Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 June 2015 Species Identity and Nest Location Predict Agonistic Interactions at a Breeding Colony of Double-Crested Cormorants (Phalacrocorax auritus) and Great Blue Herons (Ardea herodias)
Katherine E. Wyman, Francesca J. Cuthbert
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Recent dramatic growth of the North American Interior population of Double-crested Cormorants (Phalacrocorax auritus) has led to concern about potential impacts of this species on co-nesting colonial waterbirds. Previous investigations of these concerns have focused on Double-crested Cormorant interactions with other species within homogeneous breeding environments, making broad patterns difficult to identify. The present study examined how nest location, nest density, and species identity mediate agonistic interactions among Double-crested Cormorants and Great Blue Herons (Ardea herodias) nesting at a colony site in central Minnesota, USA. Twenty-six Double-crested Cormorant nests and 27 Great Blue Heron nests were observed for 30 min weekly for 8 weeks during the breeding season to estimate frequency of agonistic behavior and identify species-level patterns of interaction. Most agonistic interactions observed (81%) were intraspecific interactions among Double-crested Cormorants; Great Blue Herons engaged in a higher combined total count of interspecific and intraspecific interactions when they nested near Double-crested Cormorants. Interspecific interactions were more common among ground-nesting birds than among tree-nesting birds. This study suggests that further investigation into impacts of Double-crested Cormorants on co-nesting birds is most warranted for ground-nesting colonies.

Waterbird colonies provide breeding birds with an important resource: nest sites. Multiple species of colonial waterbirds occupy similar types of nest sites (Burger 1979; Pius and Leberg 1997; Weseloh et al. 2002), which can lead to interspecific competition for these sites (Burger 1978; Pius and Leberg 1997), often manifested through agonistic behavior (Brown and Orians 1970). Intense interspecific territoriality may reduce individual reproductive success (Burger 1978; Duckworth 2006). Accordingly, growth of the North American Interior population of Double-crested Cormorants (Phalacrocorax auritus; hereafter, “cormorants”) since the 1970s (Weseloh et al. 1995; Wires and Cuthbert 2006) has led to concern that cormorants will reduce reproductive success of co-nesting colonial waterbird species through nest-site competition (Weseloh et al. 2002). Cormorant management is authorized at some locations in the USA specifically to protect other waterbird species believed to be vulnerable to cormorant presence (U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service 2014).

Evidence for negative effects of cormorants on co-nesting waterbirds remains inconclusive. Wading bird abundance has declined at some breeding sites shared with cormorants, but researchers have been unable to identify interspecific competition as the definitive cause of these declines (Skagen et al. 2001; Cuthbert et al. 2002). Studies of ground-nesting colonies have shown conflicting results on the impacts of cormorants on co-nesters (Somers et al. 2007, 2011). For example, Herring Gulls (Larus argentatus) engaged in more agonistic (combined inter- and intraspecific) interactions when nesting among cormorants (Somers et al. 2007), while American White Pelicans (Pelecanus erythrorhynchos) engaged in fewer agonistic interactions when nesting among cormorants (Somers et al. 2011), compared to nesting among conspecifics exclusively.

Somers et al. (2011) posited that the observed variability of responses to co-nesting with cormorants could be attributable to habitat structure, nest density, or the identity of the species involved. Because cormorants breed both on the ground and in trees (Hatch and Weseloh 1999) and often nest near other species (Cuthbert and Wires 2013), there is considerable variation among colonies in each of these variables. The behavioral literature supports all three hypotheses: habitat structure (Bukacinska and Bukacinski 1993; Jensen et al. 2005; Barley and Coleman 2010); nest or territory density (Butler and Trivelpiece 1981; Stokes and Boersma 2000); and/or species identity (Burger 1978; Mott and Maret 2011).

Our study examined the Somers et al. (2011) hypotheses that three potential factors (habitat structure, nest density, and species identity) drive the rate of agonistic interactions in a mixed-species breeding colony. At our study site in Meeker County, Minnesota, cormorants and Great Blue Herons (Ardea herodias; hereafter, “herons”) nested on the ground and in trees across two islands. Thus, individual cormorants and conesters could be observed in a range of nest microenvironments all subject to the same broad environmental conditions.

Methods

Study Area

Interactions between cormorants and herons were observed at Pigeon Lake, Meeker County, Minnesota (45° 02′ 24″ N, 94° 20′ 53″ W). Three islands in this lake have been used for breeding by both species, as well as by American White Pelicans, Great Egrets (A. alba) and Black-crowned Night-Herons (Nycticorax nycticorax, Wires et al. 2006). In 2012, aerial photographs and a ground count of American White Pelican nests provided estimates of nest abundance at the island complex: Great Blue Heron (159 nests), Great Egret (8 nests), Double-crested Cormorant (3,309 nests), and American White Pelican (191 nests; L. Wires, pers. commun.).

Observations

We observed focal nests of herons and cormorants from 16 April to 11 June 2012. Of the species breeding at the study site, these species were the only two whose nests were both easily visible and abundant. In mid-April, cormorants were completing the nest-building phase of the breeding period and most herons were incubating eggs. Therefore, the colony site was settled before observations began. Focal nests were selected by assigning numbers to all visible nests in each combination of nesting location (tree or ground) and species (heron or cormorant), then randomly selecting 13 nests out of tire total available in each category. Selected nests were observed weekly for one 30-min period between 06:55 hr and 18:15 hr with a 20–60x spotting telescope from a roadside overlook. One focal nest of ground-nesting herons failed mid-season and tire selection procedure was repeated to choose a replacement from among tire ground-nesting heron nests not currently under study. Two focal nests of tree-nesting cormorants failed in the last 2 weeks of observations and were not replaced, resulting in a total sample size of 53 nests. For each focal nest, nest density was defined on a scale from one to five, corresponding to the number of contiguous territories situated around the nest and/ or above it (Butler and Trivelpiece 1981). Values were estimated from aerial and ground photographs of the colony because quarantine of the site following a New-castle disease outbreak prevented us from measuring nest density directly.

Agonistic behaviors (Table 1) were identified according to published ethograms for each species (van Tets 1965; Mock 1976). Both interspecific and intraspecific agonistic interactions were recorded, along with species identities of the birds involved. Potential effects of time of day on interaction rate were minimized by alternating species and nest locations throughout the day (e.g., a ground-nesting heron observation would be followed by a tree-nesting cormorant, then a treenesting heron, then a ground-nesting cormorant) and by observing nests in the same order from week to week. Individuals were not marked and could not be reliably distinguished on the basis of plumage or size, so tire smallest unit of study was the nest. “When both members of a nesting pair were present at tire nest, only the incubating or brooding bird's interactions were recorded to promote consistency with nests where only one adult was present.

Statistical Analyses

Generalized linear mixed models were constructed representing the study's alternative hypotheses (i.e., species identity, nest density, and/or nest location influence frequency of agonistic interactions; Table 2). Two response variables were considered: 1) total number of agonistic interactions (both interspecific and intraspecific) involving the focal nest within a 30-min observation period; and 2) number of interspecific agonistic interactions involving the focal nest within a 30-min observation period. Each model contained covariates controlling for date of observation, time of day of observation, and proportion of the four nearest neighboring nests occupied by heterospecific individuals. All these factors may influence rates of aggression (Burger 1984; Bukacinska and Bukacinski 1994; Pius and Leberg 1997; Somers et al. 2011). Models of total counts of interspecific and intraspecific interactions including species as a covariate also included an interaction term for species and proportion of heterospecific neighbors. A random intercept term accounted for non-independence of observations made on the same nest (Zuur et al. 2009).

Table 1.

Descriptions of agonistic behaviors observed in Double-crested Cormorants and Great Blue Herons.

t01_201.gif

Model fitting and selection were performed in program R (R Development Core Team 2012) using the ‘glmmADMB’ package (Fournier et al. 2012; Skaug et al. 2014). The first step of model selection determined appropriate error structure for the full model, considering four types of regression models: Poisson, zero-inflated Poisson, negative binomial, and zero-inflated negative binomial. Negative binomial models provided the best fit to the full model for both response variables and were used for selection among reduced models representing alternative hypotheses (Table 2). Model selection was accomplished using Akaike's Information Criterion (AIC; Burnham and Anderson 2002). In Tables 3 and 4, we only report models with a difference of AIC ≤12 compared to the top model.

Table 2.

Candidate model set used to predict two response variables: the total number of interspecific plus intraspecific agonistic interactions involving the focal nest in a 30-min observation period, and count of interspecific agonistic interactions involving the focal nest in a 30-min observation period.

t02_201.gif

Table 3.

Top models of total number of interspecific plus intraspecific interactions involving a focal nest per 30-min observation period, ranked according to Akaike's Information Criterion (AIC). K indicates number of model parameters, ΔAIC indicates difference in AIC compared with highest ranking model, and wi indicates AIC weight of model.

t03_201.gif

Results

During 412 30-min observation periods, we observed 436 agonistic interactions between nesting individuals at Pigeon Lake. Interspecific interactions between cormorants and herons occurred in 54 observation periods (13.1%. of all periods). Intraspecific interactions occurred in six (2.9%) heron observation periods and in 104 (50.7%.) cormorant observation periods. Most interactions of either type occurred between the focal nest and occupants of neighboring nests. No interactions occurred in 60%. of observation periods.

Figure 1.

Predicted number of combined interspecific and intraspecific agonistic interactions (± SE) in 30 min for Double-crested Cormorants (“cormorant”) and Great Blue Herons (“heron”), by nest location (ground or tree) and proportion of four nearest neighbors that are heterospecifics. Predictions were calculated according to the top model in the candidate set, with date and time fixed at their medians.

f01_201.jpg

Species identity and nest location were more important predictors of total number of agonistic interactions (interspecific and intraspecific combined) than nest density (Table 3). Great Blue Herons engaged in fewer total interactions than Double-crested Cormorants in the same location, while tree-nesting birds engaged in fewer total interactions than ground-nesting birds (Fig. 1). Proportion of heterospecific neighbors also influenced total number of agonistic interactions: herons nesting among conspecifics had fewer total interactions than herons nesting among cormorants, while cormorants nesting among conspecifics had more total interactions than cormorants nesting among herons (Fig. 1).

Nest location was a more important predictor of interspecific interaction rate than species identity or nest density (Table 4). Tree-nesting birds engaged in fewer interspecific interactions than ground-nesters. Heterospecific neighbors also had a strong positive effect on the number of interspecific agonistic interactions (Fig. 2).

Discussion

Our observations at the Pigeon Lake waterbird colony support the hypotheses proposed by Somers et al. (2011) that species identity and habitat structure are related to the rate of agonistic interactions among nesting individuals. Species identity of neighbors also had a strong influence on agonistic behavior; arrangement of nests within a colony matters as much as overall species composition.

Figure 2.

Predicted number of interspecific interactions (± SE) in 30 min involving a nest of either Double-crested Cormorants or Great Blue Herons, by nest location and proportion of four nearest neighbors that are heterospecifics. Predictions were calculated according to the top model in the candidate set, with date and time fixed at their medians.

f02_201.jpg

These results, along with previous research (Skagen et al. 2001; Cuthbert et al. 2002; Somers et al. 2007, 2011), suggest that some co-nesting species may be more affected by nesting among cormorants than others. Spatial structuring of nests within a colony is important; interaction rates can vary with nest location and species identity of neighbors. According to our study, rates of agonistic interaction will likely be highest among ground-nesters. Birds that interact little with conspecific neighbors in single-species colonies will likely engage in more total interactions in mixed-species colonies because of added interspecific interactions. At Pigeon Lake, heron interactions with conspecific neighbors were limited, so presence of cormorant neighbors increased the total number of interactions involving herons. Future research on interactions between cormorants and co-nesters should determine whether, and to what degree, co-nester reproductive success declines when co-nesters engage in agonistic interactions with cormorants.

Table 4.

Models of total number of interspecific agonistic interactions involving a focal nest per 30-min observation period, ranked according to Akaike's Information Criterion (AIC). K indicates number of model parameters, ΔAIC indicates difference in AIC compared with highest ranking model, and wi indicates AIC weight of model.

t04_201.gif

As cormorant population management has been implemented in the USA over the past 2 decades, cormorant interactions with other members of the ecological community have transitioned from a scientific to a political issue. Although this study suggests that Great Blue Heron behavior changes in the presence of co-nesting Double-crested Cormorants, it does not provide evidence either to support or discourage management activities. Other issues (e.g., reproductive impacts, potential impacts of control activities) must also be considered in decisions of whether and how to apply management to cormorant populations.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to L. Wires and D. V. C. Weseloh for sharing their knowledge of Pigeon Lake, and to A. Rothman and S. Weisberg for statistical advice. We also thank two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript. K. Wyman received support from a Sigma Xi Grant-in-Aid of Research and a Richard and Judi Huempfner Avian Research Fellowship (Bell Museum of Natural History). F. Cuthbert was funded by the Minnesota Agricultural Experiment Station.

Literature Cited

1.

A. J. Barley and R. M. Coleman . 2010. Habitat structure directly affects aggression in convict cichlids Archocentrus nigrofasciatus. Current Zoology 56: 52–56. Google Scholar

2.

J. L. Brown and G. H. Orians . 1970. Spacing patterns in mobile animals. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 1: 239–262. Google Scholar

3.

M. Bukacinska and D. Bukacinski . 1993. The effect of habitat structure and density of nests on territory size and territory behavior in the black-headed gull (Larus ridibundus L.). Ethology 94: 306–316. Google Scholar

4.

M. Bukacinska and D. Bukacinski . 1994. Seasonal and diurnal changes in aggression and territory size in the black-headed gull (Larus ridibundus L.) on islands in the middle reaches of the Vistula river. Ethology 97: 329–339. Google Scholar

5.

J. Burger 1978. Competition between Cattle Egrets and native North American herons, egrets, and ibises. Condor 80: 15–23. Google Scholar

6.

J. Burger 1979. Resource partitioning: nest site selection in mixed species colonies of herons, egrets and ibises. American Midland Naturalist 101: 191–210. Google Scholar

7.

J. Burger 1984. Pattern, mechanism, and adaptive significance of territoriality in Herring Gulls (Larus argentatus). Ornithological Monographs 34: 1–92. Google Scholar

8.

K. P. Burnham and D. R. Anderson . 2002. Model selection and multi-model inference: a practical information-theoretic approach, 2nd ed. Springer, New York, New York. Google Scholar

9.

R. G. Butler and W. Trivelpiece . 1981. Nest spacing, reproductive success, and behavior of the Great Black-backed Gull (Larus marinus). Auk 98: 99–107. Google Scholar

10.

F. J. Cuthbert and L. Wires . 2013. The fourth decadal U.S. Great Lakes colonial waterbird survey (2007– 2010): results and recommendations to improve the scientific basis for conservation and management. Unpublished report, U.S. Department of the Interior, Fish and Wildlife Service, Fort Snelling, Minnesota. Google Scholar

11.

F. J. Cuthbert , L. R. Wires and J. E. McKearnan . 2002. Potential impacts of nesting Double-crested Cormorants on Great Blue Herons and Black-crowned Night-herons in the U.S. Great Lakes region. Journal of Great Lakes Research 28: 145–154. Google Scholar

12.

R. Duckworth 2006. Behavioral correlations across breeding contexts provide a mechanism for a cost of aggression. Behavioral Ecology 17: 1011–1019. Google Scholar

13.

D. A. Fournier , H. J. Skaug , J. Ancheta , J. Ianelli , A. Magnusson , M. Maunder , A. Nielsen and J. Sibert . 2012. AD Model Builder: using automatic differentiation for statistical inference of highly parameterized complex nonlinear models. Optimization Methods and Software 27: 233–249. Google Scholar

14.

J. J. Hatch and D. V. Weseloh . 1999. Double-crested Cormorant (Phalacrocorax auritus). No. 441 in The Birds of North America Online ( A. Poole , Ed.). Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Ithaca, New York.  http://bna.birds.cornell.edu/bna/species/441, accessed 21 September 2010. Google Scholar

15.

S. P. Jensen , S.J. Gray and J. L. Hurst . 2005. Excluding neighbors from territories: effects of habitat structure and resource distribution. Animal Behaviour 69: 785–795. Google Scholar

16.

D. W. Mock 1976. Pair-formation displays of the Great Blue Heron. Wilson Bulletin 88: 185–230. Google Scholar

17.

C. L. Mott and T. J. Maret . 2011. Species-specific patterns of agonistic behavior among larvae of three syntopic species of ambystomatid salamanders. Copela 2011: 9–17. Google Scholar

18.

S. M. Pius and P. L. Leberg . 1997. Aggression and nest spacing in single and mixed species groups of seabirds. Oecologia 111: 144–150. Google Scholar

19.

R Development Core Team. 2012. R: a language and environment for statistical computing v. 2.15.1. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.  http://www.R-project.org/, accessed 25 June 2012. Google Scholar

20.

S. K. Skagen , C. P. Melcher and E. Muths . 2001. The interplay of habitat change, human disturbance and species interactions in a waterbird colony. American Midland Naturalist 145: 18–28. Google Scholar

21.

H. Skaug , D. Fournier , B. Bolker , A. Magnusson and A. Nielsen . 2014. Generalized linear mixed models using AD Model Builder. R package v. 0.7.4., R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.  http://glmmadmb.r-forge.r-project.org/, accessed 19 March 2013. Google Scholar

22.

C. M. Somers , M. N. Lozer and J. S. Quinn . 2007. Interactions between Double-crested Cormorants and Herring Gulls at a shared breeding site. Waterbirds 30: 241–250. Google Scholar

23.

C. M. Somers , J. L. Doucette , D. V. C. Weseloh , V. A. Kjoss and R. M. Brigham . 2011. Interactions between Double-crested Cormorants and other ground-nesting species. Waterbirds 34: 168–176. Google Scholar

24.

D. L. Stokes and P. D. Boersma . 2000. Nesting density and reproductive success in a colonial seabird, the Magellanic Penguin. Ecology 81: 2878–2891. Google Scholar

25.

U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service. 2014. Extension of expiration dates for Double-crested Cormorant depredation orders. Federal Register 79:30474–30483.  http://www.regulations.gov/#!documentDetail;D=FWS-HQ-MB-2013-0135-0041, accessed 4 January 2015. Google Scholar

26.

G. F. van Tets 1965. A comparative study of some social communication patterns in the Pelecaniformes. Ornithological Monographs 2: 1–88. Google Scholar

27.

D. V. Weseloh , C. Pekarik , T. Havelka , G. Barrett and J. Reid . 2002. Population trends and colony locations of Double-crested Cormorants in the Canadian Great Lakes and immediately adjacent areas, 1990– 2000: a manager's guide. Journal of Great Lakes Research 28: 125–144. Google Scholar

28.

D. V. Weseloh , P. J. Ewins , J. Struger , P. Minean , C. A. Bishop , S. Postupalsky and J. P. Ludwig . 1995. Double-crested Cormorants of the Great Lakes: changes in population size, breeding distribution and reproductive output between 1913 and 1991. Colonial Waterbirds 18: 48–59. Google Scholar

29.

L. R. Wires and F. J. Cuthbert . 2006. Historic populations of the Double-crested Cormorant (Phalacrocorax auritus): implications for conservation and management in the 21st century. Waterbirds 29: 9–37. Google Scholar

30.

L. R. Wires , K. V. Haws , F. J. Cuthbert , N. Drilling and A. C. Smith . 2006. The Double-crested Cormorant and American White Pelican in Minnesota: first statewide breeding census. Loon 78: 63–73. Google Scholar

31.

A. F. Zuur , E. N. leno , N. Walker , A. A. Saveliev and G. M. Smith . 2009. Mixed effects models and extensions in ecology with R. Springer, New York, New York. Google Scholar
Katherine E. Wyman and Francesca J. Cuthbert "Species Identity and Nest Location Predict Agonistic Interactions at a Breeding Colony of Double-Crested Cormorants (Phalacrocorax auritus) and Great Blue Herons (Ardea herodias)," Waterbirds 38(2), 201-207, (1 June 2015). https://doi.org/10.1675/063.038.0210
Received: 9 July 2014; Accepted: 4 January 2015; Published: 1 June 2015
KEYWORDS
agonistic behavior
Ardea herodias
breeding colony
competition
Double-crested Cormorant
Great Blue Heron
Phalacrocorax auritus
Back to Top