Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
31 August 2020 Retreat Sites Shared by Two Liolaemus Lizard Species: Exploring the Potential Role of Scents
Mario R. Ruiz-Monachesi, Soledad Valdecantos, Fernando Lobo, Félix B. Cruz, Antonieta Labra
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Some lizard species modulate the use of a retreat site based on the presence of scents from other individuals, behavior that requires scent recognition. Here, we investigated if two congeneric and syntopic lizards (adults of Liolaemus coeruleus and juveniles of L. ceii, which can be found sharing retreat sites in the wild) discriminate scents from each other during the pre-hibernation period. If the presence of heterospecific scents modulates sharing retreat sites, species would discriminate each other's scents. Lizards were individually exposed to four treatments, which were enclosures with scents of (1) from the focal individual (own); (2) a conspecific of the same sex as the focal lizard; (3) a heterospecific of the same sex as the focal lizard; and (4) a control (i.e., enclosure with a clean substrate). Our results suggest that there is no heterospecific recognition. This finding was not due to an inability to chemo-assess scents, since both species exhibited self-recognition (recognition of their own scents) and juveniles of L. ceii also showed conspecific recognition. Although it might be advantageous for species to share retreat sites, chemical recognition of heterospecific scents does not modulate this behavior in these syntopic species.

INTRODUCTION

Most animal species need to use retreat sites to protect themselves and/or their offspring (Mateo and Cuadrado, 2012) against different factors such as predation (Downes and Shine, 1998) or environmental conditions, such as extreme temperatures (Aguilar and Cruz, 2010; van den Berg et al., 2015). Evidence indicates that retreat sites are not chosen randomly, as they need to fulfill some basic requirements (e.g., Croak et al., 2008; van den Berg et al., 2015) such as an adequate size (Kroon et al., 2000; Caruso, 2016) or a proper three-dimensional structure (Croak et al., 2008). Besides these basic constraints, retreat site selection can also be modulated by intrinsic characteristics of the individuals, such as age, sex, or/and reproductive condition (Rutherford and Gregory, 2003; Vasconcelos et al., 2017). All these requirements determine that retreat sites can be a scarce resource for which animals need to compete (Marvin, 1998; Penado et al., 2015). There are, however, cases in which animals share these sites (Mouton, 2011; Mori et al., 2015), as occurs, for example, in communal nesting, a behavior observed across diverse taxa (e.g., mammals, Auclair et al., 2014; birds, Bertram, 2014; reptiles, Dayananda et al., 2016).

For several lizard species, retreat site selection is modulated by the presence of scents from other individuals (e.g., Hayward and Mouton, 2007; Lewis et al., 2007), and the selection process requires scent recognition. In fact, there is clear evidence that sites that contain scents associated with a threat, from conspecifics or heterospecifics, are avoided (e.g., lizards reject sites with predator scents; Stapey, 2003; Amo et al., 2004; Lloyd et al., 2009). The threat imposed by conspecifics is exemplified by Podarcis hispanica (Steindachner, 1870), in which females prefer retreat sites with scents from a small male rather than those from a large male, which should reduce the possibilities of sexual harassment (Carazo et al., 2011). Similarly, after agonistic interactions, loser males of Oedura lesueuri (Duméril and Bibron, 1836) prefer retreat sites with scents of unknown males over those with winners' scents (Kondo et al., 2007). In the case of Egernia stokesii Gray, 1845, individuals select retreat sites with scents of familiar rather than unfamiliar conspecifics (Bull et al., 2000). In contrast, for some species retreat site selection is neither modulated by conspecific nor heterospecific scents (e.g., Cooper et al., 1999; Hibbitts and Whiting, 2005; Paulissen, 2006), and the physical presence of an individual seems to be the key element for individuals to choose or reject a retreat site (Schlesinger and Shine, 1994; Langkilde and Shine, 2005).

In Argentinean Patagonia, we observed adults of Liolaemus coeruleus Cei and Ortiz-Zapata, 1983 and juveniles of L. ceii (Donoso-Barros, 1971) in shared retreat sites formed by small rocks in the soil. To understand the mechanism involved in retreat site sharing, and specifically to determine if this behavior is modulated by the presence of heterospecific scents, we tested the ability of these two lizards to discriminate between each other's scents. Since studies have shown that species of Liolaemus Wiegmann, 1834 exhibit chemical discrimination between conspecific and congeneric scents (e.g., Labra, 2011), we postulate that both species may show discrimination of heterospecifics scents. We performed the study during the pre-hibernation season, when lizards are more engaged in acquiring good retreat sites, since the hibernation period is arriving. This increases the possibility of aggregation (e.g., Weintraub, 1968; Elfström and Zucker, 1999; Bishop and Echternacht, 2004).

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Collection and maintenance of animals

As part of an ongoing project, in February 2014 during a field trip near Alumine, Neuquén (Route 13 between Kilka and Primeros Pinos: 38°54′14.70″S, 70°43′59.50″W), we found adults of Liolaemus coeruleus sharing retreat sites with juveniles of L. ceii. We collected 16 L. coeruleus (nine females, seven males) and 11 L. ceii (five females, six males). Lizards were captured by hand and kept in individual cloth bags with their identification until arriving at the laboratory, where we measured and weighted them (Table 1). Thereafter, we placed lizards individually in plastic enclosures (36 × 27 × 19 cm) covered with a plastic mesh lid. Enclosures had 3 cm of sandy substrate, a rock that served as shelter and basking site, and a small bowl of water. Lizards were maintained in an isolated room with a summer photoperiod (13:11 h, light:dark) using halogen lamps, which also allowed maintaining a mean ± SD ambient temperature of 30°C ± 2°C during the light phase. Every other day, we fed each lizard with two Tenebrio mollitor Linnaeus, 1758 larvae, dusted with vitamins. Before running the experiments, lizards remained undisturbed in their enclosures for one week. This allowed lizards to habituate to the experimental conditions and to release scents, because enclosures were used as the substrate-borne scents (for more details see Labra, 2011).

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics showing X ± SE (minimum–maximum) of snout–vent length (SVL) and weight (W) of Liolaemus coeruleus and L. ceii. Values of t-tests between species are included. In bold, the statistically significant P values (P < 0.05).

img-z2-6_79.gif

At the end of all experiments, and because individuals were primarily collected for systematic studies, they were euthanized via a pericardial injection of pentothal, following standard procedures (Scrocchi and Kretzschmar, 1996). Lizards were fixed in 10% formol and conserved in 70% ethanol to be deposited in the Herpetological collection of Instituto de Bio y Geociencias del NOA (IBIGEO). We dissected the individuals to confirm that adults of Liolaemus coeruleus were in post-reproductive condition and that all individuals of L. ceii were juveniles, as well as to confirm their sex.

Experimental design

Using a counterbalanced design, each lizard was submitted individually only once to each of the four treatments (henceforth: “scents”). Following established protocols (Labra et al., 2003; Labra, 2011), we used as a source of scents enclosures previously used by: (1) the focal individual (own), (2) a conspecific of the same sex of the focal individual, (3) a heterospecific of the same sex of the focal lizard, and (4) an odorless control (i.e., an unused enclosure with clean sand). Before starting an experiment, we removed the occupant (“donor”) of the enclosure that would be used for the experiment (“experimental enclosure”), together with the water container and the refuge. We also removed the focal lizard from its enclosure and placed it in its individual cloth bag for 10 min to minimize the stress associated with handling (Labra, 2011). Next, we placed the bag on the sand of the experimental enclosure, allowing the lizard to exit freely. Then, we removed the bag and, once we were out of the lizard's field of vision, began recording with a digital stopwatch the time to the first tongue flick. This is the period from when the lizard came into contact with the enclosure substrate (without our perturbation) until it's first tongue flick, which represents the beginning of chemical exploration (e.g., Labra and Niemeyer, 1999). Thereafter, we recorded the lizard's behavior for 8 min using a digital video camera (Sony DCR-SR67) installed 50 cm above the enclosure and connected to a monitor. We accepted as maximum time to the first tongue flick 7 min; otherwise, we canceled the trial and repeated it another day (n = 1). At the end of a trial, we verified that lizards (focal and donor) were healthy and returned them to their respective enclosures, remaining undisturbed for at least 3 d before a new trial. We used clean gloves for each trial to avoid scent cross-contamination and potentially bias responses, and appropriate actions were taken to minimize the stress of lizards during the whole process.

Digital videos were subsequently analyzed using VLC Media Player 2.2 (VideoLan, 2006). From videos, we recorded the following behaviors:

  • (1) Time in motion (s): index of behavioral exploration, which includes the total time that the lizard moved and changed its position, either the whole body or part of it. Data for Liolaemus species show that lizards increase their time in motion when they are exposed to scents that require getting more information (e.g., new scents; Labra, 2008a). A reduced exploration of scents may indicate that they are associated with potential risk (e.g., Labra and Hoare, 2015) or are very familiar (e.g., own scents) and do not require further exploration (e.g., Troncoso-Palacios and Labra, 2012). This variable excluded motions associated with the behaviors described below.

  • (2) Time escaping (s): total time spent excavating or standing up against the walls of the enclosure. This behavior may indicate a potential threat detected by the lizard (Font and Desfilis, 2002).

  • (3) Number of tongue flicks: index of chemical exploration (Font and Desfilis, 2002) that considers the number of times the lizard protrudes and rapidly retracts its tongue, regardless of whether the tongue touches the substrate or wall or is waved in the air (e.g., Labra, 2008b).

  • (4) Marking behavior: the lizard drags its cloaca, which may help to release scents (Alberts, 1992).

  • (5) Head-bob displays: stereotyped up and down head movements, usually exhibited in social interactions, involving demonstration of fighting or defense abilities, toward a competitor (Labra et al., 2007) or a predator (Ortega et al., 2017).

  • (6) Forelimb displays: forearm waving or circular motions of the forelimbs. Its function in Liolaemus is not completely clear, but it is possibly associated with intraspecific communication denoting challenging or relative arousal, conflict, or appeasement behavior (Halloy and Castillo, 2002). Alternatively, this can be an antidepredator behavior (e.g., Font et al., 2012).

  • (7) Tail waving: rapid side-to-side movement of the entire or the posterior portion of the tail, displayed in agonistic contexts, probably as a demonstration of strength (Vitt et al., 1974). This display can also be exhibited during predation risk (Ortega et al., 2017).

Statistical analysis

We compared the body measurements (snout–vent length and weight) between the species using paired t-tests for independent groups (Liolaemus coeruleus, L. ceii). To determine whether there was an effect of the species (L. coeruleus, L. ceii), scent (own, conspecific, heterospecific, control), and their interactions upon four variables (latency to first tongue flick, time in motion, time escaping, and number of tongue flicks), we used two-way ANOVAs for repeated measurements (scents). Analyses were followed by post-hoc Fisher LSD tests. The residuals of these four variables exhibited normality, except those of time in motion, which was square-root-transformed to fulfill the assumptions of normality and homoscedasticity. Preliminary analyses indicated that sex did not modulate any of these four variables; therefore, we did not include this factor. The other variables recorded (marking behavior, head-bobs displays, forelimb displays, and tail waving) were exhibited in low frequency, and therefore, we pooled them in a new variable named “displays” (for details see Labra, 2006). We tested the effect of scents over displays with Friedman nonparametric tests. For this variable, preliminary analyses showed an effect of the sex, and we analyzed this factor for each species using Friedman tests followed by Wilcoxon matched tests. We present data as x̄ ± SE.

RESULTS

Adults of Liolaemus coeruleus and juveniles of L. ceii had similar body size, although L. ceii was lighter (Table 1). Both the latency to the first tongue flick and time escaping were unaffected by the studied factors (species and scents) or their interactions (Table 2). The mean values of latency were x̄ = 47.27 ± 5.06 s for L. coeruleus and x̄ = 36.26 ± 6.10 s for L. ceii, while the mean time escaping was x̄ = 85.90 ± 10.92 s and x̄ = 64.70 ± 13.03 s for each species, respectively.

Time in motion differed between species, with Liolaemus coeruleus moving less than L. ceii (Table 2; Fig. 1A). Further, this variable was also affected by the interaction between the factors species and scents (Table 2). Figure 1A shows that the species behaved differently when confronted with their own scents. Liolaemus coeruleus moved less when exposed to its own scent than to scents of conspecifics (P = 0.030), heterospecifics (P = 0.041), and the control (P = 0.047). In contrast, L. ceii moved more when exposed to its own scent than to conspecific (P < 0.01), heterospecific (P < 0.01), or control scents (P < 0.01, see Fig. 1A).

Table 2.

Results of the two-way ANOVA for repeated measurements to determine the effect of species (Liolaemus coeruleus vs. L. ceii), scents (conspecific, heterospecific, control, and own), and their interactions (species * scents) over: latency to first tongue flick, number of tongue flicks, motion time (square–root-transformed), and time escaping. df, degrees of freedom, and F statistics (P value); in bold, the statistically significant results (P < 0.05).

img-z4-2_79.gif

The number of tongue flicks was affected by the two factors analyzed and their interaction (Table 2). First, Liolaemus coeruleus exhibited significantly fewer tongue flicks than L. ceii (Fig. 1B). Second, lizards, independent of the species, made more tongue flicks to conspecific scents than to any other scents. Third, the interaction between species and scents showed that individuals of L. coeruleus made fewer tongue flicks when exposed to their own scents than to conspecific (P = 0.012), heterospecific (P = 0.037), or control scents (P = 0.048). In contrast, individuals of L. ceii made more tongue flicks when they were exposed to scents of conspecifics than to heterospecifics (P = 0.009) or to control scents (P = 0.006), but made a similar number of tongue flicks when exposed to scents of conspecifics and their own scents (P = 0.161; Fig. 1B).

Figure 1.

Mean + SE of (A) time in motion and (B) tongue flicks recorded in the four treatments (scents: conspecific, heterospecific, control, and own) for Liolaemus coeruleus (black bars) and L. ceii (white bars). Insert shows the mean value (+ SE) of time in motion and tongue flicks exhibited by each species in the different treatments. Different letters and asterisks indicate significant differences (P < 0.05); lower and upper case letters indicate intraspecific comparison for L. coeruleus and L. ceii, respectively. Untransformed data of time in motion are presented.

img-z4-9_79.jpg

The frequency of displays was similar across scents in Liolaemus coeruleus (χ216(3) = 3.83; P = 0.282; Fig. 2A) and L. ceii (χ211(3) = 6.38; P = 0.095; Fig. 2B). However, the analysis by sex showed that males of L. coeruleus (Fig. 3) displayed differently with the diverse scents (χ27(3) = 7.96; P = 0.047); males exposed to heterospecific scents displayed more than when exposed to their own scents (P = 0.043; Fig. 3); specifically, they exhibited tail waving displays. No other comparison was statistically significant (P > 0.05). Females did not exhibit behavioral differences across scents (χ29(3) = 7.094; P = 0.068). In the case of L. ceii, males displayed similarly across the different scents (χ26(3) = 5.09; P = 0.173), as well as females (χ25(3) = 4.00; P = 0.260).

DISCUSSION

In the Patagonian steppe of Neuquén, we found that adults of Liolaemus coeruleus and juveniles of L. ceii shared retreat sites. Our experiments suggest that this sharing is not modulated by the presence of heterospecific scents; lizards did not exhibit behavioral evidence of chemical recognition of heterospecific scents. There was only very weak evidence that males of L. coeruleus discriminate between heterospecific and own scents, given that they exhibited more tail waving when confronting scents of males of L. ceii. Under the scenario of an interspecific confrontation, the tail movement might signal dominance (Fox et al., 1990), as has been observed during agonistic interactions involving aggressive displays using tail movements (Peters et al., 2016).

Figure 2.

Frequency of the four behaviors included in the “displays” variable (head-bob displays, forelimb waves, tail wave, and marking behavior) exhibited in the four treatments (scents: conspecific, heterospecific, control, and own). (A) Liolaemus coeruleus. (B) L. ceii. N = number of individuals that displayed; n = total number of displays exhibited during treatment.

img-z5-1_79.jpg

Figure 3.

Mean + SE of displays observed in the four treatments (scents: conspecific, heterospecific, control, and own) by both sexes of Liolaemus coeruleus.

img-z5-3_79.jpg

In Liolaemus, chemical recognition has usually been established on the basis of variations in time in motion and number of tongue flicks in the presence of different scents, and thus a behavioral variation among scents suggests scent discrimination (e.g., Labra, 2008b). Under this paradigm, it has been proposed that Liolaemus species discriminate heterospecific scents, prey (Mora and Labra, 2017; Ruiz-Monachesi and Valdecantos, 2017), predators (Troncoso-Palacios and Labra, 2012; Labra and Hoare, 2015), and congenerics (Labra, 2011). In contrast, L. coeruleus and L. ceii exhibited similar exploratory behaviors (time in motion and tongue flicks) when confront scents of heterospecifics and the control. This apparent lack of heterospecific (congeneric) scent recognition could be a consequence of two, non-mutually exclusive factors: 1) Heterospecifics scents constitute an irrelevant stimulus and do not trigger any major response (Paulissen, 2006), even if scents are perceived. 2) There are seasonal changes that modulate the discrimination behaviors. Such seasonal changes can entail different compounds and amounts of secretions (Alberts et al., 1992; Martins et al., 2006), which can be used as cues by heterospecific individuals to assess the presence of the other species. Further, these two species might experience seasonal changes in chemical recognition, as reported in other Liolaemus species that exhibit less discrimination after reproduction or before hibernation (Labra et al., 2001; Labra et al., 2003; Labra, 2008b; Vicente and Halloy, 2016). Therefore, considering that our study was carried out during the post-reproductive and pre-hibernation season, seasonal changes in recognition might explain the lack of heterospecific recognition, as occurs, for example, in snake scent detectability, which varies across seasons (Hayes et al., 2006).

The lack of a clear evidence of heterospecific recognition by these two Patagonian lizard species do not imply the absence of chemo-recognition, as both species exhibited self-recognition, such as other Liolaemus species do (Labra, 2008a,b; Troncoso-Palacios and Labra, 2012; Labra and Hoare, 2015; Vicente and Halloy, 2018). Self-recognition is considered the simplest and most basal type of chemical recognition (Alberts, 1992), and in the studied species, this ability might allow individuals to recognize their own space or the areas normally visited by them, potentially the retreat sites.

We found that Liolaemus coeruleus showed lower exploration towards own scents (i.e., less time in motion and fewer tongue flicks) than other scents, which indicates that this species exhibits self-recognition (e.g., Labra, 2008a). This is remarkable, because L. coeruleus lacks precloacal glands (Cei and Ortiz-Zapata, 1983), and it has been proposed that the lack of pheromonal glands would be associated with a low ability to use scents (e.g., Phrynosomatidae Fitzinger, 1843, Hews and Benard, 2001; Lacertidae Gray, 1825, Baeckens et al., 2015). Although precloacal glands produce chemical secretions (Valdecantos et al., 2014) with pheromonal properties (Labra et al., 2005; Valdecantos and Labra, 2017), there are other sources of scents in Liolaemus species, including feces (Labra et al., 2002), skin derivates (Labra, 2008a) and, presumably, substances produced by proctodeal glands in males (Valdecantos et al., 2015) and urodeal glands in females (Sánchez-Martinez et al., 2007).

Juveniles of Liolaemus ceii also show evidence of self-recognition, but in contrast to the low level of exploration with their own scents exhibited by adults of L. coeruleus and other Liolaemus species (e.g., Labra, 2008a), they were more active (motion time) when exposed to their own scents. Additionally, juveniles exhibited more tongue flicks in the presence of conspecific and own scents than in the presence of unknown scents (heterospecific and control). This suggests that species-specific scents (i.e., conspecific and own) are a relevant stimulus for the juveniles of L. ceii. Liolaemus bellii Gray, 1845 undergoes ontogenetic changes in response to own and control scents, whereby neonates, juveniles, and adults differed in the pattern of self-chemical recognition, which was proposed as a learning process to the own scents (Labra et al., 2003). Potentially, the greater environmental exploration performed by the juveniles of L. ceii (i.e., longer time in motion and more tongue flicks to own and conspecific scents) might also be part of a learning process that helps juveniles to consolidate the memory of the “species” and its own scents. It is necessary, however, to clarify whether adults of L. ceii exhibit the same behavioral pattern as juveniles or if, in fact, the behavior of these juveniles denotes ontogenetic changes in chemical recognition.

Regardless of whether or not sharing of retreat sites by these Patagonian species is modulated by the presence of heterospecific scents, individuals of both species might benefit from this behavior. The Patagonian steppe is characterized by extremely cold temperatures with high winds (Paruelo et al., 1998), and suitable thermal refuges are limited (Aguilar and Cruz, 2010). Sharing retreat sites might provide lizards with the thermal benefits of huddling behavior, which would help them maintain a stable body temperature, as Shah et al., (2003) postulated to explain aggregation in the gecko Nephrurus milii (Bory de Saint-Vincent, 1825). Moreover, considering that adult Liolaemus coeruleus and juvenile L. ceii are similar in size, sharing retreat sites might reduce predation risk via the dilution effect (Mouton, 2011). In the area where these Patagonian species occur, some predators use the same type of retreat sites as these two lizard species, such as Diplolaemus Bell, 1843 lizards (Garcia et al., 2015) and Brachistosternum Pocock, 1893 scorpions (e.g., Pérez et al., 2010). Potentially, the retreat site selection by these species of Liolaemus might be more affected by predator scents (e.g., Stapey, 2003; Amo et al., 2004; Lloyd et al., 2009), particularly considering that other Liolaemus species are known to respond to predator scents (Labra and Niemeyer, 2004; Troncoso-Palacios and Labra, 2012; Labra and Hoare, 2015). We postulate that for these two Liolaemus species, predator scents might be a more relevant stimulus to select a retreat site than those from non-risky congeners.

In summary, the evidence gathered here does not support the hypothesis that sharing of retreat sites by adult Liolaemus coeruleus and juvenile L. ceii is mediated by the scent of these congeneric species during the pre-hibernation season. The absence of heterospecific recognition, however, does not imply an inability to chemo-assess scents, as L. ceii showed conspecific recognition and both species showed self-recognition.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Authors thank T. Hibbard, M. Paz, and M. Quipildor for their help in the field, and two anonymous reviewers for their suggestions on our manuscript. MRM thanks CONICET Scholarship. Funds came from CIUNSA 2241 (S. Valdecantos), PIP CONICET 0303 (F. Lobo), and PICT 2015-2471(F. Cruz). Animals were collected with the permit № 4351-0026/2014 (F. Lobo). The study was conducted in accordance with international standards on animal welfare, and it was compliant with national regulations and the “Comité Nacional de Ética en la Ciencia y la Tecnología” of Argentina (Expte. 5344/99 Res. 1047). The euthanasia procedure was approved by the ethical committee of animal use of Instituto de Bio y Geociencias del NOA (IBIGEO).

REFERENCES

1.

Aguilar R., Cruz F.B. 2010. Refuge use in a Patagonian nocturnal lizard, Homonota darwini: the role of temperature. Journal of Herpetology 44:236–241.  DOI  Google Scholar

2.

Alberts A.C. 1992. Pheromonal self-recognition in desert iguanas. Copeia 1992:229–232.  DOI  Google Scholar

3.

Alberts A.C., Sharp T.R., Werner D.I., Weldon P.J. 1992. Seasonal variation of lipids in femoral gland secretions of male green iguanas (Iguana iguana). Journal of Chemical Ecology 18:703–712.  DOI  Google Scholar

4.

Amo L., López P., Martín J. 2004. Thermal dependence of chemical assessment of predation risk affects the ability of wall lizards, Podarcis muralis, to avoid unsafe refuges. Physiology & Behavior 82:913–918.  DOI  Google Scholar

5.

Auclair Y., König B., Lindholm A.K. 2014. Socially mediated polyandry: a new benefit of communal nesting in mammals. Behavioral Ecology 25:1467–1473.  DOI  Google Scholar

6.

Baeckens S., Edwards S., Huyghe K., Van Damme R. 2015. Chemical signalling in lizards: an interspecific comparison of femoral pore numbers in Lacertidae. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 114:44–57.  DOI  Google Scholar

7.

Bell T. 1843. The Zoology of the Voyage of H.M.S. Beagle, Under the Command of Captain Fitzroy, R.N., During the Years 1832 to 1836. Part 5. Reptiles. Smith, Elder and Co., London.  DOI  Google Scholar

8.

Bertram B.C. 2014. The Ostrich Communal Nesting System. Princeton University Press, Princeton. Google Scholar

9.

Bishop D.C., Echternacht A.C. 2004. Emergence behavior and movements of winter-aggregated green anoles (Anolis carolinensis) and the thermal characteristics of their crevices in Tennessee. Herpetologica 60:168–177.  DOI  Google Scholar

10.

Bory de Saint-Vincent J.B.G.M. 1825. Dictionnaire Classique D'Histoire Naturelle. Tome Septième. Rey et Gravier, Paris.  DOI  Google Scholar

11.

Bull C.M., Griffin C.L., Lanham E.J., Johnston G.R. 2000. Recognition of pheromones from group members in a gregarious lizard, Egernia stokesii . Journal of Herpetology 34:92–99.  DOI  Google Scholar

12.

Carazo P., Font E., Desfilis E. 2011. The role of scent marks in female choice of territories and refuges in a lizard (Podarcis hispanica). Journal of Comparative Psychology 125:362–365.  DOI  Google Scholar

13.

Caruso N.M. 2016. Surface retreats used among four genera of terrestrial salamanders in the great smoky mountains national park. Journal of Herpetology 50:87–93.  DOI  Google Scholar

14.

Cei J.M., Ortiz J.C. 1983. Descripción de una nueva especie de lagarto Liolaemus coeruleus n. sp. para Argentina (Sauria, Iguanidae). Boletín de la Sociedad de Biología de Concepción 54:35–41. Google Scholar

15.

Cooper W.E. Jr., van Wyk J.H., Mouton Le F.P., Mouton N. 1999. Incompletely protective refuges: selection and associated defences by a lizard, Cordylus cordylus (Squamata: Cordylidae). Ethology 105:687–700.  DOI  Google Scholar

16.

Croak B.M., Pike D.A., Webb J.K., Shine R. 2008. Three-dimensional crevice structure affects retreat site selection by reptiles. Animal Behaviour 76:1875–1884.  DOI  Google Scholar

17.

Dayananda B., Gray S., Pike D., Webb J.K. 2016. Communal nesting under climate change: fitness consequences of higher incubation temperatures for a nocturnal lizard. Global Change Biology 22:2405–2414.  DOI  Google Scholar

18.

Donoso-Barros R. 1971. A new Liolaemus from Neuquén (Argentina). Herpetologica 27:49–51. Google Scholar

19.

Downes S., Shine R. 1998. Heat, safety or solitude? Using habitat selection experiments to identify a lizard's priorities. Animal Behaviour 55:1387–1396.  DOI  Google Scholar

20.

Duméril A., Bibron G. 1836. Erpétologie Générale ou Histoire Naturelle Complète des Reptiles. Tome Troisième. Librarie Encyclopédique Roret, Paris.  DOI  Google Scholar

21.

Elfström B.E.O., Zucker N. 1999. Winter aggregation and its relationship to social status in the tree lizard, Urosaurus ornatus . Journal of Herpetology 33:240–248.  DOI  Google Scholar

22.

Fitzinger L.J. 1843. Systema Reptilium. Fasciculus Primus. Braumüller et Seidel, Wien.  DOI  Google Scholar

23.

Font E., Desfilis E. 2002. Chemosensory recognition of familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics by juveniles of the Iberian wall lizard Podarcis hispanica . Ethology 108:319–330.  DOI  Google Scholar

24.

Font E., Carazo P., Pérez i de Lanuza G., Kramer M. 2012. Predator-elicited foot shakes in wall lizards (Podarcis muralis): evidence for a pursuit-deterrent function. Journal of Comparative Psychology 1:87–96.  DOI  Google Scholar

25.

Fox S.F., Heger N.A., Delay L.S. 1990. Social cost of tail loss in Uta stansburiana: lizard tails as status-signalling badges. Animal Behaviour 39:549–554.  DOI  Google Scholar

26.

Garcia N.E., Ruiz Monachesi M.R., Paz M.M. 2015. Primer registro de Mesocestoides sp. (Ciclophyllidea: Cestoda) en Diplolaemus leopardinus (Werner, 1898). Cuadernos de Herpetología 29:81–83. Google Scholar

27.

Gray J.E. 1825. A synopsis of the genera of reptiles and Amphibia, with a description of some new species. Annals of Philosophy Series 2 10:193–217. Google Scholar

28.

Gray J.E. 1845. Catalogue of the Specimens of Lizards in the Collection of the British Museum. Order of the Trustees, London.  DOI  Google Scholar

29.

Halloy M., Castillo M. 2002. Forelimb wave displays in lizard species of Liolaemus (Iguania: Liolaemidae). Herpetological Natural History 9:127–133. Google Scholar

30.

Hayes R.A., Nahrung H.F., Wilson J.C. 2006. The response of native Australian rodents to predator odours varies seasonally: a by-product of life history variation? Animal Behaviour 71:1307–1314.  DOI  Google Scholar

31.

Hayward J., Mouton P.L.F.N. 2007. Group location in the group-living lizard, Cordylus cataphractus: the significance of occupancy and a group signal. Amphibia-Reptilia 28:329–335.  DOI  Google Scholar

32.

Hews D.K., Benard M.F. 2001. Negative association between conspicuous visual display and chemosensory behavior in two phrynosomatid lizards. Ethology 107:839–850.  DOI  Google Scholar

33.

Hibbitts T.J., Whiting M.J. 2005. Do male barking geckos (Ptenopus garrulus garrulus) avoid refuges scented by other males? African Journal of Herpetology 54:191–194.  DOI  Google Scholar

34.

Kondo J., Downes S.J., Keogh S.J. 2007. Recent physical encounters affect chemically mediated retreat-site selection in a gecko. Ethology 113:68–75.  DOI  Google Scholar

35.

Kroon F.J., de Graaf M., Liley N.R. 2000. Social organisation and competition for refuges and nest sites in Coryphopterus nicholsii (Gobiidae), a temperate protogynous reef fish. Environmental Biology of Fishes 57:401–411.  DOI  Google Scholar

36.

Labra A. 2006. Chemoreception and the assessment of fighting abilities in the lizard Liolaemus monticola . Ethology 112:993–999.  DOI  Google Scholar

37.

Labra A. 2008a. Sistemas de comunicación en reptiles. Pp. 447–577, in Vidal M.A., Labra A. (Eds), Herpetología de Chile. Science Verlag, Santiago. Google Scholar

38.

Labra A. 2008b Multi-contextual use of chemosignals by Liolaemus lizards. Pp. 357–365, in Hurst J.L., Beynon R.J., Roberts S.C., Wyatt T.D. (Eds.), Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 11. SpringerLink, New York.  DOI  Google Scholar

39.

Labra A. 2011. Chemical stimuli and species recognition in Liolaemus lizards. Journal of Zoology 285:215–221.  DOI  Google Scholar

40.

Labra A., Hoare M. 2015. Chemical recognition in a snake-lizard predator-prey system. Actha Ethologia 18:173–179.  DOI  Google Scholar

41.

Labra A., Niemeyer H.M. 1999. Intraspecific chemical recognition in the lizard Liolaemus tenuis . Journal of Chemical Ecology 25:1799–1811.  DOI  Google Scholar

42.

Labra A., Niemeyer H.M. 2004. Variability in the assessment of snake predation risk by Liolaemus lizards. Ethology 110:649–662.  DOI  Google Scholar

43.

Labra A., Beltrán S., Niemeyer H.M. 2001. Chemical exploratory behavior in the lizard Liolaemus bellii . Journal of Herpetology 35:51–55.  DOI  Google Scholar

44.

Labra A., Escobar C.A., Aguilar P.M., Niemeyer H.M. 2002. Sources of pheromones in the lizard Liolaemus tenuis . Revista Chilena de Historia Natural 75:141–147.  DOI  Google Scholar

45.

Labra A., Cortéz S., Niemeyer H.M. 2003. Age and season affect chemical discrimination of Liolaemus bellii own space. Journal of Chemical Ecology 29:2615–2620.  DOI  Google Scholar

46.

Labra A., Brann J.H., Fadool D.A. 2005. Heterogeneity of voltage- and chemosignal-activated response profiles in vomeronasal sensory neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology 94:2535–2548.  DOI  Google Scholar

47.

Labra A., Carazo P., Desfilis E., Font E. 2007. Agonistic interactions in a Liolaemus lizard: structure of head bob displays. Herpetologica 63:11–18.  DOI  Google Scholar

48.

Langerkilde T., Shine R. 2005. How do water skinks avoid shelters already occupied by other lizards? Behaviour 142:203–216.  DOI  Google Scholar

49.

Lewis B.A., Whiting M.J., Stapley J. 2007. Male flat lizards prefer females with novel scents. African Zoology 42:91–96.  DOI  Google Scholar

50.

Linnaeus C. 1758. Systema naturae per regna tria naturae, secundum classes, ordines, genera, species, cum characteribus, differential, synonymis, locis, Tomus I. Editio decima, reformata. Laurentiis Salvii, Holmiae.  DOI  Google Scholar

51.

Lloyd R., Alford R.A., Schwarzkopf L. 2009. Chemical discrimination among predators by lizards: responses of three skink species to the odours of high- and low-threat varanid predators. Austral Ecology 34:50–54.  DOI  Google Scholar

52.

Martins E.P., Ord T.J., Slaven J., Wright J.L., Housworth E.A. 2006. Individual, sexual, seasonal, and temporal variation in the amount of sagebrush lizard scent marks. Journal of Chemical Ecology 32:881–893.  DOI  Google Scholar

53.

Marvin G.A. 1998. Interspecific aggression and spatial relationships in the salamanders Plethodon kentucki and Plethodon glutinosus: evidence of interspecific interference competition. Canadian Journal of Zoology 76:94–103.  DOI  Google Scholar

54.

Mateo J.A., Cuadrado M. 2012. Communal nesting and parental care in Oudri's fan-footed gecko (Ptyodactylus oudrii): field and experimental evidence of an adaptive behavior. Journal of Herpetology 46:209–212.  DOI  Google Scholar

55.

Mora M., Labra A. 2017. The response of two Liolaemus lizard species to ash from fire and volcanism. Journal of Herpetology 51:388–395.  DOI  Google Scholar

56.

Mori E., Menchetti M., Balestrieri A. 2015. Interspecific den sharing: a study on European badger setts using camera traps. Acta Ethologica 18:121–126.  DOI  Google Scholar

57.

Mouton P.L.F.N. 2011. Aggregation behaviour of lizards in the arid western regions of South Africa. African Journal of Herpetology 60:155–170.  DOI  Google Scholar

58.

Ortega Z., Mencía A., Pérez-Mellado V. 2017. Rapid acquisition of antipredatory responses to new predators by an insular lizard. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 71:1.  DOI  Google Scholar

59.

Paruelo J.M., Beltrán A., Jobbágy E., Sala O.E., Golluscio R.A. 1998. The climate of Patagonia: general patterns and controls on biotic processes. Ecología Austral 8:85–101. Google Scholar

60.

Paulissen M.A. 2006. The use of odors in burrow selection by the whiptail lizard Aspidoscelis laredoensis B (Squamata: Teiidae). Texas Journal of Science 58:147–154. Google Scholar

61.

Penado A., Rocha R., Sampaio M., Gil V., Carreira B.M., Rebelo R. 2015. Where to “rock”? Choice of retreat sites by a gecko in a semiarid habitat. Acta Herpetologica 10:47–54.  DOI  Google Scholar

62.

Pérez C.H.F., Ávila L.J., Camargo A. 2010. Predation of Liolaemus huacahuasicus (Squamata: Iguania: Liolaemini) by Brachistosternus intermedius (Scorpiones: Bothriuridae) in cumbres Calchaquíes, Tucumán province, northwestern Argentina. Cuadernos de Herpetología 24:123–124. Google Scholar

63.

Peters R.A., Ramos J.A., Hernandez J., Wu Y., Qi Y. 2016. Social context affects tail displays by Phrynocephalus vlangalii lizards from China. Scientific Reports 6:31573.  DOI  Google Scholar

64.

Pocock R.I. 1893. Contributions to our knowledge of the arthropod fauna of the West Indies.—Part I. Scorpiones and pedipalpi; with a supplementary note upon the freshwater decapoda of St. Vincent. Journal of the Linnean Society of London, Zoology 24:374–409.  DOI  Google Scholar

65.

Ruiz-Monachesi M.R., Valdecantos M.S. 2017. Chemical recognition of prey in Liolaemus ceii (Donoso-Barros, 1971). Herpetology Notes 10:327–328. Google Scholar

66.

Rutherford P.L., Gregory P.T. 2003. How age, sex, and reproductive condition affect retreat-site selection and emergence patterns in a temperate-zone lizard, Elgaria coerulea . Ecoscience 10:24–32.  DOI  Google Scholar

67.

Sánchez-Martínez P.M., Ramiréz-Pinilla M.P., Miranda-Esquivel D.R. 2007. Comparative histology of the vaginal-cloacal region in Squamata and its phylogenetic implications. Acta Zoologica 88:289–307.  DOI  Google Scholar

68.

Schlesinger C.A., Shine R. 1994. Selection of diurnal retreat sites by the nocturnal gekkonid lizard Oedura lesueurii . Herpetologica 50:156–163. Google Scholar

69.

Scrocchi G., Kretzschmar S. 1996. Guía de Métodos de Captura y Preparación de Anfibios y Reptiles para Estudios Científicos y Manejo de Colecciones Herpetológicas. Fundación Miguel Lillo Tucuman, Tucumán, Argentina. Google Scholar

70.

Shah B., Shine R., Hudson S., Kearney M. 2003. Sociality in lizards: why do thick-tailed geckos (Nephrurus milii) aggregate? Behaviour 140:1039–1052.  DOI  Google Scholar

71.

Stapey J. 2003. Differential avoidance of snake odours by a lizard: evidence for prioritized avoidance based on risk. Ethology 109:785–796.  DOI  Google Scholar

72.

Steindachner F. 1870. Herpetologische Notizen (II). Reptilien gesammelt während einer Reise in Senegambien. Sitzungsberichte der Kaiserlichen Akademie der Wissenschaften. Mathematisch-Naturwissenschaftliche Classe 62:326–350. Google Scholar

73.

Troncoso-Palacios J., Labra A. 2012. Is the exploratory behavior of Liolaemus nitidus modulated by sex? Acta Herpetologica 7:69–80.  DOI  Google Scholar

74.

Valdecantos S., Labra A. 2017. Testing the functionality of precloacal secretions from both sexes in the South American lizard, Liolaemus chiliensis . Amphibia-Reptilia 38:209–216.  DOI  Google Scholar

75.

Valdecantos S., Martínez V., Labra A. 2014. Comparative morphology of Liolaemus lizards precloacal glands. Acta Herpetologica 9:147–158.  DOI  Google Scholar

76.

Valdecantos S., Martínez V., Labra A. 2015. Description of a proctodeal gland of three South American Liolaemus lizards (Iguania: Liolaemidae). Salamandra 51:182–186. Google Scholar

77.

van den Berg F.T., Thompson M.B., Hochuli D.F. 2015. When hot rocks get hotter: behavior and acclimatization mitigate exposure to extreme temperatures in a spider. Ecosphere 6:1–17.  DOI  Google Scholar

78.

Vasconcelos R., Rocha S., Santos X. 2017. Sharing refuges on arid islands: ecological and social influence on aggregation behaviour of wall geckos. PeerJ 5:e2802.  DOI  Google Scholar

79.

Vicente N.S., Halloy M. 2016. Chemical recognition of conspecifics in a neotropical lizard, Liolaemus pacha (Iguania: Liolaemidae): relation to visual displays, season and sex. Journal of Ethology 34:329–335.  DOI  Google Scholar

80.

Vicente N.S., Halloy M. 2018. El auto-reconocimiento químico en Liolaemus pacha (Iguania: Liolaemidae) está influenciado por la estación. Cuadernos de Herpetología 32:23–29.  DOI  Google Scholar

81.

VideoLan. 2006. VLC media player. Available from:  https://www.videolan.org/vlc/index.htmlGoogle Scholar

82.

Vitt L.J., Congdon J.D., Hulse A.C., Platz J.E. 1974. Territorial aggressive encounters and tail breaks in the lizard Sceloporus magister . Copeia 1974:990–993.  DOI  Google Scholar

83.

Weintraub J.D. 1968. Winter behavior of the granite spiny lizard, Sceloporus orcutti Stejneger. Copeia 1968:708–712.  DOI  Google Scholar

84.

Wiegmann A.F.A. 1834. Systematis saurorum prodromus, e specimene herpetologiae mexicanae primo seorsim editus. C.G. Lüderitz, Berolini. Google Scholar
© 2020 Brazilian Society of Herpetology
Mario R. Ruiz-Monachesi, Soledad Valdecantos, Fernando Lobo, Félix B. Cruz, and Antonieta Labra "Retreat Sites Shared by Two Liolaemus Lizard Species: Exploring the Potential Role of Scents," South American Journal of Herpetology 17(1), 79-86, (31 August 2020). https://doi.org/10.2994/SAJH-D-18-00051.1
Received: 17 July 2018; Accepted: 29 November 2018; Published: 31 August 2020
KEYWORDS
Conspecific recognition
Liolaemus ceii
Liolaemus coeruleus
self-recognition
Tongue flicks
Back to Top