Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
10 June 2022 Timeless Plays an Important Role in Compound Eye-Dependent Photic Entrainment of the Circadian Rhythm in the Cricket Gryllus bimaculatus
Yoshiyuki Moriyama, Kazuki Takeuchi, Tsugumichi Shinohara, Koichi Miyagawa, Mirai Matsuka, Taishi Yoshii, Kenji Tomioka
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

The light cycle is the most powerful Zeitgeber entraining the circadian clock in most organisms. Insects use CRYPTOCHROMEs (CRYs) and/or the compound eye for the light perception necessary for photic entrainment. The molecular mechanism underlying CRY-dependent entrainment is well understood, while that of the compound eye-dependent entrainment remains to be elucidated. Using molecular and behavioral experiments, we investigated the role of timeless (tim) in the photic entrainment mechanism in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. RNA interference of tim (timRNAi) disrupted the entrainment or prolonged the transients for resynchronization to phase-delayed light–dark cycles. The treatment reduced the magnitude of phase delay caused by delayed light-off, but augmented advance shifts caused by light exposure at late night. TIM protein levels showed daily cycling with an increase during the night and reduction by light exposure at both early and late night. These results suggest that tim plays a critical role in the entrainment to delayed light cycles.

INTRODUCTION

Entrainment to the daily environmental cycle is a fundamental property of the circadian clock. For most organisms, the most powerful entraining agent is the light-dark cycle (LD). In insects, there are two known photoreceptors for photic entrainment. One is CRYPTOCHROME (CRY), which is a blue light receptor whose sequence is similar to that of DNA photolyases (Lin and Todo, 2005), and the other is the compound eye (Helfrich-Förster, 2020). In the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster, CRY is expressed in some cerebral clock neurons (Emery et al., 1998, 2000; Stanewsky et al., 1998; Yoshii et al., 2008) and activated by blue light, leading to proteasomal TIMELESS (TIM) degradation (Lee et al., 1996; Ceriani et al., 1999; Lin et al., 2001), which thus resets the clock in a phase-dependent manner. TIM reduction during early night delays the clock because TIM content must increase to the level required for TIM to enter the nucleus and prohibit its transcription by inactivating the transcriptional activator, CLOCK (CLK)/CYCLE (CYC) heterodimer. At late night, in the declining phase of TIM, the further reduction leads to an advance shift of the clock to the phase at which the TIM level is equal to the reduced level. This resetting mechanism has also been demonstrated in monarch butterflies (Zhu et al., 2008). It has also been suggested in several other species, but has not been proven (Cortes et al., 2010; Yan et al., 2013; Jiang et al., 2018).

The compound eye is the only organ receiving photic information necessary for entrainment in some insects, including cockroaches and crickets (Tomioka and Matsumoto, 2019). Since locomotor rhythms start to free-run even under LD when optic nerves are bilaterally severed in these insects (Nishiitsutsuji-Uwo and Pittendrigh, 1968; Tomioka and Chiba, 1984), the neural information from the compound eye is required for the clock to synchronize with the LD. The underlying molecular mechanism has only been partially clarified in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. The circadian clock of this species consists of two oscillatory loops (Tokuoka et al., 2017; Tomioka and Matsumoto, 2019): the period (per)/tim oscillatory loop, similar to that of the Drosophila clock, and the cry loop. In both loops, CLK/CYC acts as a transcriptional activator that promotes per and tim, or cry2 transcription. The product proteins PER and TIM, or CRY2 are thought to negatively feed back to inhibit the transcriptional activator.

Light is perceived through opsin-long wavelength, a green-light receptor molecule expressed in the compound eye (Komada et al., 2015). The light information is conveyed through the neural pathway to the clock cell in the lamina and medulla region of optic lobes (Kutaragi et al., 2018), and activates two pathways. Delayed light-off upregulates Par domain protein 1 (Pdp1), which further upregulates Clk and tim, thus eventually delaying the clock (Kutaragi et al., 2016). Light exposure in the dark never upregulates Pdp1, but upregulates c-fosB, which acts on two CRYs, CRY1 and CRY2, via currently unknown pathways, and eventually resets the clock (Kutaragi et al., 2018). However, how tim is involved in the entrainment mechanism and its relationship with CRYs are currently unknown.

The present study aimed to clarify the role of tim in the photic entrainment of the circadian clock in G. bimaculatus. We also examined the role of Pdp1, which probably works upstream of tim to affect CLK levels (Kutaragi et al., 2016). We performed RNA interference (RNAi) to suppress expression of tim and Pdp1 and the effects were analyzed at behavioral and molecular levels. TIM protein showed cyclic expression peaking during the night/subjective night, and light induced reduction of TIM levels. RNAi of tim and Pdp1 significantly reduced the TIM protein levels. Crickets treated with timRNAi failed to synchronize to a phase-delayed LD. On the basis of these findings we propose a model in which tim plays a critical role in entrainment to delayed light cycles.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals

All experiments were performed using young adult male G. bimaculatus crickets within a week after the imaginal molt. They were taken from our laboratory colony maintained under controlled conditions of LD with 12 h light and 12 h darkness (light: 0600–1800, Japan Standard Time) and 25.0 ± 1.0°C. The crickets were fed laboratory chow (CA-1, CLEA Japan; Tokyo, Japan) and provided with water.

RNAi

Double-stranded RNA (dsRNA) of G. bimaculatus tim (GenBank/EMBL/DDBJ accession no. BAJ16356), G. bimaculatus Pdp1 (LC512908), and DsRed2 derived from a coral species (Discosoma sp.) were synthesized using the CUGA in vitro Transcription Kit (Nippon Gene, Tokyo, Japan). For tim and Pdp1, cDNAs were used as templates for PCR performed using KOD-Plus-Neo (TOYOBO, Osaka, Japan). The T7- or T3- containing primers used are listed in Table 1. Amplified tim (519 bp) and Pdp1 (475 bp) fragments were extracted with phenol/chloroform, precipitated with ethanol, and then resuspended in Ultra Pure Water (Invitrogen, Tokyo, Japan). For preparing DsRed2 dsRNA (659 bp), the linearized DsRed2 fragment was amplified from pDsRed2-N1 (Clontech, Mountain View, CA, USA) using the primers listed in Table 1. With each of these linearized fragments as a template, RNA was synthesized using T7 or T3 RNA polymerase. Synthesized RNAs were extracted with phenol/chloroform and suspended in 50 µL of TE solution after isopropanol precipitation. After mixing equal amounts of sense and antisense RNA, the RNAs were denatured for 5 min at 100°C and annealed by gradual cooling to room temperature. After ethanol precipitation, the obtained dsRNA was precipitated with ethanol, suspended in Ultra Pure Water (Invitrogen), and adjusted to 20 µM final concentration. The dsRNA solution was stored at –80°C until use. dsRNA solution (760 nL) was injected with a nanoliter injector (WPI, Sarasota, FL, USA) into the abdomen of adult insects anesthetized with CO2. Injection was performed within a week after the imaginal molt.

Table 1.

PCR primers used for quantitative RT-PCR and dsRNA synthesis. The primers tagged with T7 or T3 promoter sequences were used for PCR amplification for dsRNA synthesis. Adaptor sequences for T7 and T3 promoter are underlined.

ta_zs220011_001.gif

Measurement of mRNA levels

qPCR was used to measure mRNA levels. Total RNA was extracted and purified from two optic lobes, including only lamina and medulla region, for each adult male. The sampling was performed 1 week after dsRNA injection. TRIzol Reagent (Invitrogen) was used for RNA extraction and glycogen was added as a carrier. Total RNA was treated with DNase I to remove contaminating genomic DNA. Approximately 400 ng of total RNA from each sample was reverse transcribed with random hexamers and Oligo dT primers using the PrimeScript RT reagent kit (Takara, Otsu, Japan). Real-time PCR was performed using the Mx3000P qPCR System (Agilent Technologies, Santa Clara, CA, USA) using Thunderbird SYBR qPCR Mix (TOYOBO) including SYBR Green with primers shown in Table 1. rpl18a (GenBank/EMBL/DDBJ accession no. DC448653) was used as the internal control. Quantification was performed based using a standard curve obtained with a known amount of template. The results were analyzed using the software associated with the instrument. The values were normalized with the values of rpl18a at each time point. The results of five independent experiments were used to calculate mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM).

Anti-Gryllus TIMELESS antibody preparation

The Gryllus TIM antibody was generated in rabbits by Eurofins Genomics Inc. (Tokyo, Japan). Two different peptides of Gryllus TIM, VVQDKKELRRKKLV (residues 452–465) and SDSSKGSSKSSGDS (residues 323–336), were chemically synthesized and the N-terminal was coupled to KLH using a bifunctional crosslinker, 3-maleimidobenzoyl-N-hydroxy succinimide ester. Two rabbits were immunized with each of the conjugated antigens using a conventional method. The four different sera obtained were tested by Western blotting. Finally, an antiserum directed against the peptide “VVQDKKELRRKKLV” was selected for further analysis.

Western blotting

Before sampling, crickets were placed in LD with 12 h of light and 12 h of darkness (L: 06:00–18:00) for at least 7 days at 25 ± 1°C. For sampling in constant darkness (DD), crickets were transferred from the LD to DD at 18:00 1 day before sampling. Some crickets were exposed to a 2 h light pulse at ZT12, 20, or CT12. ZT stands for Zeitgeber time and ZT0 and ZT12 correspond to light-on and light-off, respectively, under a 12 h of light to 12 h of dark cycle, whereas CT stands for circadian time and CT0 and CT12 correspond to the beginning of the subjective day and subjective night, respectively. Light intensity of the light phase and light pulse was approximately 2 W/m2. The crickets were anesthetized with CO2 at each sampling time point, and the optic lobe-compound eye complexes of the crickets were extracted, individually placed in microcentrifuge tubes, quickly frozen in liquid nitrogen, and stored at –80°C until use. Sampling during dark phase was performed under dim red light.

Protein extracts were prepared from the single optic lobe-compound eye complexes. Each sample was homogenized using a pestle in 100 µL sodium dodecyl sulfate (SDS) sample buffer (2% SDS, 100 mM Tris-HCl (pH 6.8), 5% glycerol, 150 mM dithiothreitol, 0.0025% bromophenol blue), and sonicated for 1 min using a Bioruptor (Tosyo Denki, Yokohama, Japan). After sonication, each sample was heated to 95°C and then loaded on an 8% polyacrylamide–SDS Laemmli gel. The proteins were transferred to a nitrocellulose membrane using a Trans-Blot SD Semi-Dry Transfer Cell (Bio-Rad, Hercules, CA, USA) for 1 h at 25 V in Bjerrum Schafer–Nielsen buffer (48 mM Tris, 39 mM glycine, 20% methanol). The membrane was incubated in TBST (20 mM Tris–HCl (pH 8.0), 150 mM NaCl, 0.5% Tween-20) containing 5% skim milk and the antibody. We used the anti-TIM antibody (1:3000) and monoclonal anti-α-tubulin antibody produced in mouse (1:100,000, Sigma-Aldrich, Tokyo, Japan) as the primary antibody, and horseradish peroxidase (HRP)-linked goat anti-rabbit immunoglobulin G (1:5000, MP Biomedicals, Tokyo, Japan) and HRP-conjugated anti-mouse IgG (H+L) (1:5000, Promega, Tokyo, Japan) as the secondary antibody. The signals were visualized using the Immobilon Western Chemiluminescent HRP Substrate on LAS-4000 (Fuji Films, Tokyo, Japan).

Locomotor activity recording

The locomotor activities of the crickets were recorded as described previously (Moriyama et al., 2008). In brief, adult crickets were individually housed in a transparent plastic box (18 × 9 × 4.5 cm) with a rocking substratum immediately after dsRNA injection. Movement of the substratum caused by a moving cricket was sensed by a magnetic reed switch and recorded every 6 min using a computerized system. Food and water were provided ad libitum. The actographs were placed in an incubator in which lighting conditions were provided by a cool white fluorescent lamp connected to an electric timer, and the temperature was kept constant at 25 ± 0.5°C. The light intensity ranged from 2.04 to 9.27 W/m2 and varied depending on the distance from the light source. The raw data were displayed as conventional double-plotted actograms to determine activity patterns.

Fig. 1.

Effects of Pdp1RNAi (A) and timRNAi (B) on mRNA levels of the respective gene in the optic lobe in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. The mRNA levels were measured at Zeitgeber time (ZT)18 7 days after dsRNA injection. The values are normalized by the value of dsDsRed2-treated control. Note that Pdp1RNAi and timRNAi treatment significantly reduced mRNA levels of the respective genes compared to the dsDsRed2-treated control. *, t-test, P < 0.05.

fi_zs220011_001.jpg

Fig. 2.

Double-plotted actograms showing re-entrainment of locomotor rhythms to 6 h (A) advanced and (B) delayed light–dark cycle in dsDsRed2-treated control, Pdp1RNAi, and timRNAi adult male crickets Gryllus bimaculatus. White and black bars above the actograms show light (white) and dark (black) cycles. Gray area in actograms indicates the dark phase. Arrows indicate the day when light cycle was shifted.

fi_zs220011_002.jpg

The light cycles were shifted by 6 h in the delay or advance direction by extending or shortening the light phase, respectively, and the transient cycles for re-entrainment were estimated as the time span between the day of shift and the day when the onset restored the original phase relationship with LD.

The phase shifts caused by a 3 h single light pulse at ZT12, ZT20, and CT12 on the first day of DD were investigated. After exposure to the light pulse, the crickets were maintained under DD. The magnitude of phase shifts was estimated as the time difference between the phases of light pulse-treated and untreated animals, which were determined by extrapolation of activity onset under DD to the day of the light pulse using actogramJ (Schmid et al., 2011).

Statistics

One-way ANOVA followed by post hoc Tukey's test was used to compare the means of the differently treated groups. Student's t-test was used to compare the means of the two groups.

RESULTS

Re-entrainment of locomotor rhythms to 6 h advanced LD

We first examined the re-entrainment of locomotor rhythms to 6 h advanced LDs in DsRed2 dsRNA (dsDsRed2)-treated control, Pdp1RNAi, and timRNAi crickets. The effectiveness of RNAi was investigated in Pdp1RNAi or timRNAi crickets. The Pdp1 or tim mRNA levels in the optic lobe were measured at ZT18, when they were near the peak level (Danbara et al., 2010; Narasaki-Funo et al., 2020), and 7 days after the treatment. RNAi treatment significantly suppressed the mRNA levels of the respective genes (Fig. 1), as reported previously (Danbara et al., 2010; Narasaki-Funo et al., 2020). Figure 2 shows representative actograms of dsDsRed2-treated control, Pdp1RNAi, and timRNAi crickets. Under LD, the three groups of animals showed a nocturnal rhythm similar to that of untreated animals (Tomioka and Chiba, 1982). When the light cycles were advanced by 6 h by shortening the light period, all animals resynchronized to the newly phased LD (Fig. 2A, Table 2). The transient cycles necessary for re-entrainment were approximately 3 days (3.26 ± 1.10 (mean ± SD) days) in the dsDsRed2-treated control crickets (Fig. 3A). Pdp1RNAi and timRNAi crickets yielded results similar to those of the control crickets, with transient cycles of 3.60 ± 0.70 and 3.14 ± 1.57 days, respectively (Fig. 3A).

Re-entrainment of locomotor rhythms to 6 h delayed LD

We then tested the re-entrainment of locomotor rhythms to 6 h delayed LDs. dsDsRed2-treated control crickets resynchronized to the shifted LD with transient cycles of 3.00 ± 0.82 days (Figs. 2B, 3B; Table 2). Pdp1RNAi crickets resynchronized to LD with transient cycles of 4.33 ± 1.40 days, which was slightly longer than that in the control crickets, but the difference was not significant (Figs. 2B, 3B; Table 2). In contrast, timRNAi prevented resynchronization in 46.7% of the treated crickets and the remaining crickets resynchronized to the shifted LD with significantly longer transient cycles (5.25 ± 1.84 days; Figs. 2B, 3B; Table 2). Figure 2B right panel shows the actogram of a representative timRNAi cricket showing a free-running rhythm under LD. In this particular cricket, the activity was enhanced when the active phase overlapped with the light phase, suggesting that a photoreceptive system was functioning.

Phase shifts caused by 3 h light pulses

To investigate the effect of Pdp1RNAi and timRNAi on phase responsiveness to light, we examined the magnitude of phase shifts of locomotor rhythms caused by a 3 h light pulse given at ZT12–15, CT12–15, or ZT20–23. DsDsRed2-treated crickets were used as controls. The pulse at ZT12–15 was given as a 3 h extension of the light phase. The light treatment was followed by constant darkness.

Table 2.

Summary of results of entrainment of locomotor rhythms to light dark cycles shifted by 6 h in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus treated with dsDsRed2, Pdp1RNAi, or timRNAi.

ta_zs220011_002.gif

Fig. 3.

Transient cycles necessary for re-entrainment of locomotor rhythms to 6 h (A) advanced and (B) delayed light–dark cycles in dsDsRed2-treated control, Pdp1RNAi, and timRNAi adult male crickets Gryllus bimaculatus. Numbers in each column indicate the number of crickets used. Different symbols above the column indicate significant differences (ANOVA followed by Tukey's test, P < 0.05).

fi_zs220011_003.jpg

A light pulse at ZT20–23 caused an advance shift in all the treated crickets (Fig. 4A). The magnitudes of the shift were 1.02 ± 0.56, 1.46 ± 0.70, and 1.80 ± 0.43 h in dsDsRed2-treated control, Pdp1RNAi, and timRNAi crickets, respectively (Fig. 5A). No significant difference was found between control and Pdp1RNAi crickets, whereas timRNAi significantly increased the magnitude of the shift (Fig. 5A).

Fig. 4.

Phase shifts of locomotor rhythms caused by a 3 h light pulse given at (A) Zeitgeber time (ZT)20–23, (B) ZT12–15, or (C) circadian time (CT)12–15 in dsDsRed2-treated control, Pdp1RNAi, and timRNAi adult male crickets Gryllus bimaculatus. Thick arrows in actograms indicate the start of a light pulse. Thin arrows on the left side of actograms indicate the day of transfer to constant dark (DD). DD started at 18:00 in (A) and (C), while at 21:00 in (B). For other explanations see legend of Fig. 2.

fi_zs220011_004.jpg

A light pulse at ZT12–15 caused a delay shift (Fig. 4B), whose magnitude was similar between dsDsRed2-treated control (–3.40 ± 0.44 h) and Pdp1RNAi crickets (–3.05 ± 0.58 h) but was significantly smaller in timRNAi crickets (–2.71 ± 0.38 h; Fig. 5B).

A light pulse at CT12–15 also caused a delay shift in all treated crickets (Fig. 4C). The magnitudes of the shift were –2.55 ± 0.64 h, –1.31 ± 0.59 h, and –2.27 ± 0.80 h for dsDsRed2-treated control, Pdp1RNAi, and timRNAi crickets, respectively (Fig. 5C). Thus, Pdp1RNAi significantly reduced the magnitude of the shift compared to the shifts in control and timRNAi crickets. However, the magnitude was not significantly different between control and timRNAi crickets.

TIM cycling and light-induced reduction

By Western blotting, daily changes in TIM levels in the optic lobe-compound eye complex were examined under LD and DD conditions (Fig. 6A). TIM was detected as a protein of approximately 100 kDa, which was close to that expected from the amino acid sequence deduced from the obtained cDNA (Danbara et al., 2010). In dsDsRed2-treated animals, TIM showed cyclic expression, with low levels during daytime, increasing during the night to peak at the end of nighttime. The rhythmic changes in the TIM were maintained in constant darkness. TIM levels were strongly reduced by timRNAi. The TIM levels were also reduced by Pdp1RNAi.

Fig. 5.

Magnitude of the phase shifts of locomotor rhythms caused by a 3 h light pulse given at (A) Zeitgeber time (ZT)20–23, (B) ZT12–15, or (C) circadian time (CT)12–15 in dsDsRed2-treated control, Pdp1RNAi, and timRNAi adult male crickets Gryllus bimaculatus. Numbers in each column indicate the number of crickets used. Different symbols indicate significant differences (ANOVA followed by Tukey's test, P < 0.05).

fi_zs220011_005.jpg

We then examined the effects of 2 h light exposure at early night (ZT12–14), late night (ZT20–22), and early subjective night (CT12–14). We used 2 h light pulses here because maximum c-fosB induction associated with phase shift was caused by 1 h light exposure (Kutaragi et al., 2018), and our preliminary results showed that a 2 h light pulse at ZT18-20 caused significant phase advance of locomotor rhythms: activity onset for crickets with light exposure was 16.47 ± 0.40 h (n = 13) and that for crickets without exposure 17.42 ± 0.25 h (n = 14) (t-test, P < 0.001). The results of light exposure are shown in Fig. 6B. In all the phases examined, TIM levels were strongly reduced 2 h after exposure. The reduction at ZT12–14 and at CT12–14 was greater than that at ZT20–22, followed by a gradual increase in the ensuing darkness (Fig. 6B).

Fig. 6.

TIMELESS (TIM) protein levels in the optic lobe-compound eye complex of the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. (A) TIM expressed in a daily cyclic manner under light–dark cycle (LD) and constant darkness (DD) with increase during night or subjective night. timRNAi and Pdp1RNAi suppressed TIM expression. (B) Light exposure for 2 h at Zeitgeber time (ZT)12–14, circadian time (CT)12–14, or ZT20–22 strongly reduced TIM levels. At early night or early subjective night the reduction followed re-increase of TIM protein levels. White, black, and gray bars indicate day, night or subjective night, and subjective day, respectively. Hatched bars indicate light exposure.

fi_zs220011_006.jpg

DISCUSSION

TIM cycling and effects of light on TIM

The present study revealed that TIM protein was rhythmically expressed in the optic lobe-compound eye complex with a peak at late night (Fig. 6). The cycling of TIM probably reflects the rhythmic expression of tim both in the optic lobe and the compound eye, lagging behind the mRNA rhythm by approximately 4 h (Danbara et al., 2010; Ohguro et al., 2020). The cycling is similar to that in Drosophila and the monarch butterfly, Danaus plexippus (Zeng et al., 1996; Zhu et al., 2008), suggesting that TIM cycling may be conserved across insect species. TIM protein levels are reduced by light exposure during both early and late night. The light-induced TIM reduction is also similar to that in Drosophila and the monarch butterfly (Lee et al., 1996; Myers et al., 1996; Zhu et al., 2008), suggesting that insect clock resetting, at least partially, includes TIM degradation. According to the Drosophila hypothesis, TIM reduction at early night delays the clock phase until a sufficient amount of TIM is produced (Lee et al., 1996). In this study, the light-induced TIM reduction at early night was followed by an increase in TIM (Fig. 6B). Light exposure at late night, however, advances the clock phase by further decreasing the TIM, which is in its declining phase (Lee et al., 1996; Myers et al., 1996). Our Western blot results also showed no clear increase in TIM levels 2 h after light exposure at ZT20–22 (Fig. 6B). Thus, crickets and flies most likely share the use of TIM for light entrainment, even if only partially, suggesting that TIM may have a common role in light entrainment of insect circadian clocks across species.

However, the mechanism underlying TIM degradation is likely to differ between crickets and flies. In Drosophila, TIM is degraded by light activated CRY (Hunter-Ensor et al., 1996; Lee et al., 1996; Emery et al., 1998), while Drosophila-type CRY (CRY1) does not seem to be involved in crickets, since light entrainment of the optic lobe circadian clock depends only on light information perceived by the compound eye in crickets (Tomioka and Chiba, 1984; Komada et al., 2015), and cry1RNAi had no effect on light entrainment (Tokuoka et al., 2017). In the cricket, the light information perceived by the compound eye is conducted by a neural pathway toward the optic lobe and affects the clock through a neurotransmitter that activates pathways including TIM and CRYs to shift the clock (Kutaragi et al., 2016, 2018). A similar neurotransmitter-dependent photic entrainment may exist in the clock within the compound eye, because the clock in the compound eye is controlled via neural pathways from the optic lobe (Ohguro et al., 2020). This hypothesis should be investigated in future studies.

TIM reduction at early night requires TIM production to operate the negative feedback loop of PER/TIM (Tomioka and Matsumoto, 2019). TIM re-accumulation after its reduction by a prolonged light phase is consistent with the upregulation of tim transcription that follows Pdp1 upregulation (Kutaragi et al., 2016). The enhancement of tim expression may contribute to TIM production. Pdp1RNAi slightly increased transient cycles for re-entrainment of locomotor rhythms to 6 h delayed LD, although the increase was not significant. Western blotting in Pdp1RNAi crickets showed that TIM levels were reduced, but there was a detectable daily TIM level oscillation. Therefore, the low-level TIM oscillation that survived Pdp1RNAi may be sufficient for the resynchronization of the rhythm in the delay direction.

Although the mechanism of light-induced TIM degradation in this cricket is currently unknown, it is apparently neurotransmitter-dependent in the optic lobe circadian clock (Fig. 7). It is possible that CRYs are involved in this degradation because cry1/cry2 double RNAi often prevents photic entrainment (Kutaragi et al., 2018). Since CRYs may be regulated by neurotransmitters light-dependently without apparent changes in cry1 and cry2 mRNA levels (Kutaragi et al., 2018), TIM degradation may be regulated by CRY proteins that should be modified by light through some neurotransmitters. In addition, a neurotransmitter-mediated, CRY-independent TIM degradation pathway is also known in some cerebral clock neurons in Drosophila (Guo et al., 2014), which should be investigated in the future.

Role of tim in photic entrainment

The results of the present study showed that tim is required for the phase delay of the cricket clock. Re-entrainment to 6 h advanced LD was not affected by either timRNAi or Pdp1RNAi (Figs. 2A, 3A). Therefore, the mechanism without tim and Pdp1 is sufficient for the phase advance of the rhythm. timRNAi prevented re-entrainment to 6 h delayed LD cycles in nearly 50% of the treated crickets and lengthened the transient cycles to re-entrainment in the rest (Figs. 2B, 3B). The disruption of re-entrainment to delayed LD in timRNAi crickets may be attributable, at least partially, to the reduced magnitude of phase delay caused by 3 h light phase extension (Fig. 5B). It is also likely that a greater advance shift induced by a light pulse at late night (ZT20-23, Figs.4A, 5A) and shorter free-running periods in timRNAi crickets (Danbara et al., 2010) caused slow re-entrainment or phase shifts in the opposite direction.

Fig. 7.

Current hypothesis of the molecular mechanism of photic entrainment of the circadian clock in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. The cricket's clock consists of two major feedback loops, i.e., the per/tim-loop and the cry2-loop (Tokuoka et al., 2017). The latter includes CRY1 and CRY2 variants. Light is perceived by opsin-long wavelength (opLW) expressed in the compound eye, conducted to the optic lobe clock neuron via a neural pathway, and causes upregulation of Pdp1 and c-fosB via neurotransmitters, increasing PDP1 and C-FOSB levels. C-FOSB subsequently modulates CRYs, which may cause TIM degradation, leading to reset of the per/tim oscillatory loop. See text for detailed explanation.

fi_zs220011_007.jpg

This delay shift disruption is probably because delay shift requires the per/tim oscillatory loop. timRNAi stops tim oscillation, and mRNA levels are maintained below the level of its normal oscillation (Danbara et al., 2010). The present finding that TIM protein levels were kept very low in timRNAi optic lobe-compound eye complexes is consistent with the previous results on mRNA (Danbara et al., 2010). However, the results of Western blotting showed that very weak TIM expression with a daily rhythm may survive timRNAi in some crickets (Fig. 6). This weak expression might contribute to the slow resynchronization of entrained crickets.

TIM reduction at early night should be required for the delay shift of the clock. TIM reduction is probably coupled with the cry oscillatory loop, because cry1/cry2 double knockdown by RNAi disrupted the normal entrainment to shifted LDs in both phase advance and delay (Kutaragi et al., 2018). This also suggests that events, including TIM degradation, are most likely downstream of CRYs. To verify this, the effects of cry1 and cry2 RNAi on light-induced TIM degradation should be examined.

Advance shifts caused by light at ZT20-23 were greater in timRNAi insects than those in dsDsRed2-treated control crickets. This suggests that the advance shift is caused by a mechanism involving some factor(s) other than tim and that tim may weaken the advance mechanism. The advance mechanism needs to be elucidated, but it probably includes CRYs since we have previously reported that cry1 and cry2 double RNAi strongly disrupted entrainment to 6 h advanced LD (Kutaragi et al., 2018).

Considering the currently available data on photic entrainment, we propose the following hypothesis of light entrainment for the cricket clock (Fig. 7). Light is perceived by opsin-long wavelength expressed in photoreceptor cells in the compound eye (Komada et al., 2015). This information is transmitted to the circadian clock in the optic lobe. In this conducting pathway, the photic information from the receptor converges to some interneurons because partial removal of the compound eye reduces entrainability (Tomioka et al., 1990). The light information sent to the clock cells acts on the molecular oscillatory mechanism of the clock via neurotransmitters. The released neurotransmitter induces c-fosB, which acts on CRY through an unknown system (Kutaragi et al., 2018). Then, CRY degrades TIM via a system that is yet to be elucidated, thereby resetting the phase of the per/tim oscillatory loop. This hypothesis should be tested in future studies.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This study was partially supported by JSPS grants No. 23370033, No. 18H02480, and 21K06267.

COMPETING INTERESTS

The authors have no competing interests to declare.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

YM and K Tomioka conceived and designed the study. YM, TS, K Takeuchi, and K Tomioka performed the RNAi and behavioral experiments. TY prepared antibodies, and MM and KM performed the Western blot experiments. K Tomioka analyzed the data. YM and K Tomioka wrote the original draft of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

REFERENCES

1.

Ceriani MF, Darlington TK, Staknis D, Mas P, Petti AA, Weitz CJ, Kay SA (1999) Light-dependent sequentation of TIMELESS by CRYPTOCHROME. Science 285: 553–556 Google Scholar

2.

Cortes T, Ortiz-Rivas B, Martinez-Torres D (2010) Identification and characterization of circadian clock genes in the pea aphid Acyrthosiphon pisum. Insect Mol Biol 19 (Suppl. 2): 123–139 Google Scholar

3.

Danbara Y, Sakamoto T, Uryu O, Tomioka K (2010) RNA interference of timeless gene does not disrupt circadian locomotor rhythms in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. J Insect Physiol 56: 1738–1745 Google Scholar

4.

Emery P, So WV, Kaneko M, Hall JC, Rosbash M (1998) CRY, a Drosophila clock and light-regulated cryptochrome, is a major contributor to circadian rhythm resetting and photosensitivity. Cell 95: 669–679 Google Scholar

5.

Emery P, Stanewsky R, Hall JC, Rosbash M (2000) Drosophila cryptochromes: a unique circadian-rhythm photoreceptor. Nature 404: 456–457 Google Scholar

6.

Guo F, Cerullo I, Chen X, Rosbash M (2014) PDF neuron firing phase-shifts key circadian activity neurons in Drosophila. eLife 3: e02780 Google Scholar

7.

Helfrich-Förster C (2020) Light input pathways to the circadian clock of insects with an emphasis on the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Physiol A 206: 259–272 Google Scholar

8.

Hunter-Ensor M, Ousley A, Sehgal A (1996) Regulation of the Drosophila protein timeless suggests a mechanism for resetting the circadian clock by light. Cell 84: 677–686 Google Scholar

9.

Jiang Y-D, Yuan X, Zhou W-W, Bai Y-L, Wang G-Y, Zhu Z-R (2018) Cryptochrome regulates crcadian locomotor rhythms in the small brown planthopper Laodelphax striatellus (Fallén). Front Physiol 9 Google Scholar

10.

Komada S, Kamae Y, Koyanagi M, Tatewaki K, Hassaneen E, Saifullah A, et al. (2015) Green-sensitive opsin is the photoreceptor for photic entrainment of an insect circadian clock. Zool Lett 1: 11 Google Scholar

11.

Kutaragi Y, Miki T, Bando T, Tomioka K (2016) Transcriptional and non-transcriptional events are involved in photic entrainment of the circadian clock in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. Physiol Entomol 41: 358–368 Google Scholar

12.

Kutaragi Y, Tokuoka A, Tomiyama Y, Nose M, Watanabe T, Bando T, Moriyama Y, Tomioka K (2018) A novel photic entrainment mechanism for the circadian clock in an insect: involvement of c-fos and cryptochromes. Zool Lett 4: 26 Google Scholar

13.

Lee C, Parikh V, Itsukaichi T, Bae K, Edery I (1996) Resetting the Drosophila clock by photic regulation of PER and a PER-TIM complex. Science 271: 1740–1744 Google Scholar

14.

Lin C, Todo T (2005) The cryptochromes. Genome Biol 6: 220 Google Scholar

15.

Lin F-J, Song W, Meyer-Bernstein E, Naidoo N, Sehgal A (2001) Photic signaling by cryptochrome in the Drosophila circadian system. Mol Cell Biol 21: 7287–7294 Google Scholar

16.

Moriyama Y, Sakamoto T, Karpova SG, Matsumoto A, Noji S, Tomioka K (2008) RNA interference of the clock gene period disrupts circadian rhythms in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. J Biol Rhythms 23: 308–318 Google Scholar

17.

Myers MP, Wager-Smith K, Rothenfluh-Hilfiker A, Young MW (1996) Light-induced degradation of TIMELESS and entrainment of the Drosophila circadian clock. Science 271: 1736–1740 Google Scholar

18.

Narasaki-Funo Y, Tomiyama Y, Nose M, Bando T, Tomioka K (2020) Functional analysis of Pdp1 and vrille in the circadian system of a cricket. J Insect Physiol 127: 104156 Google Scholar

19.

Nishiitsutsuji-Uwo J, Pittendrigh CS (1968) Central nervous system control of circadian rhythmicity in the cockroach. II. The pathway of light signals that entrain the rhythm. Z Vergl Physiol 58: 1–13 Google Scholar

20.

Ohguro C, Moriyama Y, Tomioka K (2020) The compound eye possesses a self-sustaining circadian oscillator in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. Zool Sci 38: 82–89 Google Scholar

21.

Schmid B, Helfrich-Förster C, Yoshii T (2011) A new ImageJ plug-in “ActogramJ” for chronobiological analyses. J Biol Rhythms 26: 464–467 Google Scholar

22.

Stanewsky R, Kaneko M, Emery P, Beretta B, Wager-Smith K, Kay SA, et al. (1998) The cry b mutation identifies cryptochrome as a circadian photoreceptor in Drosophila. Cell 95: 681–692 Google Scholar

23.

Tokuoka A, Itoh TQ, Hori S, Uryu O, Danbara Y, Nose M, et al. (2017) cryptochrome genes form an oscillatory loop independent of the per/tim loop in the circadian clockwork of the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. Zool Lett 3: 5 Google Scholar

24.

Tomioka K, Chiba Y (1982) Post-embryonic development of circadian rhythm in the cricket, Gryllus bimaculatus. J Comp Physiol A 147: 299–304 Google Scholar

25.

Tomioka K, Chiba Y (1984) Effects of nymphal stage optic nerve severance or optic lobe removal on the circadian locomotor rhythm of the cricket, Gryllus bimaculatus. Zool Sci 1: 375–382 Google Scholar

26.

Tomioka K, Matsumoto A (2019) Chapter Three – The circadian system in insects: Cellular, molecular, and functional organization. Advances in Insect Physiology 56: 73–115 Google Scholar

27.

Tomioka K, Okada Y, Chiba Y (1990) Distribution of circadian photoreceptors in the compound eye of the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. J Biol Rhythms 5: 131–139 Google Scholar

28.

Yan S, Ni H, Li H, Zhang J, Liu X, Zhang Q (2013) Molecular cloning, characterization, and mRNA expression of two cryptochrome genes in Helicoverpa armigera (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae). J Econ Entomol 106: 450–462 Google Scholar

29.

Yoshii T, Todo T, Wülbeck C, Stanewsky R, Helfrich-Förster C (2008) Cryptochrome is present in the compound eyes and a subset of Drosophila's clock neurons. J Comp Neurol 508: 952–966 Google Scholar

30.

Zeng H, Qian Z, Meyers MP, Rosbash M (1996) A light-entrainment mechanism for the Drosophila circadian clock. Nature 380: 129–135 Google Scholar

31.

Zhu H, Sauman I, Yuan Q, Casselman A, Emery-Le M, Emery P, Reppert SM (2008) Cryptochromes define a novel circadian clock mechanism in monarch butterflies that may underlie sun compass navigation. PLOS Biol 6: e4 Google Scholar
© 2022 Zoological Society of Japan
Yoshiyuki Moriyama, Kazuki Takeuchi, Tsugumichi Shinohara, Koichi Miyagawa, Mirai Matsuka, Taishi Yoshii, and Kenji Tomioka "Timeless Plays an Important Role in Compound Eye-Dependent Photic Entrainment of the Circadian Rhythm in the Cricket Gryllus bimaculatus," Zoological Science 39(4), 397-405, (10 June 2022). https://doi.org/10.2108/zs220011
Received: 4 February 2022; Accepted: 16 April 2022; Published: 10 June 2022
KEYWORDS
circadian clock
compound eye
cricket
entrainment
phase shifts
RNAi
tim
Back to Top