4.04, df = 129.79, p = .00009).

Table 2.

The Number of Tiger’s Kills and Biomass of Species in 150 Kills in Huai Kha Khaeng Wildlife Sanctuary, Thailand.

10.1177_1940082917719000-table2.tif

Ecological Covariates

To identify the ecological variables that best predict kill site characteristics, we first examined colinearity and eliminated distance to salt licks, which was highly correlated with elevation (r = .73) as well as distance to permanent streams (r = .87). The model with lowest Akaike information criteria included three variables: low shrub density, high crown cover, and low tree density (Table 3). The deviation of z-values from zero and their associated p values indicated the strength of each variable in the top two models. In the top model, the strongest deviation of z from zero was shrub cover (z = −2.813, p > |z| = .0049). The next most important variable was crown cover (z = 2.721, p > |z| = .0065). There was only weak support for the third variable, tree density (Table 3). The second best model included a fourth ecological correlate, barrier cover, but its z-value was not significant (>0.05). The third- and fourth-ranked models were subsets of Models 1 and 2 and garnered weights of 9% and 5%. These top four models had an accumulative weight of 90%).

Table 3.

Summary Statistics for the Top Four Models of Kill Site Characteristics With an Accumulative Weight of 0.90 for Tigers in Huai Kha Khaeng Wildlife Sanctuary, Thailand.

10.1177_1940082917719000-table3.tif

Discussion

Identification of Prey at Kill Sites

We identified only 11 mammalian prey species at kill sites as compared with 16 species reported in an analysis of scats (Phetdee, 2000) from the same area. Larger prey in our study (sambar, gaur, and banteng) composed 95.1% of the biomass of kills we identified, which was higher than the 88% biomass of these prey reported by Phetdee (2000). It is not surprising that prey identified from kill sites are biased toward larger prey species because we identified kill sites by noting a sequence of clumped 1-hr interval locations. Small kills could be processed before we noticed a clump. Also we visited kill sites a mean of 8 days after a kill and the scattering of small kills made them more difficult to find. However, identifying smaller prey is less important to understanding kill site selection because, as shown by scat surveys, these animals compose 8.5% of the tiger’s diet Phetdee (2000).

Three of our 87 kills were elephants (Elephas maximus) < 1 year in age, which is an observation not reported in the previous scat study (Phetdee, 2000). We speculate that young elephants were not found in the past because, prior to the mid-1990s, elephant poaching was widespread. With improved management beginning in the 1990s, elephant numbers and recruitment have increased (Kanchanasaka, 2010).

We initially thought that high prey abundance would be a good predictor of kill sites because several studies on kill site habitat selection by large carnivores support the hypothesis that kill site location within an animal’s home range is largely influenced by prey abundance (Davidson et al., 2012). Furthermore, a strategy to hunt in areas of high prey abundance, especially by adult females that need to meet the energetic demands of feeding their offspring, should optimize energy gained at the lowest risk cost (Heurich et al., 2016). Our study, however, did not support the prey abundance-hypothesis that killing success, and thus energy, are maximized by hunting in areas of high prey abundance. On the contrary, we found that kill sites had a significantly lower prey abundance than random sites located along a tiger’s route of travel. We do not know the extent to which prey may have avoided kill sites, but found no literature indicating prey shift their range.

Thus, our findings led us to evaluate an alternative set of landscape hypotheses that certain habitat attributes are more important to killing success than prey abundance. Several previous studies also support landscape hypotheses that carnivores select habitats where prey are more susceptible to predation (Balme et al., 2007; Belotti et al., 2013; Davidson et al., 2012; Hopcraft, Sinclair, & Packer, 2005).

Of the 12 ecological correlates we examined to explain tiger hunting location, low cover was the most important variable in all of the top-ranked models. This was surprising because tigers, lions (Panthera leo), and other felids favor stalking to within a short distance followed by a relatively short chase. Thus, adequate cover is essential. Lions and tigers accelerate faster than many of their prey, but their top speed peaks much below that of their prey so they must initiate an attack at a close distance (Elliott et al., 1977). Furthermore, their large, muscular body makes it energetically costly to maintain high speed over a long chase.

However, large cats must first detect their prey, thus they need an optimal combination of habitat structure to first locate, and then successfully stalk and ambush their prey (Lamprecht, 1978; Murphy & Ruth, 2010). To understand the role of different ecological covariates in large carnivore hunting success, Hebblewhite et al. (2005) suggest using Holling’s (1959) theoretical framework to decompose hunting success into two components: first, the instantaneous probability of encounter followed by the conditional probability of a successful stalk leading to a kill. In this context, less cover at a site with generally high cover would be favored to increase the initial probability of detection. The median cover at kill sites was 10,410 shrubs/ha. Given that kills are made at sites with considerably less cover than random sites (median = 14,190 shrubs/ha), areas with lower cover have adequate cover for tigers to successfully hunt.

The second most important ecological correlate was crown cover. It is unclear what advantage crown cover offers hunting tigers. Mysterud (1996) hypothesized that crown cover provides shade which is sought by deer for sites to rest and ruminate. He reported that roe deer (Capreolus capreolus) prefers to bed down below dense canopy to seek shade: similarly, in Thailand, sambar and muntjac also bed at sites with high over story cover (Brodie & Brockelman, 2009). If indeed, crown cover creates preferred resting sites, it may have a consequence of reducing vigilance in resting and ruminating ungulates (Blanchard & Fritz, 2007).

Low tree density was also an ecological correlate in the top two supported models, but in neither was the probability of its beta coefficient significant (Table 4). In the second-ranked model, ground-level barrier (e.g., fallen trees, lianas, dense tangled vegetation) was a forth ecological correlate. We hypothesize that barriers such as fallen trees increase cover for stalking tigers and they may also limit escape options for prey, but the probability associated its beta coefficient was also not significant (Table 4). Models 3 and 4 were subsets of Models 1 and 2 and again low shrub cover and high crown cover were significant.

Table 4.

The Beta Coefficients for the Best Model Which Included Low Shrub Cover, High Crown Cover and Low Tree Density.

10.1177_1940082917719000-table4.tif

Implications for conservation

Globally, significant remaining tiger habitat is fragmented into small units, and as a result, most tiger populations are critically small (Kenny, Allendori, McDougal, & Smith, 2014; Walston et al., 2010). Managers have only three options for increasing the viability of tiger populations. They can increase the land base that supports tigers, increase connectivity between populations or increase prey abundance, especially where human activities have resulted in prey depletion (Karanth & Stith, 1999). Fire management has been widely used to reduce shrub cover in Huai Kha Khaeng, especially of less nutritious species and to provide young, more nutritious high-protein grasses (Sunquist & Sunquist, 1989) for ungulates and ultimately tiger prey. Although we found tigers kill more often in areas of low shrub cover, if cover is reduced too much, adequate cover for stalking prey will be limited. We suggest that altering fire management to create smaller burns will potentially create a mosaic between burned and nonburned areas which may optimize cover for prey detection, but also produce adequate cover for tigers.

Author Contributions

Field research and analysis: SP, AS, SD; Research design: SP, AS, VC, NP, Advice on statistical analysis: NP; Manuscript preparation: SP, AS, JS.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Chorpaka Vichittrakulchai, Ekaphol Plaidaeng, Kittisak Thongvichit, Supawat Khaewphakdee, Onsa Norrasarn, and Zack Beach for assistance in the field. Saksit Simcharoen provided advice throughout the study. The authors also thank Oranuch Mahaphant for supporting this project. The authors appreciate comments from Francie Cuthbert on an earlier version of the manuscript.

Declaration of conflicting interests

The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding

The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: Funding was provided by USFWS, PTTEP, and the Department of National Parks, Wildlife and Plant Conservation, Thailand.

References

1.

Balme, G., Hunter, L., Slotow, R., (2007) Feeding habitat selection by hunting leopards Panthera pardus in a woodland savanna: Prey catchability versus abundance. Animal Behavior 74: 589–598. Google Scholar

2.

Belotti, E., Červený, J., Šustr, P., Kreisinger, J., Gaibani, G., Bufka, L., (2013) Foraging sites of Eurasian lynx Lynx lynx: Relative importance of microhabitat and prey occurrence. Wildlife Biology 19: 188–201. Google Scholar

3.

Blanchard, P., Fritz, H., (2007) Induced or routine vigilance while foraging. Oikos 116: 1603–1608. Google Scholar

4.

Brodie, J. F., Brockelman, W. Y., (2009) Bed site selection of red muntjac (Muntiacus muntjak) and sambar (Rusa unicolor) in a tropical seasonal forest. Ecological Research 24: 1251–1256. Google Scholar

5.

Burnham, K. P., Anderson, D. R., (2002) Model selection and multimodel inference: A practical information-theoretical approach, New York, NY: Springer-Verlag. Google Scholar

6.

Caro, T. M., Fitzgibbon, C. D., (1992) Large carnivores and their prey: The quick and the dead. In: Crawley, M. J., (ed.) Natural enemies: The population biology of predators parasites and diseases, Oxford, England: Blackwell Scientific Publications, pp. 117–142. Google Scholar

7.

Davidson, Z., Valeix, M., Loveridge, A. J., Hunt, J. E., Johnson, P. J., Madzikanda, H., Macdonald, D. W., (2012) Environmental determinants of habitat and kill site selection in large carnivore: Scale matters. Journal of Mammalogy 93: 677–685. Google Scholar

8.

Duangchantrasiri, S., Umponjan, M., Simcharoen, S., Pattanavibool, A., Chaiwattana, S., Maneerat, S., Karanth, K. U., (2016) Dynamics of a low-density tiger population in Southeast Asia in the context of improved law enforcement. Conservation Biology 30: 639–648. Google Scholar

9.

Elliott, J. P., Cowan, I. M., Hollings, C. S., (1977) Prey capture by the African lion. Canadian Journal of Zoology 55: 1811–1828. Google Scholar

10.

Hayward, M. W., Kerley, G. I. H., (2005) Prey preferences of the lion (Panthera leo). Journal of Zoology 267: 309–322. Google Scholar

11.

Hebblewhite, M., Merrill, E. H., McDonald, T. L., (2005) Spatial decomposition of predation risk using resource selection functions: An example in wolf-elk predator-prey system. Oikos 111: 101–111. Google Scholar

12.

Heurich, M., Zeis, K., Küchenhoff, H., Müller, J., Belotti, E., Bufka, L., Woelfing, B., (2016) Selective predation of a stalking predator on ungulate prey. PLoS One 11: e0158449. Google Scholar

13.

Holling, C. S., (1959) The Components of predation as revealed by a study of small-mammal predation of the European sawfly. The Canadian Entomologist 91: 293–320. Google Scholar

14.

Hopcraft, J. G. C., Sinclair, A. R. E., Packer, C., (2005) Planning for success: Serengeti lions seek prey accessibility rather than abundance. Journal of Animal Ecology 74: 559–566. Google Scholar

15.

Johnson, D. H., (1980) The comparison of usage and availability measurements for evaluating resource preference. Ecology 61: 65–71. Google Scholar

16.

Kanchanasaka, B., (2010) Status and distribution of large mammals in Thailand, Bangkok, Thailand: Department of National Parks, Wildlife and Plant Conservation. Google Scholar

17.

Karanth, K. U., Stith, B. M., (1999) Prey density as a critical determinant of tiger population viability. In: Seidensticker, J., Christie, S., Jackson, P., (eds) Riding the tiger: Tiger conservation in human-dominated landscapes, Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press, pp. 110–113. Google Scholar

18.

Kenny, J., Allendori, F. W., McDougal, C., Smith, J. L. D., (2014) How much gene flow is needed to avoid inbreeding depression in wild tiger populations? Proceedins of the Royal Society Publishing B 281: 20133337. Google Scholar

19.

Krebs, J. R., Davies, N. B., (1993) An introduction to behavioural ecology, Oxford, England: Blackwell Scientific Publications. Google Scholar

20.

Kruuk, H., (1986) Interactions between Felidae and their prey species: A review. In: Miller, S. D., Everett, D. D., (eds) Cats of the world: Biology, conservation and management, Washington, DC: National Wildlife Federation, pp. 333–352. Google Scholar

21.

Lamprecht, J., (1978) The relationship between food competition and foraging group size in some larger carnivores. Ethology 46: 337–343. Google Scholar

22.

Lekagul, B., McNeely, J. A., (1977) Mammals of Thailand, Bangkok, Thailand: Kurusapha. Google Scholar

23.

Litvaitis, J. A., Sherburne, J. A., Bissonette, J. A., (1986) Bobcat habitat use and home range size in relation to prey density. The Journal of Wildlife Management 50: 110–117. Google Scholar

24.

Manly, B. F., McDonald, L., Thomas, D., McDonald, T. L., Erickson, W. P., (2002) Resource selection by animals: Statistical design and analysis for field biology, Netherlands: Springer. Google Scholar

25.

Miller, C. S., Hebblewhite, M., Petrunenko, Y. K., Serodkin, I. V., DeCesare, N. J., Goodrich, J. M., Miquelle, D. G., (2013) Estimating Amur tiger (Panthera tigris altaica) kill rates and potential consumption rates using global positioning system collars. Journal of Mammalogy 94: 845–855. Google Scholar

26.

Murphy, K., Ruth, T., (2010) Diet and prey selection of a perfect predator. In: Hornocker, M., Negri, S., (eds) Cougar: ecology and conservation, Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press, pp. 118–137. Google Scholar

27.

Murray, D. L., Boutin, S., O’Donoghue, M., Nams, V. O., (1995) Hunting behavior of sympatric felid and canid in relation to vegetative cover. Animal Behaviour 50: 1203–1210. Google Scholar

28.

Mysterud, A., (1996) Bed-site selection by adult roe deer Capreolus capreolus in southern Norway during summer. Wildlife Biology 2: 101–106. Google Scholar

29.

Nudds, T. D., (1977) Quantifying the vegetative structure of wildlife cover. Wildlife Society Bulletin 5: 113–117. Google Scholar

30.

Palomares, F., Delibes, M., Revilla, E., Calzada, J., Fedriani, J. M., (2001) Spatial ecology of Iberian lynx and abundance of European rabbits in southwestern Spain. Wildlife Monographs 148: 1–36. Google Scholar

31.

Phetdee, A. (2000). Feeding habits of the tiger (Panthera tigris Linnaeus) in Huai Kha Khaeng Wildlife Sanctuary by fecal analysis (MSc Thesis). Kasetsart University, Bangkok, Thailand. Google Scholar

32.

R Core Team. (2015). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Retrieved from  http://www.R-project.org/Google Scholar

33.

Schaller, G. B., (1967) The deer and the tiger: A study of wildlife in India, Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Google Scholar

34.

Simcharoen, A., Savini, T., Gale, G. A., Simcharoen, S., Duangchantrasiri, S., Pakpien, S., Smith, J. L. D., (2014) Female tiger Panthera tigris home range size and prey abundance: Important metrics for management. Oryx 48: 370–377. Google Scholar

35.

Smith, J. L. D., McDougal, C., Miquelle, D., (1989) Scent marking in free-ranging tigers, Panthera tigris. Animal Behaviour 37: 1–10. Google Scholar

36.

Spong, G., (2002) Space use in lions, Panthera leo, in the Selous Game Reserve: Social and ecological factors. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 52: 303–307. Google Scholar

37.

Sunquist, M. E., Sunquist, F. C., (1989) Ecological constraints on predation by large felids. In: Gittleman, J. L., (ed.) Carnivore behavior, ecology, and evolution, New York, NY: Springer, pp. 283–301. Google Scholar

38.

Stephens, D. W., Krebs, J. R., (1986) Foraging theory, New Jersey: Princeton University Press. Google Scholar

39.

Walston, J., Robinson, J. G., Bennett, E. L., Breitenmose, U., Fonseca, G. A. B. F., Goodrich, J., Wibisono, H., (2010) Bringing the tiger back from the brink-the six percent solution. Plos Biology 8: 1–4. Google Scholar
© The Author(s) 2017 This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 License (http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/) which permits non-commercial use, reproduction and distribution of the work without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages (https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage).
Somporn Pakpien, Achara Simcharoen, Somphot Duangchantrasiri, Vijak Chimchome, Nantachai Pongpattannurak, and James L. D. Smith "Ecological Covariates at Kill Sites Influence Tiger (Panthera tigris) Hunting Success in Huai Kha Khaeng Wildlife Sanctuary, Thailand," Tropical Conservation Science 10(1), (1 January 2020). https://doi.org/10.1177/1940082917719000
Received: 30 April 2017; Accepted: 9 June 2017; Published: 1 January 2020
KEYWORDS
cluster locations
Huai Kha Khaeng Wildlife Sanctuary
hunting success
tiger kill site characteristics
tiger prey
Back to Top