BioOne.org will be down briefly for maintenance on 17 December 2024 between 18:00-22:00 Pacific Time US. We apologize for any inconvenience.
Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
17 July 2018 The foraging ecology of reintroduced African wild dog in small protected areas
John T. Vogel, Michael J. Somers, Jan A. Venter
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Restoring large predators to small confined areas (< 400 km2) is inherently complex and therefore any data on the foraging behaviour and top–down influences hold significant value for the conservation and reintroduction planning of the species. Conservation efforts are increasingly applied to small or fragmented landscapes. However, it is unclear what the effect of these small areas have on processes such as foraging behaviour as these spatial constraints may reduce the likelihood of innate predator–prey dynamics. We investigated African wild dog Lycaon pictus foraging patterns on five small fenced protected areas in South Africa. We report on the diet composition, prey preferences and potential influence of pack size and fences on the diet of African wild dogs. Data from 553 kills collected by direct observations at the five sample sites were analysed. Sixteen species of prey were recorded. A narrow dietary niche breadth was determined. Impala Aepycerosmelampus and nyala Tragelaphus angasii collectively, form 75% of diet, and 67% of edible biomass. However, only nyala were significantly selected for. The mean wild dog pack sizes in our sample sites were relatively smaller than those frequently encountered in larger systems. We found that larger wild dog pack sizes did not select for larger prey. Contrary to studies investigating the influence of hard boundaries on smaller protected areas, the upward bias caused by fences on prey mass selection was inconsistent across sample sites. By characterising African wild dog diet on smaller protected areas, our results are suggestive of potential top–down influences that should be investigated by future studies. The results add to a growing body of literature that aims to assist in the reintroduction planning of endangered carnivore species.

Protected areas are important conduits for the preservation of biodiversity (Le Saout et al. 2013, Watson et al. 2016). However, the ecological requirements of wide ranging species often extend beyond the periphery of the protected area (Jenkins et al. 2015), frequently leading to human–wildlife conflict within an increasingly human dominated landscape (Woodroffe and Ginsberg 1998, Balme et al. 2010). As competition for land intensifies, anthropogenic influences continue to reduce available habitat for wildlife species, and therefore conservation efforts are increasingly being applied to highly modified or small fragmented pockets of remaining habitat (Lindsey et al. 2011, Davies-Mostert et al. 2015, Miller et al. 2015).

Reintroductions are frequently used as a conservation tool to restore species to portions of their historical distribution range (Armstrong and Seddon 2008). Despite occurring throughout much of sub-Saharan Africa, the endangered African wild dog Lycaon pictus (wild dog hereafter) has experienced significant population size contractions on account of habitat fragmentation, conflict with human activities, and infectious diseases (Woodroffe and Sillero-Zubiri 2012). To halt population declines in South Africa, wild dog have been reintroduced into several small geographically discrete protected areas (Davies-Mostert et al. 2015), some of which are often considerably smaller than the estimated home range size (537 km2) in a large South African system (Mills and Gorman 1997).

Wildlife-proof fencing is commonly employed as a management tool to enclose these small protected areas (Somers and Hayward 2012, Packer et al. 2013). Although useful in separating large predators from humans, these hard boundaries are a potential catalyst to a range of cascading ecological challenges (Creel et al. 2013). Large carnivores often apply significant selective forces on prey assemblages as an explicit result of predation (Hayward et al. 2006). However, when confined to small areas, natural processes that frequently characterise open intact systems are disrupted (Gadd 2012, Murphy et al. 2017, Pokorny et al. 2017). For instance, fences that enclose small areas impede the potential for prey to carry out natural migratory patterns, as often observed in larger open systems (Whyte and Joubert 1988, Harris et al. 2009, Løvschal et al. 2017). The sedentary nature and small population sizes of the prey base in fenced areas inflate their vulnerability to predation (Fryxell and Sinclair 1988, Power 2003). As migratory prey populations are predominantly regulated by bottom–up processes (Mduma et al. 1999), the clumping of multi-prey multipredator assemblages in fenced protected areas can modify the trajectory of these associations (Mills and Shenk 1992, Tambling and Du Toit 2005). Predators in larger intact systems can directly and indirectly influence the diversity and stability within ecological communities (Estes et al. 2011, Ripple et al. 2014). Destabilisation can occur if the availability of refugia to escape lethal predatory encounters becomes spatially and temporally diluted, consequently increasing the influence of top–down processes (Tambling and Du Toit 2005, Robinson et al. 2010).

Wild dog are endangered, social, cursorial hunting carnivores (Creel and Creel 1995, Woodroffe and Sillero-Zubiri 2012). Small populations are disproportionally susceptible to demographic, environmental and genetic stochasticity (Shaffer 1981), therefore in South Africa, a managed metapopulation approach has been employed to collectively manage the species as a single population whereby dispersal and gene flow are mimicked through human intervention (Mills et al. 1998). The reintroduction of wild dog on small reserves in South Africa has become a frequent occurrence (Davies-Mostert et al. 2009), however several attempts have previously lead to unsustainable declines of prey species, subsequently leading to the removal of wild dog sub-populations from the protected area.

Understanding the foraging patterns of a predator in a small protected area (<400 km2) is the first step in unravelling the complex ecological community networks and potential influences on the prey base (Hayward et al. 2007, Kapfer et al. 2011). Whether predatory patterns on small reserves are similar to findings elsewhere in larger areas is uncertain (Hayward et al. 2006). Insight into the feeding ecology and predatory patterns of reintroduced predators to small fenced areas is essential in guiding evidence-based conservation management actions of the protected area, but also that of the focal species.

Large carnivores generally display a degree of selectivity towards a particular prey species, mass range or demographic class (Hayward et al. 2006, Clements et al. 2014, Makin and Kerley 2016). In large African predators, prey size typically increases with predator body size (Carbone et al. 1999), however wild dogs that weigh 20–25 kg (Creel and Creel 1995) employ communal hunting strategies that enable them to catch larger prey relative to their body size (Gorman et al. 1998). Cooperative hunting is suspected to reduce chase distance, increase kill rate and allow for the capture of larger prey (Rasmussen et al. 2008). Wild dog in small protected areas are further believed to modify their hunting behaviour in the presence of electric fences, by using barriers to subdue larger prey than what would otherwise be killed (van Dyk and Slotow 2003, Rhodes and Rhodes 2004).

Here, we quantify and describe the feeding patterns of reintroduced wild dog in five small range size protected areas in South Africa. We investigate the 1) prey species and biomass contributions to wild dog diet, 2) prey species sex and age contribution, 3) prey species preference, 4) the proximity to boundary fences on wild dog prey size usage, 5) diversity and evenness of diet, and 6) the influence of pack size on prey selection. We then discuss the implications of our results for the conservation management of the species, and predator–prey related processes.

Material and methods

Study area

Wild dog have been reintroduced to several small protected areas in KwaZulu-Natal including Somkhanda Game Reserve (Somkhanda: 106 km2), Zimanga Private Game Reserve (Zimanga: 53.3 km2), Manyoni Game Reserve (Manyoni: 217 km2), uMkuze Game Reserve (uMkhuze: 338 km2) and Thanda Royal Private Game Reserve (Thanda: 68.6 km2). The wild dog sub-population size at each protected area has fluctuated over time, however at the time of this study (May 2017), there are seven adult wild dog on Somkhanda Game Reserve, five at Manyoni Game Reserve, nine at uMkhuze Game Reserve, and 14 at Zimanga Game Reserve (Table 1, Fig. 1). Thanda currently does not host a pack of wild dog, as the 12 wild dogs were removed in 2013 due to the perceived impact on prey populations by the entire large predator guild.

The study areas are situated within the Maputaland Albany Hotspot and forms part of the African Savanna biome (Steenkamp et al. 2004, Mucina and Rutherford 2006). Two distinct seasons characterise the warm to hot, humid subtropical region; the warm dry winter from April to September and a hot humid summer from October to March (Balme et al. 2010). Eighty percent of the annual rainfall (580 mm; Mkuze Town weather station) occurs in the spring and summer months between September and March. Winters are generally cool to warm (van Rooyen and van Rooyen 2014). Frost is rare in the region, and the mean annual temperature is 21.8°C with the mean monthly temperature of 25.5°C and 16.4°C for January and July respectively (van Rooyen and van Rooyen 2014).

The protected areas support a rich diversity of ungulates and potential prey including blue wildebeest Connochaetes taurinus, common warthog Phacochoerus africanus, greater kudu Tragelaphus strepsiceros, impala Aepyceros melampus, nyala Tragelaphus angasii, plains zebra Equus quagga and waterbuck Kobus ellipsiprymnus. Due to the presence of potentially dangerous wildlife species, the protected areas are fully enclosed by electrified perimeter fencing (Chapman and Balme 2010).

Field sampling

Wild dog dietary data were collected from direct observations of prey killed. At least one individual in each pack was fitted with a VHF tracking device (frequency range 148–151 Mhz, from either African Wildlife Tracking (AWT cc-Pretoria, South Africa) or Sirtrack (Havelock North, New Zealand)). Wild dog were located and followed during periods that coincide with the bimodal crepuscular activity pattern (dawn and dusk) of wild dog (Creel and Creel 1996, Saleni et al. 2007, Davies-Mostert et al. 2013).

Table 1.

Protected area size (km2), mean annual precipitation (mm), density of sympatric predators (estimated number of individuals/100km2, u = unknown, a = absent), year of wild dog reintroduction, and focus study period in parenthesis for the five sample sites.

t01_01.gif

Once the pack was located, observations recorded include the geographic coordinates (latitude and longitude) of the pack using a GPS unit (Garmin), time of day, current behaviour and observed kills. Where possible, the prey item was identified and categorised to species, sex and age (adults > 2 years; subadults 1 to 2 years; juveniles 0 to 1 year).

Direct observation as a tool to obtain kill data is considered useful to determine the diets of large predators (Mills 1992). However, due to the quick-handling time, small prey species may be underrepresented (Okuyama 2010). It has been argued that where the observation effort is high, as in our case, the bias should be low (Radloff and Du Toit 2004, Rapson and Bernard 2007).

Prey availability

To determine prey availability, we used data from a number of sources. Aerial based total wildlife cens using was done at Somkhanda, Manyoni, Zimanga and Thanda using an R44 helicopter containing a crew of the same four people; pilot, recorder and two observers. Parallel predetermined transects were flown in an east–west orientation and were systematically arranged to cover the entire protected area. Census data for uMkhuze were collected using Distance based ground sampling (Thomas et al. 2010). All prey censuses were carried out at the end of the dry season (August, September) to aid in visually detecting an animal as the foliage is reduced compared to other seasons (Bothma 2002). Potential prey abundance estimates using aerial counts may be biased towards species that are more easily detected from the air (Owen-Smith and Mills 2008). Therefore standard visibility correction factors generated for savanna ecosystems (Owen-Smith and Mills 2008) were applied to compensate for these imprecisions.

Species and species biomass contribution to diet

The contribution of each prey species from direct observations to wild dog diet has been expressed as the prey percentage composition (g) of species i, as:

e01_01.gif

where ni represents the total prey species item and N is the total prey items of all species per protected area and year. Confidence intervals (95%) were generated by means of running 1000 bootstrap simulations around the mean relative frequency of a prey item.

To determine the overall biomass of prey killed, we estimated the mass of each kill based on the species sex and age. Adult mass for each species was obtained from Bothma (2002), and Skinner and Smithers (1990). Subadult mass was taken to be 75% of an adult female, and a juvenile as 30% (Radloff and Du Toit 2004). When the sex of an individual prey item could not be determined, the unit mass (adult female multiplied by 0.75) for the species was used (Jooste et al. 2013). To compensate for wastage, and inedible material, edible biomass (hereafter corrected mass) was determined as carcass weight > 80 kg = 67% is edible, carcass weight 40–80 kg = 75%, carcass weight 5–40 kg = 90%, carcass weight < 5 kg = 99% (Blumenschine and Caro 1986). The mean prey mass killed, prey mass range used and bootstrap confidence intervals (95%, 1000 iterations) were determined for each sample site.

Figure 1.

The location of the wild dog metapopulation sample reserves in KwaZulu-Natal Province, South Africa; Somkhanda Game Reserve (1), Zimanga Private Game Reserve (2), Manyoni Game Reserve (3), Thanda Royal Private Game Reserve (4) and uMkhuze Game Reserve (5).

f01_01.jpg

Dietary niche breadth and evenness

To determine the relative degree of diet specialisation, we determined the dietary niche breadth using Levins' index (Levins 1968) for each wild dog pack and year. We applied standardised trophic niche measures following Hurlbert (1978):

e02_01.gif

where BA is the standardised index of niche breadth, pij is the proportion of diet of pack i on prey j, n is the number of possible resource states. Index values close to ‘0’ represent specialised dietary niches, and values close to ‘1’ represent generalist foraging patterns (Krebs 1999). The mean standardised dietary niche index value determined (BA ± 1 SE) from prey species composition is presented.

We further determined diet evenness by constructing Shannon (H) and Shannon equitability diversity (Eh) index values as:

e03_01.gif

Where p is the proportional diet contribution of prey species i, ln is the natural log transformation and s represents the species richness found in the respective diet. Diet evenness is derived from H using a Shannon equitability diversity index (Eh). Eh is determined as H / Hmax (here Hmax=ln s). Index values range from Eh = 0 (uneven) to Eh = 1 (complete evenness).

Selection for prey species

In order to determine the selectivity for specific prey items, a preference ratio analysis was employed by using Ivlevs electivity index (D) as modified by Jacobs (1974). Various selection indices have been developed to test selection, however many show bias towards rarer items and suffer from non-linearity (Jacobs 1974). The use of the Jacobs' index is expected to reduce these biases (Hayward and Kerley 2005) and in addition allows for comparison with other studies investigating large predator prey preferences (Hayward et al. 2006) as follows:

e04_01.gif

where r is the proportion of kills of each species and p is the proportional availability of that prey species in comparison to the number of species preyed on by wild dogs during the study period. The availability of prey is based on game census data for each respective year at each study site. The D value of the Jacobs' index ranges from -1 to + 1, with a +1 indicating maximum preference and a–1 value indicating maximum avoidance (Jacobs 1974). Jacobs’ index values 0.2 > 0 > -0.2 are considered to be indicative of prey being used as frequently as expected (Hayward et al. 2011). Each prey species Jacobs' index was tested for significant avoidance/preference by using a t-test against a hypothetical mean on ‘0’. Where data did not conform to normality (Shapiro–Wilks test) we used a z-sign test. The analysis of prey preference further identifies prey species utilised proportional to their density, therefore provides an estimate of prey utilisation relative to prey abundance.

Selection for prey age and sex classes

We quantified the known sex and age (juvenile, subadult, and adult) prey composition of wild dog kills as the percentage of observed kills. To determine the utilisation of certain prey population classes by wild dog, we used nyala as a representative species. As nyala are non-seasonal breeders and are sexually dimorphic, the known sex ratio and age distributions from road based distance sampling were compared to observed kills. A Jacobs' selection index and a goodness-of-fit test were used by comparing the frequency of predation between male and female nyala and also for adult, subadult and juveniles. Bonferonni Z- statistics were used to generate 95% confidence intervals.

The influence of pack size on prey size

We used Kendalls tau-b correlation coefficient (τb) of independence to determine whether a monotonic relationship exists between pack size and prey mass. As wild dog subadults often contribute to hunting efforts (Vogel et al. unpubl.), we considered pack sizes as 1) adult pack size, 2) adult + subadults and 3) adult + an applied subadult weighted unit equivalent of 0.5. Wild dog are estimated to reach sexual maturity at approximately 18 months of age (Becker et al. 2012), and often disperse from natal packs at this age (Gusset et al. 2006). We therefore categorised subadult pack members as 12 to 18 months, and adults as >18 months (Malcolm and Marten 1982).

We repeated this procedure for the mean mass of prey killed for each pack size category and for the most frequently preyed upon prey item (based on prey species, sex and age) for each pack size category. Where two species equally occurred as the most frequently used prey item, the mean mass between the species was used.

The influence of fences in prey mass selection

As wild dog may alter their hunting strategies on small reserves by incorporating fences to capture larger prey items (van Dyk and Slotow 2003), we compared the mass and proportional contribution of fence impeded kills with non-fence impeded kills. If fences increase the vulnerability of larger prey to wild dog predation than what naturally would occur, the median mass of prey items (based on prey species, age and sex) will be skewed. Using wild dog kill site GPS coordinates, we binomially categorised kills by creating a 200m buffer tolerance from fences. We made the assumption that the benefit of using a fence for hunting purposes would be most rewarding at the capture (restraining and bringing down), see Bailey et al. (2013), phase of the hunt. Allowing for temporary getaways of the prey during restraining. The 200 m buffer was an estimate from observing hunts over a four year field study (Somers unpubl.). We compared the size mass of kills within the 200 m buffer to size mass of kills >200 m from fences for four protected areas (Somkhanda, Manyoni, uMkuze, Zimanga). GPS data associated with kills made by wild dog at Thanda were however not available, therefore we report on kills made with GPS coordinates at the remaining four sample sites. The upward bias caused by barriers was tested using a Mann–Whitney U-test and p-values were computed using 10 000 Monte Carlo simulations. As wild dog are generally cursorial hunting carnivores, we believe that 200 m from a boundary is sufficient to determine prey mass differences and further reduces the influence of GPS error and detection biases.

Table 2.

Comparison between the five protected areas, n is total count of all kills observed, S is the species richness (count of prey species) in diet, mean prey (kg) and lower and upper 0.05 confidence levels (Cl). Range represents the smallest and largest prey items killed.

t02_01.gif

Results

Sixteen prey species (Table 2) were recorded by direct observations (n = 553) over a cumulative time period of 91 months. Impala (40% ± 3.92) followed by nyala (35% ± 6.03) formed the greatest frequency of known kills (Table 3). However, if transformed to corrected biomass, nyala forms the greatest edible biomass (8245.81 kg) followed by impala (6742.8 kg) of the total 22254.41 kg (Supplementary material Appendix Table A1).

Dietary niche breadth and evenness

Wild dog foraging patterns indicated specialised niche breadths for pooled observations (BA = 0.17, n = 16) and across sample sites (BAmean = 0.3 ± 0.03). Wild dog further showed low diversity in diet (H = 1.68, Hmean = 1.36 ± 0.1), and erred (0.50 <) on uniformity (Eh = 0.61, Ehmean = 0.67 ± 0.02) in terms of diet evenness.

Prey preference

Jacobs' index values were calculated for sixteen species (Table 3). Wild dog within the KwaZulu-Natal cluster prefer (D > 0.2) to consume common duiker (z = 0.92, p = 0.359), and red duiker (z = 0.97, p = 0.333). However, only nyala (t7 = 6.37, p = <0.001) are significantly preferred. Buffalo (z = -2.79, p = 0.005), plains zebra (z = -8.35, p = <0.001), warthog (z = -4.36, p = <0.001), waterbuck (z = 2.46, p = 0.014) and blue wildebeest (t8 = -5.01, p = 0.002) are significantly avoided prey species. Giraffe Giraffa giraffa and suni Neotragus moschatus (D = -1) were completely avoided.

Table 3.

Mean annual wild dog prey species contribution (g) and mean prey species Jacobs' index values (D ± SE). Significant avoidance, preference or whether a species is utilised in proportion to its abundance is indicated.

t03_01.gif

Prey demographic selection

Female ungulate prey (66 ± 7.02%) occurred more frequently than male ungulates (34 ± 7.02%) in the diet of wild dog. Adult prey items were recorded more frequently (69 ± 6.92%) than subadult (21 ± 5.52%) and juvenile (9 ± 41%) age categories.

Male and female nyala were utilised disproportionally (χ2 (1) = 6.71, p = 0.01) to their availability. Female nyala are killed more frequently by wild dog than expected, in contrast to male nyala prey items that were actively avoided (D = -0.23).

Adult and juvenile nyala are utilised as expected (Table 4), however subadults are preyed upon more frequently than their proportional availability (χ2 (2) = 13.638, p = 0.001).

Influence of pack size on diet

In context of observed kills made by wild dog, adult pack size ranged from two to seven adults with a mean of 4.6 ± 0.08 individuals. If subadults are considered as hunting pack members, pack sizes ranged from two to 13 equivalents (6.19 ± 0.11), and two to eight if weighted as 0.5 equivalents (5.39 ± 0.07).

While pack sizes in our study were relatively small in comparison to those encountered elsewhere (Creel et al. 2004, Marnewick et al. 2014), we found no evidence to suggest that larger packs selected for larger prey. There was no significant association between mean mass of prey killed and adult pack size (τb = 0.2, p = 0.719), adult and equivalent subadult pack size (τb = -0.333, p = 0.216) or adult and weighted subadult pack size (τb = -0.200, p = 0.445). Using the most frequently preyed item, there was no significant relationship with adult pack size (τb = 0.447, p = 0.227), adult pack size including subadults as an equivalent weighting (τb = -0.511, p = 0.054) or adult and weighted subadult pack size (τb = -0.407, p = 0.099).

Table 4.

Wild dog prey selection of nyala age structure, observed age utilisation (pi), expected age utilisation (pioxn) and preference (JI=Jacobs' index), Z=2,37.

t04_01.gif

The influence of fences on prey mass selection

A total of 422 kills with associated GPS coordinates were observed at the four sample sites. Wild dog kills located within 200m proximity of fence lines contributed 50% of all recorded kill locations. Wild dog at each sample site used prey mass in a similar pattern (H = 5.963, p = 0.113). The median mass for prey killed within 200 m of a fence (60 kg, 60 kg, 45 kg) was not significantly larger than prey killed away from fences (45 kg, 53.25 kg, 46.5 kg) for Manyoni (U = 1988.5, p = 0.279), Somkhanda (U = 2965.5, p = 0.076) and uMkhuze (U = 131, p = 0.961) respectively. Prey killed <200 m from the fence were only significantly larger in mass at a single site, Zimanga Game Reserve (U = 238.5, p = 0.007). Prey mass difference of kills at Somkhanda were insignificant (p = 0.076), however with a less conservative alpha level (e.g. 0.10) half of our sample sites with spatial data on kill distribution would have had significantly larger prey killed within 200 m of fences.

Discussion

Our investigation into the foraging behaviour of reintroduced wild dog to five small protected areas concurs that wild dog prey on the most abundant small and medium sized ungulates (Ginsberg and Macdonald 1990, Hayward et al. 2006). Nyala and impala collectively compromise 75% of the mean species preyed upon. These two-prey species featured as the most frequently observed prey item, with the exception of the smallest protected area (Zimanga), where impala and warthog were observed to be utilised far more frequently in comparison. Warthog are generally avoided by wild dog, however the preferred larger nyala occur at much lower densities at this particular site likely leading to comparatively fewer encounters with wild dog. Wild dog have disproportionally high rates of daily energetic expenditure (Gorman et al. 1998), and require high daily food consumption rates (Gorman et al. 1998, Carbone et al. 2007), but see Hubel et al. (2016) suggesting that wild dog are more energetically robust that previously thought. If wild dog optimise their foraging strategy, the more abundant warthog (corrected sow biomass 45 kg) are expected to contribute more frequently to their diet, as they would be required to concede between searching for the rarer nyala, albeit preferred (corrected ewe biomass 46.5 kg) versus selecting for the more abundant warthog in the Zimanga case, as this requires the least energy to secure through search and handling time.

Very large prey (>350 kg e.g. giraffe) were avoided across all sites. Small prey items (<5 kg) do occur in their diet, however not frequently and can be considered as rare and incidental prey items. The detectability of very small prey items using direct and opportunistic methods to quantify large predator diets have previously been questioned, as small prey items are rapidly or entirely consumed. As our times of sampling mirror that of wild dog hunting periods, these continuous observations during activity phases are likely to reliably reflect on real dietary patterns (Rasmussen et al. 2008). The viability of alternative methods such as scat analysis is further compounded particularly in context of distinguishing between prey age and sex classes (Davies-Mostert et al. 2010). In addition, wild dog in our sample sites, did scavenge on prey that had died by other means than wild dog predation and occasionally returned to kills the following days. Although we did not quantify scavenging events (feeding on prey remains known to not have been killed by wild dog) or kill return intervals (feeding on prey killed by focal pack, but defined by moving away from kill and returning at a later stage), wild dog have previously been observed to scavenge on prey remains killed by other predators (Fitzgibbon and Fanshawe 1989). Therefore using scat analysis would increase the percent occurrence and estimated percent biomass of the scavenged species (Morehouse and Boyce 2011). Returning to kills may be an artefact of limited exposure to kleptoparistism as a result of low densities of spotted hyaena Crocuta crocuta. Interference competition inflicted by spotted hyaena may result in considerable loss (up to 22%) of prey killed by wild dog through kleptoparistism (Fuller et al. 1995). Considering the costly expense of hunting to attain energy balances together with direct antagonistic interactions with larger carnivores, can explain the low population densities of wild dog where competition is high (Creel and Creel 1996, Gorman et al. 1998, Woodroffe and Ginsberg 1998). If the intensity of competition within the large predator guild increases within our sample sites, we envisage that the frequency of wild dog returning to kills and scavenging will decrease.

Using nyala as a frequently preyed upon proxy, it is expected that vulnerability will vary among prey sex and age classes (Pole et al. 2004). Nyala females and subadults were utilised more frequently than expected. Analogous to studies carried out in open systems (Fitzgibbon and Fanshawe 1989), the proportional partiality towards a particular prey or sex class may be a biological innate phenomenon. The avoidance of larger male nyala may be an attribute of sexual dimorphism, where males have horns that can be used for weaponry defence against potential lethal encounters with predators. The selective hunting of female and subadult prey may therefore be more profitable in terms of avoiding injury as they do not possess horns.

A narrow dietary niche, as observed with wild dog in our study, has been suggested to contribute to the inherent rarity of the species (Hayward and Kerley 2008). Dietary niche estimates are often used in the process of evaluating the potential competition amongst sympatric carnivores (Mbizah et al. 2012, Broekhuis et al. 2018). However, in our study, wild dog consistently indicated a niche specialisation across all five sample sites, despite the variability in the presence and density of competing sympatric carnivores. Wild dogs are expected to discriminate between prey items based on functional traits of prey such as defensive weaponry, size, body condition, herd structure and habitat affinity (Pole et al. 2004, Hayward et al. 2006, Clements et al. 2016). In support of this, our results from preference indices show that wild dog did not use species at random and showed a high proportional utilisation of nyala. When compared to wild dog prey preference estimates carried out elsewhere (Hayward et al. 2006), nyala was highly avoided. Nyala are nested within the preferred prey weight range of wild dog (Clements et al. 2014) Furthermore, nyala occur at greater densities and overall ungulate composition within our sample sites (16.8 ± 3.26) in comparison to other regions of wild dog distribution (6.47 ± 4.41%, Hayward et al. 2006). This confounding high variability for prey resources highlights the measure of caution when interpreting preference analysis. Values are non-independent, in that the increase in preference for species ‘A’ conversely influences prey species ’B’ (Louw et al. 2012). Determining foraging preferences is further compounded by being primarily methodologically contingent on the known proportional prey species kills vs the species availability to the predator (Jacobs 1974). The proportional availability of a prey item is required to be known, however game census data are often subject to wide confidence intervals, particularly those for cryptic or thicket dwelling species, leading to unreliable estimates of prey species proportional availability.

The higher proportional contribution of larger bodied prey animals to diet profiles in other smaller protected areas has been described as a consequence of fence aided hunting (van Dyk and Slotow 2003, Rhodes and Rhodes 2004, Davies-Mostert et al. 2013). Except for the smallest protected area (53 km2), wild dog in Kwazulu-Natal did not provide sufficient behavioural evidence to support this assumption. A hefty proportion of kills occur within 200 m of artificial boundaries, although the discrepancy in prey mass utilisation facilitated by fences is negligible.

The mean wild dog pack sizes in larger areas such as northern Botswana and the Selous Game Reserve are estimated at 10.4 and 8.9 individuals per pack respectively (Creel et al. 2004). Our largest pack, including subadults included up to 13 individuals with a mean of 6.19. The pack sizes in our sample sites were smaller than those encountered elsewhere. Despite the comparatively smaller pack sizes encountered, our larger wild dog packs were expected to select for larger prey items (Creel and Creel 1995). Wild dog in our core sample sites illustrated a dissociated relationship between pack size and prey mass. An increase in sample size and a greater spectrum in pack size may increase or decrease even further the strength of these relationships.

The differences in attitudes towards African wild dogs between conservation practitioners and the general public can potentially hinder conservation actions and efforts as these values and attitudes play a major role in acceptance of conservation activities (Karanth et al. 2008). Perceptions hold that wild dog deplete the availability of prey, particularly during the denning season, when wild dog are generally more sedentary. Mbizah et al. (2014) found that wild dog did not significantly reduce impala populations, and wild dog actually select for low prey density areas. Selecting for low prey density areas is likely to be an artefact of avoiding costly encounters with larger competitors (Mills and Gorman 1997). However this could potentially contribute to the misconception that wild dog deplete prey stocks (Mbizah et al. 2014). Although we did not test for the impact of wild dog predation on prey populations, we did not find sufficient evidence to suggest that wild dog adversely influenced prey populations sizes. Despite receiving below average regional rainfall for the latter period of this study, principal dietary prey species such as impala remained stable or increased at three of the five sample sites. At the remaining two sites, the uMkhuze impala population size were estimated at 10 937 individuals and dropped to 5076 individuals two years later in 2016. The estimate of 10 937 impala is the highest the population has been recorded in the last 20 years. Considering that wild dog have been present for the last 11 years (initial reintroduction in 2005), impala populations returned to population densities previously estimated. Impala population size at Zimanga Game Reserve was reduced from an estimated 1337 individuals to 482 individuals a year later in 2016. Management at this particular site removed substantial amounts of wildlife from the protected area during this period (exact head of game not given). However, in combination with prey removal by management, the influence of wild dog on prey species is therefore unknown.

The undesirable influences induced by fences as found in other studies (Davies-Mostert et al. 2013), were not as evident in our study. Our smallest reserve and to a lesser extent Somkhanda, displayed evidence of fencing significantly influencing prey mass selection. The proximity to these hard barriers largely influenced the proportional contribution of prey kills with more kills occurring within the vicinity of fences than expected. The affinity towards ‘fence patrolling’ may facilitate the capture of prey, however it may also be a reflection of spatial avoidance of larger competitors.

The use of fencing to enclose small protected areas in South Africa is useful to minimize negative interactions between wildlife and humans. Protected areas interested in reintroducing large carnivores such as wild dog should ensure that sufficient principal prey items are present to sustain their consumptive influence. Where fencing has adversely manipulated predator–prey interactions, mechanisms such as area expansion should be investigated to decrease perimeter fencing to area size (Davies-Mostert et al. 2013). This study provides evidence that wild dog in small protected areas can display a narrow dietary niche and can select specific prey for consumption. Given the prey and foraging requirements of wild dog, harbouring populations of large carnivores in small fenced protected areas is integrally complex and will likely continue to require intensive management approaches. Wild dog foraging behaviour in small protected areas we studied reflects that of larger open systems, and fences in our sample sites showed only marginal evidence of management induced niche shifts or evolutionary constraints imposed by fragmentation.

Acknowledgemeats

We thank Wildlife Act, iSimangaliso Wetland Park Authority, Ezemvelo KZN Wildlife, Thanda Royal Private Game Reserve, Manyoni Game Reserve, Somkhanda Game Reserve and Zimanga Private Game Reserve for providing data and permission to use them in this paper. We are grateful to Bogdan Cristescu for providing invaluable comments on the initial manuscript.

Permits – This research was carried out with the approval of the provincial conservation authority (Ezemvelo KZN Wildlife, registration no. E/1079/02). Ethics approval was granted by the Nelson Mandela University Animal Research Ethics Committee (registration no. A17-SCI-SNRM-002).

References

1.

Armstrong, D. P. and Seddon, P. J. 2008. Directions in reintroduction biology. — Trends Ecol. Evol. 23: 20–25. Google Scholar

2.

Bailey, I. et al. 2013. Group hunting within the Carnivora: physiological, cognitive and environmental influences on strategy and cooperation. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 67: 1–17. Google Scholar

3.

Balme, G. A. et al. 2010. Edge effects and the impact of non-protected areas in carnivore conservation: leopards in the Phinda-Mkhuze Complex, South Africa. — Anim. Conserv. 13: 315–323. Google Scholar

4.

Becker, P. A. et al. 2012. Inbreeding avoidance influences the viability of reintroduced populations of African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus). — PLoS One 7: e37181. Google Scholar

5.

Blumenschine, R. J. and Caro, T. M. 1986. Unit flesh weights of some East African bovids. — Afr. J. Ecol. 24: 273–286. Google Scholar

6.

Bothma, J. du P. 2002. Game ranch management. — Van Schaik Publishers. Google Scholar

7.

Broekhuis, F. et al. 2018. Feeding ecology of cheetahs in the Maasai Mara, Kenya and the potential for intra- and interspecific competition. — J. Zool. 304: 65–72. Google Scholar

8.

Carbone, C. et al. 1999. Energetic constraints on the diet of terrestrial carnivores. — Nature 402: 286–288. Google Scholar

9.

Carbone, C. et al. 2007. The costs of carnivory. — PLoS Biol. 5: 363–368. Google Scholar

10.

Chapman, S. and Balme, G. 2010. An estimate of leopard population density in a private reserve in KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa, using camera-traps and capture—recapture models. — S. Afr. J. Wildl. Res. 40: 114–120. Google Scholar

11.

Clements, H. S. et al. 2014. An objective approach to determining the weight ranges of prey preferred by and accessible to the five large African carnivores. — PLoS One 9(7): e101054. Google Scholar

12.

Clements, H. S. et al. 2016. Prey morphology and predator sociality drive predator prey preferences. — J. Mammal. 97: 919–927. Google Scholar

13.

Creel, S. and Creel, N. M. 1995. Communal hunting and pack size in African wild dogs, Lycaon pictus. — Anim. Behav. 50: 1325–1339. Google Scholar

14.

Creel, S. and Creel, N. M. 1996. Limitation of African wild dogs by competition limitation with larger carnivores. — Conserv. Biol. 10: 526–538. Google Scholar

15.

Creel, S. et al. 2004. Demography and population dynamics of African wild dogs in three critical populations. — In: Macdonald, D. W. and Sillero-Zubiri, C. (eds), The biology and conservation of wild canids. Oxford Univ. Press, pp. 337–350. Google Scholar

16.

Creel, S. et al. 2013. Conserving large populations of lions — the argument for fences has holes. — Ecol. Lett. 16: 1413–e3. Google Scholar

17.

Davies-Mostert, H. T. et al. 2009. A critical assessment of south africas managed metapopulation recovery strategy for African wild dogs. — In: Reintroduction of top-order predators. Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 10–42. Google Scholar

18.

Davies-Mostert, H. T. et al. 2010. Reducing potential sources of sampling bias when quantifying the diet of the African wild dog through scat analysis. — S. Afr. J. Wildl. Res. 40: 105–113. Google Scholar

19.

Davies-Mostert, H. T. et al. 2013. Hard boundaries influence African wild dogs' diet and prey selection. — J. Appl. Ecol. 50: 1358–1366. Google Scholar

20.

Davies-Mostert, H. T. et al. 2015. The demography and dynamics of an expanding, managed African wild dog metapopulation. — Afr. J. Wildl. Res. 45: 258–273. Google Scholar

21.

Estes, J. A. et al. 2011. Trophic downgrading of planet Earth. — Science 333: 301–306. Google Scholar

22.

Fitzgibbon, C. D. and Fanshawe, J. H. 1989. The condition and age of Thomsons gazelles killed by cheetahs and wild dogs. — J. Zool. 218: 99–107. Google Scholar

23.

Fryxell, J. M. and Sinclair, A. R. E. 1988. Causes and consequences of migration by large herbivores. — Trends Ecol. Evol. 3: 237–241. Google Scholar

24.

Fuller, T. K. et al. 1995. Prey and estimated food consumption of African wild dogs in Kenya. — S. Afr. J. Wildl. Res. 25: 106–110. Google Scholar

25.

Gadd, M. E. 2012. Barriers, the beef industry and unnatural selection: a review of the impact of veterinary fencing on mammals in southern Africa. — In: Somers, M. J. and Hayward, M. W. (eds), Fencing for conservation. Springer, pp. 153–186. Google Scholar

26.

Ginsberg, J. R. and Macdonald, D. W. 1990. Foxes, wolves, jackals, and dogs: an action plan for the conservation of canids. — IUCN. Google Scholar

27.

Gorman, M. L. et al. 1998. High hunting costs make African wild dogs vulnerable to kleptoparasitism by hyaenas. — Nature 391: 479–481. Google Scholar

28.

Gusset, M. et al. 2006. Divided we fail: the importance of social integration for the re-introduction of endangered African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus). — J. Zool. 270: 502–511. Google Scholar

29.

Harris, G. et al. 2009. Global decline in aggregated migrations of large terrestrial mammals. — Endanger. Species Res. 7: 55–76. Google Scholar

30.

Hayward, M. W. and Kerley, G. I. H. 2005. Prey preferences of the lion (Panthera leo). — J. Zool. 267: 309–322. Google Scholar

31.

Hayward, M. W. and Kerley, G. I. H. 2008. Prey preferences and dietary overlap amongst Africa's large predators. — S. Afr. J. Wildl. Res. 38: 93–108. Google Scholar

32.

Hayward, M. W. et al. 2006. Prey preferences of the African wild dog Lycaon pictus (Canidae: Carnivora): ecological requirements for conservation. — J. Mammal. 87: 1122–1131. Google Scholar

33.

Hayward, M. W. et al. 2007. Carrying capacity of large African predators: predictions and tests. — Biol. Conserv. 139: 219–229. Google Scholar

34.

Hayward, M. W. et al. 2011. Do lions Panthera leo actively select prey or do prey preferences simply reflect chance responses via evolutionary adaptations to optimal foraging? — PLoS One 6 (9): e23607. Google Scholar

35.

Hubel, T. Y. et al. 2016. Energy cost and return for hunting in African wild dogs and cheetahs. — Nat. Commun. 7: 11034. Google Scholar

36.

Hurlbert, S. H. 1978. The measurement of niche overlap and some relatives. — Ecology 59: 67–77. Google Scholar

37.

Jacobs, J. 1974. Quantitative measurement of food selection. — Oecologia 14: 413–417. Google Scholar

38.

Jenkins, E. et al. 2015. Home range and habitat selection of a reintroduced African wild dog (Lycaon pictus) pack in a small South African game reserve. — Afr. J. Wildl. Res. 45: 233–246. Google Scholar

39.

Jooste, E. et al. 2013. Effect of prey mass and selection on predator carrying capacity estimates. — Eur. J. Wildl. Res. 59: 487–494. Google Scholar

40.

Kapfer, P. M. et al. 2011. Fine-scale spatio-temporal variation in tiger Panthera tigris diet: effect of study duration and extent on estimates of tiger diet in Chitwan National Park, Nepal. — Wildlife Biol. 17: 277–285. Google Scholar

41.

Karanth, K. K. et al. 2008. Examining conservation attitudes, perspectives, and challenges in India. — Biol. Conserv. 141: 2357–2367. Google Scholar

42.

Krebs, C. J. 1999. Ecological methodology. — Addison Welsey Educational Publ. Google Scholar

43.

Le Saout, S. et al. 2013. Protected areas and effective biodiversity conservation. — Science (6160). 342: 803–805. Google Scholar

44.

Levins, R. 1968. Evolution in changing environments: some theoretical explorations. — Princeton Univ. Press. Google Scholar

45.

Lindsey, P. A. et al. 2011. Minimum prey and area requirements of the Vulnerable cheetah Acinonyx jubatus: implications for reintroduction and management of the species in South Africa. — Oryx 45: 587–599. Google Scholar

46.

Louw, J. et al. 2012. The applicability of lion prey selection models to smaller game reserves in South Africa. — S. A. J. Wildl. Res. 42: 73–81. Google Scholar

47.

Løvschal, M. et al. 2017. Fencing bodes a rapid collapse of the unique Greater Mara ecosystem. — Sci. Rep. 7(41450): 1–7. Google Scholar

48.

Makin, D. F. and Kerley, G. I. H. 2016. Selective predation and prey class behaviour as possible mechanisms explaining cheetah impacts on kudu demographics. — Afr. Zool. 51: 217–219. Google Scholar

49.

Malcolm, J. R. and Marten, K. 1982. Natural selection and the communal rearing of pups in African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus). — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 10: 1–13. Google Scholar

50.

Marnewick, K. et al. 2014. Evaluating the status of and African wild dogs Lycaon pictus and cheetahs Acinonyx jubatus through tourist-based photographic surveys in the Kruger National Park. — PLoS One 9(1):e86265. Google Scholar

51.

Mbizah, M. M. et al. 2012. Diet of four sympatric carnivores in Save Valley Conservancy, Zimbabwe: implications for conservation of the African wild dog (Lycaon pictus). — S. Afr. J. Wildl. Res. 42: 94–103. Google Scholar

52.

Mbizah, M. M. et al. 2014. Implications of African wild dog (Lycaon pictus) denning on the density and distribution of a key prey species: addressing myths and misperceptions. — Biodivers. Conserv. 23: 1441–1451. Google Scholar

53.

Mduma, S. A. R. et al. 1999. Food regulates the Serengeti wildebeest: a 40-year record. — J. Anim. Ecol. 68: 1101–1122. Google Scholar

54.

Miller, S. M. et al. 2015. Fenced and fragmented: conservation value of managed metapopulations. — PLoS One 10(12): e0144605. Google Scholar

55.

Mills, M. G. L. 1992. A comparison of methods used to study food habits of large African carnivores. — In: McCullough, D. R. and Barrett, R. H. (eds), Wildlife 2001 : populations. Springer, pp. 1112–1124. Google Scholar

56.

Mills, M. G. L. and Shenk, T. M. 1992. Predator—prey relationships: the impact of lion predation on wildebeest and zebra populations. — J. Anim. Ecol. 61: 693–702. Google Scholar

57.

Mills, M. G. L. and Gorman, M. L. 1997. Factors affecting the density and distribution of wild dogs in the Kruger National Park. — Conserv. Biol. 11: 1397–1406. Google Scholar

58.

Mills, M. G. L. et al. 1998. Population and habitat viability assessment African wild dog (Lycaon pictus) in southern Africa. — Unpublished IUCN/SSC Conservation Breeding Specialist Group Final Workshop Report. Google Scholar

59.

Morehouse, A. T. and Boyce, M. S. 2011. From venison to beef: seasonal changes in wolf diet composition in a livestock grazing landscape. — Front. Ecol. Environ. 9: 440–445. Google Scholar

60.

Mucina, L. and Rutherford, M. C. 2006. The vegetation of South Africa, Lesotho and Swaziland. — In: The vegetation of South Africa, Lesotho and Sawziland. — South African National Biodiversity Inst., pp. 749–790. Google Scholar

61.

Murphy, S. M. et al. 2017. Consequences of severe habitat fragmentation on density, genetics, and spatial capture—recapture analysis of a small bear population. — PLoS One 12(7): e0181849. Google Scholar

62.

Okuyama, T. 2010. Prey density-dependent handling time in a predator—prey model. — Commun. Ecol. 11: 91–96. Google Scholar

63.

Owen-Smith, N. and Mills, M. G. L. 2008. Predator—prey size relationships in an African large-mammal food web. — J. Anim. Ecol. 77: 173–183. Google Scholar

64.

Packer, C. et al. 2013. Conserving large carnivores: dollars and fence. — Ecol. Lett. 16: 635–641. Google Scholar

65.

Pokorny, B. et al. 2017. Border fence: a new ecological obstacle for wildlife in southeast Europe. — Eur. J. Wildl. Res. 63: 1. Google Scholar

66.

Pole, A. et al. 2004. Prey selection by African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus) in southern Zimbabwe. — J. Zool. 262: 207–215. Google Scholar

67.

Power, R. J. 2003. Evaluating how many lions a small reserve can sustain. — S. Afr. J. Wildl. Res. 33: 3–11. Google Scholar

68.

Radloff, F. G. T. and Du Toit, J. T. 2004. Large predators and their prey in a southern African savanna: a predators size determines its prey size range. — J. Anim. Ecol. 73: 410–423. Google Scholar

69.

Rapson, J. A. and Bernard, R. T. F. 2007. Interpreting the diet of lions (Panthera leo); a comparison of various methods of analysis. — S. Afr. J. Wildl. Res. 37: 179–187. Google Scholar

70.

Rasmussen, G. S. A. et al. 2008. Achilles' heel of sociality revealed by energetic poverty trap in cursorial hunters. — Am. Nat. 172: 508–518. Google Scholar

71.

Rhodes, R. and Rhodes, G. 2004. Prey selection and use of natural and man-made barriers by African wild dogs while hunting. — S. Afr. J. Wildl. Res. 34: 135–142. Google Scholar

72.

Ripple, W. J. et al. 2014. Status and ecological effects of the Worlds largest carnivores. — Science 343: 1241484. Google Scholar

73.

Robinson, B. G. et al. 2010. Are migrant and resident elk (Cervus elaphus) exposed to similar forage and predation risk on their sympatric winter range? — Oecologia 164: 265–275. Google Scholar

74.

Saleni, P. et al. 2007. Refuges in time: temporal avoidance of interference competition in endangered wild dogs Lycaon pictus. — Canid News 10: 1–5. Google Scholar

75.

Shaffer, M. L. 1981. Minimum for species population sizes conservation. — Bioscience 31: 131–134. Google Scholar

76.

Skinner, J. D. and Smithers, R. 1990. The mammals of the southern Africa subregion. — Pretoria Univ. Press. Google Scholar

77.

Somers, M. J. and Hayward, M. W. 2012. Fencing for conservation: restriction of evolutionary potential or a riposte to threatening processes? — Springer. Google Scholar

78.

Steenkamp, Y. et al. 2004. Maputaland-Pondoland-Albany. — In: Mittermeier, R. A. et al. (eds), Hotspots revisited: Earths biologically richest and most endangered ecoregions. Monterrey: CEMEX, pp. 219–228. Google Scholar

79.

Tambling, C. J. and Du Toit, J. T. 2005. Modelling wildebeest population dynamics: implications of predation and harvesting in a closed system. — J. Appl. Ecol. 42: 431–441. Google Scholar

80.

Thomas, L. et al. 2010. Distance software: design and analysis of distance sampling surveys for estimating population size. — J. Appl. Ecol. 47: 5–14. Google Scholar

81.

van Dyk, G. and Slotow, R. 2003. The effects of fences and lions on the ecology of African wild dogs reintroduced to Pilanesberg National Park, South Africa. — Afr. Zool. 38: 79–94. Google Scholar

82.

van Rooyen, N. and van Rooyen, G. 2014. Vegetation monitoring of the herbaceous layer in Thanda and Mduna. — Unpubl. Report. Google Scholar

83.

Watson, J. E. M. et al. 2016. Bolder science needed now for protected areas. — Conserv. Biol. 30: 243–248. Google Scholar

84.

Whyte, I. J. and Joubert, S. C. J. 1988. Blue wildebeest population trends in the Kruger National Park and the effects of fencing. — S. Afr. J. Wildl. Res. 18: 78–87. Google Scholar

85.

Woodroffe, R. and Ginsberg, J. R. 1998. Edge effects and the extinction of populations inside protected areas. — Science 280: 2126–2128. Google Scholar

86.

Woodroffe, R. and Sillero-Zubiri, C. 2012. African wild dog (Lycaon pictus).— The IUCN Red List of threatened species. 8235. Google Scholar

Appendices

Supplementary material (available online as Appendix wlb-00424 at < www.wildlifebiology.org/appendix/wlb-00424>). Appendix 1.

© 2018 The Authors. This is an Open Access article This work is licensed under the terms of a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (CC-BY). The license permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
John T. Vogel, Michael J. Somers, and Jan A. Venter "The foraging ecology of reintroduced African wild dog in small protected areas," Wildlife Biology 2018(1), (17 July 2018). https://doi.org/10.2981/wlb.00424
Accepted: 16 May 2018; Published: 17 July 2018
Back to Top