Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 January 2020 Spider Monkeys in Human-Modified Landscapes : The Importance of the Matrix
Víctor Arroyo-Rodríguez, Gloria Karina Pérez-Elissetche, José D. Ordóñez-Gómez, Arturo González-Zamora, Óscar M. Chaves, Sònia Sánchez-López, Colin A. Chapman, Karenina Morales-Hernández, Miriam Pablo-Rodríguez, Gabriel Ramos-Fernández
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

With the extant of tropical forest degradation, primates increasingly inhabit forest patches embedded in anthropogenic matrices. Such matrices are composed of different land cover types (e.g., agricultural lands and cattle pastures), but large uncertainty remains about the ability of primates to use these land covers. Here, we assessed the use of the landscape matrix by spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) in 13 forest sites from three countries (Mexico, Costa Rica, and El Salvador). Based on ad libitum records from >212 months of field observations, we found that spider monkeys used four types of land covers for feeding or traveling: secondary vegetation, isolated trees, tree crops, and vegetation corridors. Secondary vegetation was more frequently used than the other land covers. The number of land covers present in the matrix was positively related to the number of land covers used for traveling and feeding. Monkeys consumed 53 plant species in the matrix, mostly native and old-growth or late-successional forest species, although they also used three cultivated tree species. Most species were trees, especially from preferred food species, although monkeys also used palms, lianas, and shrubs. Monkeys fed principally from fruits, but they also used leaves, wood, and flowers. Most species were used from secondary vegetation and isolated trees. These findings suggest that the landscape matrix can provide supplementary food sources for this endangered primate and opportunities for traveling (i.e., spatial connectivity) in human-modified landscapes—information that can be used to improve conservation strategies, especially under the context of land-sharing management strategies (e.g., agroforestry).

Introduction

With increasing land-use change across the tropics (Achard et al., 2014), a large proportion of global biodiversity is found in human-modified tropical landscapes (HMTLs; Melo, Arroyo-Rodríguez, Fahrig, Martínez-Ramos, & Tabarelli, 2013). Such landscapes are highly heterogeneous, as they are usually composed of forest patches surrounded by different types of human-created land covers, including human settlements, agricultural lands, cattle pastures, secondary forests, live fences, and isolated trees (hereafter, anthropogenic matrix). Because forest patches in these landscapes may support a limited availability of food resources (e.g., Arroyo-Rodríguez & Mandujano, 2006; Chaves, Stoner, & Arroyo-Rodríguez, 2012), forest animals, including most primates, can supplement their food intake by using resources from the surrounding matrix, a process named “landscape supplementation” (Dunning, Danielson, & Pulliam, 1992). The matrix can also be used by forest animals to move among forest patches (Franklin & Lindenmayer, 2009), but different land covers show different resistance to interpatch movements (da Silva, Ribeiro, Hasui, da Costa, & da Cunha, 2015; Ricketts, 2001). Unfortunately, large uncertainty remains about the ability of most animal species to use the matrix, the land cover types more frequently used for feeding and traveling, and the food resources used in each land cover. Such knowledge is urgently needed to understand biodiversity patterns in HMTLs, and thus for the construction of informed conservation planning (Franklin & Lindenmayer, 2009; Prevedello & Vieira, 2010; Watling, Nowakowski, Donnelly, & Orrock, 2011), specially for threatened species such as most primates (Estrada et al., 2017).

Primates are often found in HMTLs (Marsh et al., 2013), where a large number of patterns and processes may threaten their survival (Arroyo-Rodríguez & Mandujano, 2009; Estrada et al., 2017; Graham, Matthews, & Turner, 2016). Yet, because most studies in HMTLs assess the impact of forest patch characteristics (e.g., patch size, isolation) on the diet, behavior, and demography of primates (Arroyo-Rodríguez et al., 2013; Carretero-Pinzón, Defler, McAlpine, & Rhodes, 2016), our understanding of the importance of the landscape matrix for primates is incipient. This gap of information may be related, at least partially, to the fact that forest animals may perceive the matrix as a dangerous place where they are more exposed to predation and hunting, and thus, they avoid the use of this landscape element. However, there is evidence that primates are able to use different land covers in the matrix (Estrada et al., 2017), including agroecosystems (Estrada, Raboy, & Oliveira, 2012), and other land covers, such as secondary forests, live fences, subsistence orchards, and isolated trees; but the available information is limited to only a few folivorous (Colobus guereza: Harris & Chapman, 2007; Colobus angolensis: Anderson, Rowcliffe, & Cowlishaw, 2007; Alouatta palliata: Asensio, Arroyo-Rodríguez, Dunn, & Cristóbal-Azkarate, 2009; Alouatta pigra: Pozo-Montuy, Serio-Silva, Chapman, & Bonilla-Sánchez, 2013; Alouatta guariba: Bicca-Marques & Calegaro-Marques, 1995; Chaves & Bicca-Marques, 2017), omnivorous (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii: Reynolds, Wallis, & Kyamanywa, 2003), and frugivorous/insectivorous monkeys (Callicebus negrifrons: Trevelin, Port-Carvalho, Silveira, & Morell, 2007; Cercopithecus ascanius: Baranga, Basuta, Teichroeb, & Chapman, 2012). The available information for frugivorous primates, such as the spider monkey (genus Ateles), is very scarce.

The Geoffroy’s spider monkey (Ateles geoffroyi) is distributed from southeastern Mexico to northwestern Colombia (Di Fiore, Link, & Campbell, 2011). Given its large body size (6–9.4 kg; Ford & Davis, 1992) and mostly frugivorous diet (González-Zamora et al., 2009), A. geoffroyi uses large home ranges (95–900 ha; Wallace, 2008). These ecological features make this species particularly sensitive to forest loss and fragmentation (Boyle & Smith, 2010; Garber, Estrada, & Pavelka, 2006; Michalski & Peres, 2005; Ramos-Fernández & Wallace, 2008). Although there are some published reports of spider monkeys using food resources from the matrix, mostly in agricultural lands (Chaves et al., 2012; Estrada et al., 2006, 2012) and secondary forests (Ramos-Fernández & Ayala-Orozco, 2003; Ramos-Fernández, Smith-Aguilar, Schaffner, Vick, & Aureli, 2013), we lack information concerning the land cover types that are frequently used by this species for feeding and traveling in the matrix. Such information is needed to design biodiversity-friendly landscapes (sensu Melo et al., 2013); for example, increasing resource availability and landscape connectivity for endangered species such as A. geoffroyi.

Here, we compiled ad libitum observations from 10 studies of spider monkeys carried out in 13 forest patches surrounded by different land cover types in three countries (Mexico, Costa Rica, and El Salvador) to document, for the first time: (a) the general patterns of land cover types used for feeding and traveling in the matrix; and (b) the life forms, plant species, and food items used as food resources in each land cover. Because spider monkeys are considered forest-specialist primates, we hypothesized that monkeys can use the matrix for feeding and traveling, but mainly use those land cover types that are structurally and compositionally more similar to the forest patches in which they reside. Spider monkeys in fragmented forests are known to increase their consumption of leaves (e.g., González-Zamora et al., 2009)—a plant item with lower energetic content and higher levels of secondary compounds and structural material than other plant items, such as fruits (Felton, Felton, Lindenmayer, & Foley, 2009a; Felton et al., 2009b; Milton, 1981). Therefore, we hypothesized that monkeys in the matrix will use primarily fruits (especially from preferred tree species) to supplement their diet, and thus increase the quality of their diet (Asensio et al., 2009; Dunning et al., 1992). This hypothesis is particularly plausible considering that the matrix can be experienced by primates as a relatively hostile place, and thus, they are not expected to leave forest patches to feed from plant items such as leaves, which are readily available within their home patches (Asensio et al., 2009; although note that leaf quality is highly variable: Snaith & Chapman, 2005).

Methods

Data Base

We compiled ad libitum observations of spider monkeys feeding or traveling (i.e., movements within and between land cover types) in different land cover types in the anthropogenic matrix surrounding their home forest patches from 10 studies of their ecology and behavior carried out in 13 forest sites from Mexico (n = 9 patches), El Salvador (n = 3), and Costa Rica (n = 1), totaling more than 212 months ( > 17 years) of field observations (Table 1). We did not take into account the parts of the matrix used for resting, because we did not observed sleeping sites in the matrix. As these studies were not designed to assess matrix use by monkeys, we do not have information on the time spent in each activity and land cover type. Also, sampling efforts were highly variable among studies (18 ± 32 months per study, mean ± SD). Therefore, we based this study on presence/absence data, considering a single event of feeding or traveling within a given land cover type as evidence of occurrence (see below). The forest patches (i.e., discrete forest masses separated by agricultural lands) we studied ranged from 4.6 to 10,800 ha, and included both tropical wet (n = 8 patches) and tropical dry forests (n = 5) following Gentry’s (1982) classification of tropical forests (i.e., <2,000 mm rain/year in tropical dry forests, and >2,800 mm rain/year in tropical wet forests). The composition of the landscape matrix was highly variable, ranging from relatively homogeneous matrices composed of three land covers (e.g., secondary vegetation, cattle pastures, and isolated trees) to heterogeneous matrices composed of six land covers: secondary vegetation, tree crops (i.e., Mangifera indica, Psidium guajava, and Theobroma cacao), vegetation corridors (i.e., live fences and riparian corridors), cattle pastures, and isolated trees (Table 1). Although old secondary forests can be difficult to differentiate from old-growth forests, we focused on secondary forests relatively easy to identify in the field, mostly young regenerating forest stands (<30 years of succession) dominated by light-demanding pioneer genera, such as Cecropia, Ochroma, Piper, Miconia, and Helioparpus, which usually form a discontinuous canopy of less than 20 m tall, with a very high density of relatively small stems. For each forest site we recorded: (a) the land cover types present in the surrounding landscape matrix; (b) the land cover types used for feeding and/or traveling; and (c) the plant species, live forms, and plant items (e.g., fruit, flowers, leaves, and wood) used as food resources in each land cover type. Due to the dominance of pioneer (early colonizer) species that are typically indicative of forest disturbance (Santos et al., 2008; Tabarelli, Peres, & Melo, 2012), we followed the procedure described by Arroyo-Rodríguez, Pineda, Escobar, and Benítez-Malvido (2009) to classify the species based on their successional status: early colonizers (or pioneer species), late-successional (or non-secondary light demanding) species, and old-growth forest (shade-tolerant) species. This ecological group classification was based on information from several floras (e.g., Flora of Veracruz and Neotropical Flora), as well as several species lists (Arroyo-Rodríguez et al., 2009; Hernández-Ruedas et al., 2014, and references therein). Plant nomenclature was updated according to the Royal Botanical Garden and the Missouri Botanical Garden databases ( http://www.theplantlist.org/, accessed on December 10, 2016).

Table 1.

Characteristics of Forest Sites (n = 13) Where Spider Monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) Have Been Observed Using Different Land Cover Types for Feeding and Traveling in the Anthropogenic Matrix Surrounding Their Home Forest Patches.

10.1177_1940082917719788-table1.tif

Data Analyses

Each forest site was considered as an independent record. We first calculated the proportion of forest sites that presented each land cover type (based on a total of 13 forest sites). This proportion indicates the distribution of each land cover type across forest sites, and can be considered a proxy of availability (e.g., secondary vegetation occurred in all forest sites, but isolated trees were found in 9 out of 13 sites; Table 1). We also calculated the proportion of forest sites where monkeys were observed feeding or traveling in each land cover type. As monkeys cannot use land covers that are not present in a given site, in this case, proportions are calculated considering the distribution of each land cover across forest types, and not based on the total number of forest sites (e.g., monkeys fed from isolated trees in 6 sites, but as this land cover type was only present in 9 sites, they used isolated trees for feeding in 67% of sites, and not 46% if we would consider 13 sites; Table 1). We then assessed whether the observed proportions differed from the proportions expected based on the distribution of each land cover type across forest sites with a χ2 test. Using a simple linear regression, we also tested if the number of land cover types used in each forest site were related to the number of land cover types present in the surrounding matrix. Finally, as there was a wide variation in the length of studies and forest patch size (Table 1), and such variation can affect our results and conclusions, we also used linear regressions to assess the effect of study length and patch size on the number of plant species used in the matrix and the number of land covers used for feeding and traveling. All statistical analyses were done with R software (R Core Team 2017) assuming a statistical threshold (alpha) of .05.

Results

Spider monkeys fed and traveled in four different matrix types: secondary vegetation, isolated trees, tree crops (i.e., Mangifera indica, Manilkara zapota, and Theobroma cacao) and vegetation corridors (i.e., live fences and riparian corridors; Table 1). In general, secondary vegetation was more frequently used than the other land covers, both for feeding and traveling (Figure 1). This pattern may be related to the fact that all forest sites showed secondary vegetation in their surrounding matrix, whereas the rest of land covers were present in six to nine forest sites (Table 1; Figure 1). In fact, the proportion of sites where monkeys were observed feeding (χ2 = 0.81, df = 3, p = .85) or traveling (χ2 = 1.85, df = 3, p = .60) within each land cover type did not differ from what we can expect based on the distribution of such land covers across forests sites (Figure 1). In this sense, we found a positive association between the number of land covers in the matrix and the number of land covers used for traveling (r = .66, p = .02) and feeding (r = .54, p = .059; Figure 2).

Figure1.

Proportion (and total numbers above each column) of forest sites where spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) fed or traveled in different land cover types present in the anthropogenic matrix. The proportion of forest sites surrounded by each land cover is added as a proxy of the availability of each land cover, and are calculated based on the total number of study sites (n = 13). Yet, the proportion of forest sites used for feeding and traveling are calculated considering the availability of each land cover across the sites.

10.1177_1940082917719788-fig1.tif

Figure 2.

Association between the number of land covers in the matrix (x-axis) and the number of land covers used by spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) for feeding and traveling in 13 forest sites from Mexico, El Salvador, and Costa Rica. Note that each point represents a different forest site.

10.1177_1940082917719788-fig2.tif

In total, spider monkeys consumed 53 plant species belonging to 20 families (Table 2). Most species (83%) were trees, but spider monkeys also used palms (7%), lianas (6%), and shrubs (4%). All tree species, except Mangifera indica, are native to the respective forest sites and three species are cultivated (Mangifera indica, Theobroma cacao, and Psidium guajava). Most species are old-growth forest species (42%) or late-successional species (34%), and only 19% are early colonizer species.

Table 2.

Plant Species and Food Items Used by Spider Monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) for Feeding in Different Land Cover Types Surrounding 13 Forest Sites Distributed in Three Countries (Costa Rica, El Salvador, and Mexico).

10.1177_1940082917719788-table2.tif

Regarding the frequency of use of each species, most species (72%) were used in one single site (Table 1), and nine species (17%) were used in two forest sites (i.e., Mangifera indica, Tapirira mexicana, Bursera simaruba, Heliocarpus donnellsmithii, Theobroma cacao, Miconia argentea, Brosimum sp., Ficus aurea, and Ficus yoponensis). The species most frequently used were Brosimum alicastrum, Spondias mombin, and Ficus sp., which were used in 3 out of 13 forest sites.

Regarding the plant items eaten, monkeys fed from fruits of 29 species (55% of species) and leaves of 19 species (36%). Five species (Licania platypus, Swietenia macrophylla, Ficus sp., Ficus aurea, and Ficus insipida) were also used as a source of wood and two species (Dussia mexicana and Luehea seemannii) as a source of flowers (Table 2). In fact, Ficus spp. were used for their fruits, leaves, and wood, and monkeys used two plant items of seven species: Bursera simaruba, Enterolobium cyclocarpum, Miconia argentea, Brosimum sp., Ficus aurea, Ficus yoponensis, and Maclura tinctoria.

Most plant species (26 species, 49%) were used in secondary vegetation, and a large number of species (22 species, 42%) were also used as isolated trees (Table 2; Figure 3). Monkeys fed from a lower number of plant species in tree crops (11 species) and vegetation corridors (8 species). Because most species (40 species) were used in one single land cover, the species turnover (β-diversity) between land covers was very high (only 13 plant species were used in two land covers, and no species was used in three or more land covers; Table 2).

Figure 3.

Number of plant species (and percentages from a total of 53 species) used by spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) as a food source in different land cover types within the anthropogenic matrix surrounding their home forest patches.

10.1177_1940082917719788-fig3.tif

Differences in patch size and study length across studies were not related to the number of food species (Figure 4(a) and (b)) and the number of land covers used by spider monkeys for feeding and traveling (Figure 4(c) and (d)). This suggests that matrix use by spider monkeys was largely independent of patch size and study length.

Figure 4.

Effect of home patch size and study length on the number of plant species used by spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) as food resources in the matrix (a to b), and the number of land covers used for feeding and traveling in 13 forest sites from Mexico, El Salvador, and Costa Rica. All R2 values were not significant (p > .7, in all cases).

10.1177_1940082917719788-fig4.tif

Discussion

This study shows that spider monkeys use different land cover types from the anthropogenic matrix for feeding and traveling in HMTLs. We observed monkeys feeding from 53 plant species—a significant figure considering that 16 groups of spider monkeys in five countries (Mexico, Guatemala, El Salvador, Costa Rica, and Panama) fed from 364 plant species (González-Zamora et al., 2009). In fact, most of the plant species are native and old-growth or late-successional forest species considered top food species for spider monkeys (González-Zamora et al., 2009). They consumed different life forms (trees, lianas, palms, and shrubs) and plant items (fruits, leaves, flowers, and wood), but as expected, fruits were the most frequently plant item eaten. Although we cannot quantify the importance of these feeding and traveling events for the fitness of these monkeys in HMTLs, our findings demonstrate that some human-created land covers in the matrix are not entirely hostile to this primate, but rather contain usable resources, not only to supplement their diet but also to move within and between land cover types (Arroyo-Rodríguez & Mandujano, 2009; Dunning et al., 1992; Franklin & Lindenmayer, 2009; Tscharntke et al., 2012). Thus, our study adds to an increasing line of evidence suggesting that forest edges do not represent home range boundaries for different species in fragmented forests (Dunning et al., 1992; Fahrig, 2013; Mendenhall et al., 2014; Tscharntke et al., 2012; Watling et al., 2011).

Although we have no information on the time spent within each land cover outside their home forest patches, we observed monkeys feeding and traveling in patches of secondary vegetation, different types of tree crops, vegetation corridors, and isolated trees, including records from both tropical wet forests and tropical dry forests in Mexico, El Salvador, and Costa Rica. Such behaviors can allow primates to supplement their diet in HMTLs (Dunning et al., 1992). As increasingly demonstrated in studies of primates (Anderson et al., 2007; Asensio et al., 2009; Chaves & Bicca-Marques, 2017; Estrada et al., 2012; Pozo-Montuy et al., 2013), supplementation dynamics are likely of critical relevance for species persistence in fragmented landscapes (Dunning et al., 1992), especially because the availability of food resources can be limited in forest patches, or because patches can be too small to sustain viable populations (e.g., Arroyo-Rodríguez & Mandujano, 2006, 2009).

Supporting the idea that monkeys are supplementing their diet with resources obtained from the matrix, we found that, as predicted, fruits were the most frequently eaten plant item, followed by leaves, wood, and flowers. Because fruits are expected to be scarce within forest patches (e.g., Chaves et al., 2012; González-Zamora et al., 2009, 2012), and have high concentrations of soluble carbohydrates (and energetic content), but low levels of secondary compounds and structural material (Felton et al., 2009a, 2009b; Milton, 1981), the consumption of this food item in the matrix probably improved the quality of spider monkeys’ diet. The consumption of leaves, wood, and flowers was also common in most forest sites, probably as a strategy of protein, lipid, and mineral supplementation (Chaves, Stoner, Ángeles-Campos, & Arroyo-Rodríguez, 2011a; Felton et al., 2009a, 2009b; Rothman, Van Soest, & Pell, 2006). Yet, additional studies comparing the nutritional value of the diet within their home forest patches versus the diet in the matrix are needed to better understand the effect of these feeding events in the matrix on primate’s diet and nutrition.

As expected, secondary vegetation was more frequently used than other land covers. Yet, when comparing the frequency of use of each land cover with their distribution in the sites, our findings suggest that the use of each land cover was directly proportional to their availability, suggesting no selection of particular land covers. In fact, the number of land covers in the matrix was positively related to the number of land covers used for feeding and traveling. Thus, although spider monkeys are strongly threatened by forest loss and disturbance (Garber et al., 2006; Ramos-Fernández & Wallace, 2008), this finding highlights the behavioral flexibility of spider monkeys (Amici, Aureli, & Call, 2008; González-Zamora et al., 2009; Schaffner, Rebecchini, Ramos-Fernandez, Vick, & Aureli, 2012; Wallace, 2008), and the importance of matrix heterogeneity for species conservation in HMTLs (Revilla, Wiegand, Palomares, Ferreras, & Delibes, 2004; Ricketts, 2001; Tubelis, Cowling, & Donnelly, 2004). Each land cover patch can support different resources, thus providing higher resilience and stability in ecological processes, such as feeding and dispersal (see the “landscape–moderated insurance hypothesis”; Tscharntke et al., 2012). The fact that monkeys used different plant species in each land cover suggests that vegetation composition differs widely between land covers (high beta diversity). Thus, for arboreal species such as spider monkeys, the presence of different types of tree covers in the matrix can provide opportunities for traveling (landscape connectivity) and feeding from different food resources (Revilla et al., 2004; Ricketts, 2001; Tubelis et al., 2004). This is probably important in severely deforested/degraded landscapes where increasing interpatch isolation distances can limit species persistence (Fahrig, 2013; Tubelis et al., 2004).

Implications for Conservation

Old-growth tropical forests are the main habitat of spider monkeys, and thus, the preservation of this species-rich ecosystem should be considered of highest priority for the conservation of this endangered primate. The great value of large forest remnants for primate conservation is incontrovertible (Estrada et al., 2017), but what is questionable is the exclusion of the anthropogenic matrix from conservation initiatives (Chapman, Chapman, & Glander 1989; Hockings, Yamakoshi, & Matsuzawa, 2017; Nekaris et al., 2017; Perfecto & Vandermeer, 2010).

In this sense, the maintenance of native and some cultivated trees in the anthropogenic matrix is of critical importance for spider monkeys, as all land cover types used by this primate were composed of trees (both native and cultivated). We refer to agroforests, such as shade cacao plantations (Theobroma cacao), mango (Mangifera indica), and guava (Psidium guajava; e.g., Estrada et al., 2012; Hockings et al., 2017), and other important landscape elements, such as vegetation corridors and isolated trees of native species (Asensio et al., 2009). Overall, the maintenance of these trees in the matrix has numerous benefits for both primates and humans. The benefits for primates go beyond their importance as supplementary food sources, as they are also critical to increase landscape connectivity, favoring interpatch dispersal movements (Pozo-Montuy et al., 2013). The benefits for humans include key ecosystem services, such as carbon sequestration, climate regulation, and water quantity (Díaz, Fargione, Chapin, & Tilman 2006). Furthermore, there are many cobenefits for humans of having both higher tree cover and primates in the landscape, including the soil nutrient enrichment through primates’ defecations, seed dispersal and rapid forest recovery (see below), education/inspiration, and esthetic values (e.g., Díaz et al., 2006; Feeley, 2005; Hockings et al., 2017). Related to the education/inspiration and esthetic values, local people can also obtain important economic resources from ecotourism associated with visits to primate groups, which is known to be an economically significant activity in many locations worldwide (Serio-Silva, 2006; Wolfe, 1991).

The fact that secondary vegetation was more frequently used by spider monkeys than other land covers supports an increasing number of studies underlining the importance of secondary forests for biodiversity conservation (Arroyo-Rodríguez et al., 2017; Martínez-Ramos et al., 2016; Melo et al., 2013; Omeja et al., 2016; Wright & Muller-Landau, 2006). Secondary forest does not only serve as supplementary habitat for forest species (Martínez-Ramos et al., 2016), but it also enhances landscape connectivity (Arroyo-Rodríguez et al., 2017), and provides key ecosystem services (e.g., carbon sequestration; Martínez-Ramos et al., 2016). Here, we show that secondary forests can be used by spider monkeys for feeding and traveling. Although additional studies are needed to better understand the role of these behaviors on primates’ survival and well-being in HMTLs, some long-term studies demonstrate that spider monkeys can actually live and even reproduce successfully in secondary forests, at least when located next to old-growth forests (Ramos-Fernandez & Ayala-Orozco, 2003; Ramos-Fernández et al., 2013). Of course, this does not mean that secondary forests alone are enough for the maintenance of primate populations in the long term, but that they can be used to improve the quality of the anthropogenic matrix, and accelerate forest recovery through secondary succession (see below). Tree crops, vegetation corridors, and isolated trees can also contribute to improve the quality of the matrix, as they can provide important food resources for spider monkeys (note that most plant species consumed from these land covers are classified as top food species for this primate; González-Zamora et al., 2009).

A topic that merits particular attention is the fact that spider monkeys are effective seed dispersers of a large number of plant species (Chaves, Stoner, Arroyo-Rodríguez, & Estrada, 2011b; González-Zamora et al., 2012). Thus, the use of all these land cover patches in the matrix likely contributes to forest recovery in HMTLs. Therefore, maintaining different types of tree covers in the vicinity of occupied forest patches is not only needed to conserve the remaining populations of this endangered primate species in HMTLs but also to preserve important ecological services, such as forest regeneration.

Our results present a straightforward message for conservation biologists and managers, especially when conservation strategies involve the retention of habitat patches in agricultural mosaics (land-sharing approach; Perfecto & Vandermeer, 2010); namely, some land covers in the landscape matrix contain food tree species for primates and other taxa (Dunning et al., 1992), and thus some matrix types can be beneficial to conserve and expand spider monkey populations in fragmented forests. Secondary forest cover is increasing across the Neotropics due to migration of people to urban areas and the abandonment of productive lands (Aide et al., 2013). This represents good news for the conservation of spider monkeys, and puts a priority on taking advantage of this changing situation and managing regenerating landscapes and investigations into primate restoration ecology (Jacob, Vaccaro, Sengupta, Hartter, & Chapman, 2008; Wright & Muller-Landau, 2006; Melo et al., 2013).

Acknowledgments

GKPE thanks Prof. A. Estrada for his useful advice during the development of her MSc thesis. Such advice contributed to improve the present manuscript. We also gratefully acknowledge three anonymous reviewers for their valuable and constructive comments and suggestions.

Author Contributions

Víctor Arroyo-Rodríguez and Gloria K. Pérez-Elissetche planned the research and designed methodology with the advice of G. Ramos-Fernandez. Gloria K. Pérez-Elissetche organized the data and Víctor Arroyo-Rodríguez analyzed the data. Víctor Arroyo-Rodríguez and Gloria K. Pérez-Elissetche wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors provided data and contributed critically to the drafts.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding

The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This research was supported by the American Society of Primatologist (2012 ASP Conservation Grant). GKPE obtained a scholarship from CONACyT, Mexico. The Instituto de Investigaciones en Ecosistemas y Sustentabilidad (UNAM) provided logistical support.

References

1.

Achard, F., Beuchle, R., Mayaux, P., Stibig, H. J., Bodart, C., Brink, A., Simonetti, D., (2014) Determination of tropical deforestation rates and related carbon losses from 1990 to 2010. Global Change Biology 20: 2540–2554. https://doi.org/10.1111/gcb.12605Google Scholar

2.

Aide, T. M., Clark, M. L., Grau, H. R., López-Carr, D., Levy, M. A., Redo, D., Muñiz, M., (2013) Deforestation and reforestation of Latin America and the Caribean (2001–2010). Biotropica 45: 262–271. https://doi.org/0.1111/j.1744-7429.2012.00908.xGoogle Scholar

3.

Amici, F., Aureli, F., Call, J., (2008) Fission-fusion dynamics, behavioral flexibility, and inhibitory control in primates. Current Biology 18: 1415–1419. Retrieved from  http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.020Google Scholar

4.

Anderson, J., Rowcliffe, J. M., Cowlishaw, G., (2007) Does the matrix matter? A forest primate in a complex agricultural landscape. Biological Conservation 135: 212–222. Retrieved from  http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2006.10.022Google Scholar

5.

Argueta Rivas, N. A., & Rivera Hernández, G. M. (2004). Uso de hábitats del mono araña (Ateles geoffroyi) en el área natural protegida Normandia, Usulután, El Salvador (Doctoral dissertation, Universidad de El Salvador). Google Scholar

6.

Arroyo-Rodríguez, V., Cuesta-del Moral, E., Mandujano, S., Chapman, C. A., Reyna-Hurtado, R., Fahrig, L., (2013) Assessing habitat fragmentation effects for primates: The importance of evaluating questions at the correct scale. In: Marsh, L., Chapman, C. A., (eds) Primates in fragments: Complexity and resilience, New York, NY: Springer, pp. 503–523. Google Scholar

7.

Arroyo-Rodríguez, V., Mandujano, S., (2006) Forest fragmentation modifies habitat quality for Alouatta palliata. International Journal of Primatology 27: 1079–1096. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-006-9061-0Google Scholar

8.

Arroyo-Rodríguez, V., Mandujano, S., (2009) Conceptualization and measurement of rainforest fragmentation from the primates’ perspective. International Journal of Primatology 30: 497–514. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-009-9355-0Google Scholar

9.

Arroyo-Rodríguez, V., Melo, F. P. L., Martínez-Ramos, M., Bongers, F., Chazdon, R., Meave, J. A., Tabarelli, M., (2017) Multiple successional pathways in human-modified tropical landscapes: New insights from forest succession, forest fragmentation and landscape ecology research. Biological Reviews 92: 326–340. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12231Google Scholar

10.

Arroyo-Rodríguez, V., Pineda, E., Escobar, F., Benítez-Malvido, J., (2009) Value of small patches in the conservation of plant-species diversity in highly fragmented rainforest. Conservation Biology 23: 729–739. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1523-1739.2008.01120.xGoogle Scholar

11.

Asensio, N., Arroyo-Rodríguez, V., Dunn, J. C., Cristóbal-Azkarate, J., (2009) Conservation value of landscape supplementation for howler monkeys living in forest patches. Biotropica 41: 768–773. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-7429.2009.00533.xGoogle Scholar

12.

Baranga, D., Basuta, G. I., Teichroeb, J. A., Chapman, C. A., (2012) Crop raiding patterns of solitary and social groups of red-tailed monkeys on cocoa pods in Uganda. Tropical Conservation Science 5: 104–111. Google Scholar

13.

Bicca-Marques, J. C., Calegaro-Marques, C., (1995) Locomotion of black howlers in habitat with discontinuous canopy. Folia Primatologica 64: 55–61. https://doi.org/10.1159/000156833Google Scholar

14.

Boyle, S. A., Smith, A. T., (2010) Can landscape and species characteristics predict primate presence in forest fragments in the Brazilian Amazon? Biological Conservation 143: 1134–1143. Retrieved from  http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2010.02.008Google Scholar

15.

Carretero-Pinzón, X., Defler, T. R., McAlpine, C. A., Rhodes, J. R., (2016) What do we know about the effect on patch size on primate species across life history traits? Biodiversity and Conservation 25: 37–66. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10531-015-1028-zGoogle Scholar

16.

Chapman, C. A., Chapman, L. J., Glander, K. E., (1989) Primate populations in northwestern Costa Rica: Potential for recovery. Primate Conservation 10: 37–44. Google Scholar

17.

Chaves, O. M., Bicca-Marques, J. C., (2017) Crop feeding by brown howlers (Alouatta guariba clamitans) in forest fragments: The conservation value of cultivated species. International Journal of Primatology 38: 263–281. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-016-9927-8Google Scholar

18.

Chaves, O. M., Stoner, K. E., Ángeles-Campos, S., Arroyo-Rodríguez, V., (2011a) Wood consumption by Geoffroyi’s spider monkeys and its role in mineral supplementation. PLoS One 6(9): e25070. Retrieved from  http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025070Google Scholar

19.

Chaves, O. M., Stoner, K. E., Arroyo-Rodríguez, V., Estrada, A., (2011b) Effectiveness of spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi vellerosus) as seed dispersers in continuous and fragmented rainforests in southern Mexico. International Journal of Primatology 32: 177–192. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-010-9460-0Google Scholar

20.

Chaves, O. M., Stoner, K. E., Arroyo-Rodríguez, V., (2012) Differences in diet between spider monkey groups living in forest fragments and continuous forest in Lacandona, Mexico. Biotropica 44: 105–113. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-7429.2011.00766.xGoogle Scholar

21.

da Silva, L. G., Ribeiro, M. C., Hasui, E., da Costa, C. A., da Cunha, R. G. T., (2015) Patch size, functional isolation, visibility and matrix permeability influences Neotropical primate occurrence within highly fragmented landscapes. PLoS One 10: e0114025. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0114025Google Scholar

22.

Díaz, S., Fargione, J., ChapinF. S. III, Tilman, D., (2006) Biodiversity loss threatens human well-being. PLoS Biology 4: e277. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0040277Google Scholar

23.

Di Fiore, A., Link, A., Campbell, C. J., (2011) The Atelines: Behavioral and socioecological diversity in a New World radiation. In: Campbell, C., Fuentes, A., MacKinnon, K. C., Panger, M., Beader, S. K., (eds) Primates in perspective, 2nd ed. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press, pp. 155–188. Google Scholar

24.

Dunning, J. B., Danielson, B. J., Pulliam, R., (1992) Ecological processes that affect populations in complex landscapes. Oikos 65: 169–175. Google Scholar

25.

Estrada, A., Garber, P. A., Rylands, A. B., Roos, C., Fernandez-Duque, E., Di Fiore, A., Li, B., (2017) Impending extinction crisis of the world’s primates: Why primates matter. Science Advances 3: e1600946. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.1600946Google Scholar

26.

Estrada, A., Raboy, B. E., Oliveira, L. C., (2012) Agroecosystems and primate conservation in the tropics: a review. American Journal of Primatology 74: 696–711. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22033Google Scholar

27.

Estrada, A., Saenz, J., Harvey, C., Naranjo, E., Muñoz, D., Rosales-Meda, M., (2006) Primates in agroecosystems: Some agricultural practices in Mesoamerican landscapes. In: Estrada, A., Garber, P. A., Pavelka, M. S. M., Luecke, M., (eds) New perspectives in the study of Mesoamerican primates: Distribution, ecology, behaviour and conservation, New York, NY: Springer, pp. 437–470. Google Scholar

28.

Fahrig, L., (2013) Rethinking patch size and isolation effects: the habitat amount hypothesis. Journal of Biogeography 40: 1649–1663. https://doi.org/10.1111/jbi.12130Google Scholar

29.

Feeley, K., (2005) The role of clumped defecation in the spatial distribution of soil nutrients and the availability of nutrients for plant uptake. Journal of Tropical Ecology 21: 99–102. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0266467404001701Google Scholar

30.

Felton, A. M., Felton, A., Lindenmayer, D. B., Foley, W.J., (2009a) Nutritional goals of wild primates. Functional Ecology 23: 70–78. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2435.2008.01526.xGoogle Scholar

31.

Felton, A. M., Felton, A., Wood, J. T., Foley, W. J., Raubenheimer, D., Wallis, I. R., Lindenmayer, D. B., (2009b) Nutritional ecology of Ateles chamek in lowland Bolivia: How macronutrient balancing influences food choices. International Journal of Primatology 30: 675–696. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-009-9367-9Google Scholar

32.

Ford, S., Davis, L., (1992) Systematics and body size: Implications for feeding adaptations in New World monkeys. American Journal of Primatology 24: 723–741. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.1330880403Google Scholar

33.

Franklin, J. F., Lindenmayer, D. B., (2009) Importance of matrix habitats in maintaining biological diversity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America 106: 349–350. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0812016105Google Scholar

34.

Garber, P. A., Estrada, A., Pavelka, M. S. M., (2006) New perspective in the study of Mesoamerican Primates: Concluding comments and conservation priorities. In: Estrada, A., Garber, P. A., Pavelka, M. S. M., Luecke, M., (eds) New perspectives in the study of Mesoamerican primates: Distribution, ecology, behaviour and conservation, New York, NY: Springer, pp. 489–511. Google Scholar

35.

Gentry, A. H., (1982) Patterns of Neotropical plant species diversity. Evolutionary Biology 15: 1–85. Google Scholar

36.

González-Zamora, A., Arroyo-Rodríguez, V., Chaves, O. M., Sánchez-López, S., Stoner, K. E., Riba-Hernández, P., (2009) Diet of spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) in Mesoamerica: Current knowledge and future directions. American Journal of Primatology 71: 8–20. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20625Google Scholar

37.

González-Zamora, A., Arroyo-Rodríguez, V., Oyama, A. K., Sork, V., Chapman, C. A., Stoner, K. E., (2012) Sleeping sites and latrines of spider monkeys in continuous and fragmented rainforests: implications for seed dispersal and forest regeneration. PLoS One 7 (10): e46852. Retrieved from  http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0046852Google Scholar

38.

Graham, T. L., Matthews, H. D., Turner, S. E., (2016) A global-scale evaluation of primate exposure and vulnerability to climate change. International Journal of Primatology 37: 158–174. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-016-9890-4Google Scholar

39.

Harris, T. R., Chapman, C. A., (2007) Variation in diet and ranging of black and White colobus monkeys in Kibale National Park, Uganda. Primates 48: 208–221. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-006-0036-8Google Scholar

40.

Hernández-Ruedas, M. A., Arroyo-Rodríguez, V., Meave, J. A., Martínez-Ramos, M., Ibarra-Manríquez, G., Martínez, E., Santos, B. A., (2014) Conserving tropical tree diversity and forest structure: The value of small rainforest patches in moderately-managed landscapes. PLoS One 9 (6): e98931. Retrieved from  http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0098931Google Scholar

41.

Hockings, K. J., Yamakoshi, G., Matsuzawa, T., (2017) Dispersal of a human-cultivated crop by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) in a forest–farm matrix. International Journal of Primatology 38: 172–193. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-016-9924-yGoogle Scholar

42.

Jacob, A. L., Vaccaro, I., Sengupta, R., Hartter, J., Chapman, C. A., (2008) Integrating landscapes that have experienced rural depopulation and ecological homogenization into tropical conservation planning. Tropical Conservation Science 1: 307–320. https://doi.org/abs/10.1177/194008290800100402Google Scholar

43.

Marsh, L. K., Chapman, C. A., Arroyo-Rodríguez, V., Cobden, A. K., Dunn, J. C., Gabriel, D., Wasserman, M. D., (2013) Primates in fragments 10 years later: Once and future goals. In: Marsh, L., Chapman, C. A., (eds) Primates in fragments: Complexity and Resilience (pp. 503–523). Developments in Primatology: Progress and Prospects, New York: Springer. Google Scholar

44.

Martínez-Ramos, M., Pingarroni, A., Rodríguez-Velázquez, J., Toledo-Chelala, L., Zermeño-Hernández, I., Bongers, F., (2016) Natural forest regeneration and ecological restoration in human-modified tropical landscapes. Biotropica 48: 745–757. https://doi.org/10.1111/btp.12382Google Scholar

45.

Melo, F. P. L., Arroyo-Rodríguez, V., Fahrig, L., Martínez-Ramos, M., Tabarelli, M., (2013) On the hope for biodiversity-friendly tropical landscapes. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 28: 462–468. Retrieved from  http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2013.01.001Google Scholar

46.

Mendenhall, C. D., Frishkoff, L. O., Santos-Barrera, G., Pacheco, J., Mesfun, E., Quijano, F. M., Pringl, R. M., (2014) Countryside biogeography of Neotropical reptiles and amphibians. Ecology 95: 856–870. https://doi.org/10.1890/12-2017.1Google Scholar

47.

Michalski, F., Peres, C. A., (2005) Anthropogenic determinants of primate and carnivore local extinctions in a fragmented forest landscape of southern Amazonia. Biological Conservation 124: 383–396. Retrieved from  http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2005.01.045Google Scholar

48.

Milton, K., (1981) Food choice and digestive strategies of two sympatric primate species. The American Naturalist 117: 476–495. Google Scholar

49.

Nekaris, K. A. I., Poindexter, S., Reinhardt, K. D., Sigaud, M., Cabana, F., Wirdateti, W., Nijman, V., (2017) Coexistence between Javan Slow Lorises (Nycticebus javanicus) and humans in a dynamic agroforestry landscape in west Java, Indonesia. International Journal of Primatology 38: 303–320. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-017-9960-2Google Scholar

50.

Omeja, P., Lawes, M. J., Corriveau, A., Valenta, K., Sarkar, D., Paim, F. P., Chapman, C. A., (2016) Recovery of the animal and plant communities across large scales in Kibale National Park, Uganda. Biotropica 48: 770–729. https://doi.org/10.1111/btp.12360Google Scholar

51.

Perfecto, I., Vandermeer, J., (2010) The agroecological matrix as alternative to the land-sparing/agriculture intensification model. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107: 5786–5791. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0905455107Google Scholar

52.

Pozo-Montuy, G., Serio-Silva, J. C., Chapman, C. A., Bonilla-Sánchez, Y. M., (2013) Resource use in a landscape matrix by an arboreal primate: Evidence of supplementation in black howlers (Alouatta pigra). International Journal of Primatology 34: 714–731. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-013-9691-yGoogle Scholar

53.

Prevedello, J. A., Vieira, M. V., (2010) Does the type of matrix matter? A quantitative review of the evidence. Biodiversity and Conservation 5: 1205–1223. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10531-009-9750-zGoogle Scholar

54.

Ramos-Fernández, G., Ayala-Orozco, B., (2003) Population size and habitat use of spider monkeys in Punta Laguna, México. In: Marsh, L., Chapman, C.A., (eds) Primates in fragments: Ecology and conservation, New York, NY: Springer, pp. 191–210. Google Scholar

55.

Ramos-Fernandez, G., Smith Aguilar, S. E., Schaffner, C. M., Vick, L. G., Aureli, F., (2013) Site fidelity in space use by spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) in the Yucatan Peninsula, Mexico. PLoS One 8(5): e62813. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0062813Google Scholar

56.

Ramos-Fernández, G., Wallace, R. B., (2008) Spider monkey conservation in the twenty-first century: Recognizing risks and opportunities. In: Campbell, C. J., (ed.) Spider monkeys: Behavior, ecology and evolution of the genus Ateles, Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press, pp. 351–376. Google Scholar

57.

Revilla, E., Wiegand, T., Palomares, F., Ferreras, P., Delibes, M., (2004) Effects of matrix heterogeneity on animal dispersal: From individual behavior to metapopulation-level parameters. The American Naturalist 164: 130–153. https://doi.org/10.1086/424767Google Scholar

58.

Reynolds, V., Wallis, J., Kyamanywa, R., (2003) Fragments, sugar and chimpanzees in Masindi District, Western Uganda. In: Marsh, L., (ed.) Primates in fragments: Ecology and conservation, New York, NY: Springer, pp. 309–320. Google Scholar

59.

Ricketts, T. H., (2001) The matrix matters: Effective isolation in fragmented landscapes. The American Naturalist 158: 87–99. Google Scholar

60.

Rothman, J. M., Van Soest, P. J., Pell, A. N., (2006) Decaying wood is asodium source formountain gorillas. Biology Letters 2: 321–324. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2006.0480Google Scholar

61.

Santos, B. A., Peres, C. A., Oliveira, M. A., Grillo, A., Alves-Costa, C. P., Tabarelli, M., (2008) Drastic erosion in functional attributes of tree assemblages in Atlantic forest fragments of northeastern Brazil. Biological Conservation 141: 249–260. Retrieved from  http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2007.09.018Google Scholar

62.

Serio-Silva, J. C., (2006) Las Islas de los Changos (the Monkey Islands): The economic impact of ecotourism in the region of Los Tuxtlas, Veracruz, Mexico. American Journal of Primatology 68: 499–506. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20245Google Scholar

63.

Schaffner, C. M., Rebecchini, L., Ramos-Fernandez, G., Vick, L. G., Aureli, F., (2012) Spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi yucatenensis) cope with the negative consequences of hurricanes through changes in diet, activity budget, and fission–fusion dynamics. International Journal of Primatology 33: 922–936. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-012-9621-4Google Scholar

64.

Snaith, T. V., Chapman, C. A., (2005) Towards an ecological solution to the folivores paradox: Patch depletion as an indicator of within-group scramble competition in red colobus monkeys. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology 59: 185–190. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-005-0023-xGoogle Scholar

65.

Tabarelli, M., Peres, C. A., Melo, F. P. L., (2012) The ‘few winners and many losers’ paradigm revisited: emerging prospects for tropical forest biodiversity. Biological Conservation 155: 136–140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2012.06.020Google Scholar

66.

Trevelin, L. C., Port-Carvalho, M., Silveira, M., Morell, E., (2007) Abundance, habitat use and diet of Callicebus nigrifrons Spix (Primates, Pitheciidae) in Cantareira State Park, Sao Paulo, Brazil. Revista Brasileira de Zoologia 24: 1071–1077. Retrieved from  http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0101-81752007000400026Google Scholar

67.

Tscharntke, T., Tylianakis, J. M., Rand, T. A., Didham, P. K., Fahrig, L., Bátary, P., (2012) Landscape moderation of biodiversity patterns and processes-eight hypotheses. Biological Reviews 87: 661–685. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-185x.2011.00216.xGoogle Scholar

68.

Tubelis, D. P., Cowling, A., Donnelly, C., (2004) Landscape supplementation in adjacent savannas and its implications for the design of corridors for forest birds in the central Cerrado, Brazil. Biological Conservation 118: 353–364. Retrieved from  http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2003.09.014Google Scholar

69.

Wallace, R. B., (2008) Factors influencing spider monkeyhabitat use and ranging patterns. In: Campbell, C. A., (ed.) Spider monkeys: Behavior, ecology and evolution of the genus Ateles, Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press, pp. 138–154. Google Scholar

70.

Watling, J. L., Nowakowski, A. J., Donnelly, M. A., Orrock, J. L., (2011) Meta-analysis reveals the importance of matrix composition for animals in fragmented habitat. Global Ecology and Biogeography 20: 209–217. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1466-8238.2010.00586.xGoogle Scholar

71.

Wolfe, L. D., (1991) Macaques, pilgrims, and tourist revisited. National Geographic Research and Education 7: 241. Google Scholar

72.

Wright, S. J., Muller-Landau, H. C., (2006) The future of tropical forest species. Biotropica 38: 287–301. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-7429.2006.00154.xGoogle Scholar
© The Author(s) 2017 This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 License (http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/) which permits non-commercial use, reproduction and distribution of the work without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages (https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage).
Víctor Arroyo-Rodríguez, Gloria Karina Pérez-Elissetche, José D. Ordóñez-Gómez, Arturo González-Zamora, Óscar M. Chaves, Sònia Sánchez-López, Colin A. Chapman, Karenina Morales-Hernández, Miriam Pablo-Rodríguez, and Gabriel Ramos-Fernández "Spider Monkeys in Human-Modified Landscapes : The Importance of the Matrix," Tropical Conservation Science 10(1), (1 January 2020). https://doi.org/10.1177/1940082917719788
Received: 10 March 2017; Accepted: 16 June 2017; Published: 1 January 2020
KEYWORDS
Atelidae
Behavioral flexibility
compositional heterogeneity
corridors
fragmentation
habitat loss
land sharing
Back to Top